Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Neural basis of anticipation and premature impulsive action in the frontal cortex

Abstract

Planning motor actions can improve behavioral performance; however, it can also lead to premature actions. Although the anterior lateral motor cortex (ALM) is known to be important for correct motor planning, it is currently unknown how it contributes to premature impulsive motor output. This was addressed using whole-cell voltage recordings from layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons within the ALM while mice performed a cued sensory association task. Here, a robust voltage response was evoked during the auditory cue, which was greater during incorrect premature behavior than during correct performance in the task. Optogenetically suppressing ALM during the cued sensory association task led to enhanced behavior, with fewer, and more delayed, premature responses and faster correct responses. Taken together, our findings extend the current known roles of the ALM, illustrating that ALM plays an important role in impulsive behavior by encoding and influencing premature motor output.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Instructive cues shift the behavioral licking response in a cued sensory association task.
Fig. 2: ALM neurons have a robust voltage response during the auditory cue in expert mice.
Fig. 3: ALM neurons have an enhanced voltage response to the auditory cue during correct performance.
Fig. 4: M1 neurons have a small voltage response to the instructive auditory cue during the cued sensory association task.
Fig. 5: ALM neurons have a large voltage response during impulsive premature behavior.
Fig. 6: The cue-evoked voltage response during premature behavior is larger than during correct behavior.
Fig. 7: Suppression of ALM activity during the auditory cue reduces impulsive premature behavior.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

Data supporting the findings of this study are available within the paper and its Supplementary Information files. Source data are provided with this paper.

Code availability

Source code for acquisition and analysis of voltage recordings is available by request.

References

  1. Bruce, C. J. & Goldberg, M. E. Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. J. Neurophysiol. 53, 603–635 (1985).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Chen, T. W., Li, N., Daie, K. & Svoboda, K. A map of anticipatory activity in mouse motor cortex. Neuron 94, 866–879 e4 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Guo, Z. V. et al. Flow of cortical activity underlying a tactile decision in mice. Neuron 81, 179–194 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Inagaki, H. K., Fontolan, L., Romani, S. & Svoboda, K. Discrete attractor dynamics underlies persistent activity in the frontal cortex. Nature 566, 212–217 (2019).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Li, N., Guo, Z. V., Chen, T. W. & Svoboda, K. in Micro-, Meso- and Macro-Dynamics of the Brain (eds Buzsaki, G. & Christen, Y.) (Springer, Cham (CH), 2016).

  6. Gao, Z. et al. A cortico-cerebellar loop for motor planning. Nature 563, 113–116 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Gilad, A., Gallero-Salas, Y., Groos, D. & Helmchen, F. Behavioral strategy determines frontal or posterior location of short-term memory in neocortex. Neuron 99, 814–828.e7 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Maimon, G. & Assad, J. A. A cognitive signal for the proactive timing of action in macaque LIP. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 948–955 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Nobre, A. C. & van Ede, F. Anticipated moments: temporal structure in attention. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 34–48 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Svoboda, K. & Li, N. Neural mechanisms of movement planning: motor cortex and beyond. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 49, 33–41 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Coddington, L. T. & Dudman, J. T. The timing of action determines reward prediction signals in identified midbrain dopamine neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 1563–1573 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Dalley, J. W., Everitt, B. J. & Robbins, T. W. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron 69, 680–694 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Lee, K. et al. Temporally restricted dopaminergic control of reward-conditioned movements. Nat. Neurosci. 23, 209–216 (2020).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Otis, J. M. et al. Prefrontal cortex output circuits guide reward seeking through divergent cue encoding. Nature 543, 103–107 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Kim, S. & Lee, D. Prefrontal cortex and impulsive decision making. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 1140–1146 (2011).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Dalley, J. W. & Ersche, K. D. Neural circuitry and mechanisms of waiting impulsivity: relevance to addiction. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 374, 20180145 (2019).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Dalley, J. W. & Roiser, J. P. Dopamine, serotonin and impulsivity. Neuroscience 215, 42–58 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Catanese, J. & Jaeger, D. Premotor ramping of thalamic neuronal activity is modulated by nigral inputs and contributes to control the timing of action release. J. Neurosci. 41, 1878–1891 (2021).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Li, B., Nguyen, T. P., Ma, C. & Dan, Y. Inhibition of impulsive action by projection-defined prefrontal pyramidal neurons. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 117, 17278–17287 (2020).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Ebbesen, C. L. et al. More than just a ‘motor’: recent surprises from the frontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 38, 9402–9413 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Miri, A. et al. Behaviorally selective engagement of short-latency effector pathways by motor cortex. Neuron 95, 683–696 e11 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Galinanes, G. L., Bonardi, C. & Huber, D. Directional reaching for water as a cortex-dependent behavioral framework for mice. Cell Rep. 22, 2767–2783 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Li, N., Chen, T. W., Guo, Z. V., Gerfen, C. R. & Svoboda, K. A motor cortex circuit for motor planning and movement. Nature 519, 51–56 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Bradley, M. M., Miccoli, L., Escrig, M. A. & Lang, P. J. The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology 45, 602–607 (2008).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Reimer, J. et al. Pupil fluctuations track fast switching of cortical states during quiet wakefulness. Neuron 84, 355–362 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Bollu, T. et al. Cortex-dependent corrections as the tongue reaches for and misses targets. Nature 594, 82–87 (2021).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Banerjee, A. & Long, M. A. Ready, steady, go! Imaging cortical activity during movement planning and execution. Neuron 94, 698–700 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Fiser, A. et al. Experience-dependent spatial expectations in mouse visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 1658–1664 (2016).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Gavornik, J. P. & Bear, M. F. Learned spatiotemporal sequence recognition and prediction in primary visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 732–737 (2014).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Jaramillo, S. & Zador, A. M. The auditory cortex mediates the perceptual effects of acoustic temporal expectation. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 246–251 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Lin, P.-A., Asinof, S. K., Edwards, N. J. & Isaacson, J. S. Arousal regulates frequency tuning in primary auditory cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 116, 25304–25310 (2019).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. McGinley, M. J., David, S. V. & McCormick, D. A. Cortical membrane potential signature of optimal states for sensory signal detection. Neuron 87, 179–192 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Churchland, M. M., Yu, B. M., Ryu, S. I., Santhanam, G. & Shenoy, K. V. Neural variability in premotor cortex provides a signature of motor preparation. J. Neurosci. 26, 3697–3712 (2006).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Komiyama, T. et al. Learning-related fine-scale specificity imaged in motor cortex circuits of behaving mice. Nature 464, 1182–1186 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Funahashi, S., Bruce, C. J. & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey’s dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 61, 331–349 (1989).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Riehle, A. & Requin, J. Monkey primary motor and premotor cortex: single-cell activity related to prior information about direction and extent of an intended movement. J. Neurophysiol. 61, 534–549 (1989).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Schultz, W., Apicella, P. & Ljungberg, T. Responses of monkey dopamine neurons to reward and conditioned stimuli during successive steps of learning a delayed response task. J. Neurosci. 13, 900–913 (1993).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Mirenowicz, J. & Schultz, W. Preferential activation of midbrain dopamine neurons by appetitive rather than aversive stimuli. Nature 379, 449–451 (1996).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Mininni, C. J., Caiafa, C. F., Zanutto, B. S., Tseng, K. Y. & Lew, S. E. Putative dopamine neurons in the ventral tegmental area enhance information coding in the prefrontal cortex. Sci. Rep. 8, 11740 (2018).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Perez-Lopez, J. L., Contreras-Lopez, R., Ramirez-Jarquin, J. O. & Tecuapetla, F. Direct glutamatergic signaling from midbrain dopaminergic neurons onto pyramidal prefrontal cortex neurons. Front. Neural Circuits 12, 70 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Schultz, W., Dayan, P. & Montague, P. R. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275, 1593–1599 (1997).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Miller, E. K. The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1, 59–65 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Takahashi, N. et al. Thalamic input to motor cortex facilitates goal-directed action initiation. Curr. Biol. 31, 4148–4155 e4 (2021).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Stringer, C. et al. Spontaneous behaviors drive multidimensional, brainwide activity. Science 364, 255 (2019).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Musall, S., Kaufman, M. T., Juavinett, A. L., Gluf, S. & Churchland, A. K. Single-trial neural dynamics are dominated by richly varied movements. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 1677–1686 (2019).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Quiquempoix, M. et al. Layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons control the gain of cortical output. Cell Rep. 24, 2799–2807 e4 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Scott, D. J. et al. A novel ultra-stable, monomeric green fluorescent protein for direct volumetric imaging of whole organs using CLARITY. Sci. Rep. 8, 667 (2018).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Bergmann, R. et al. Coordination between eye movement and whisking in head-fixed mice navigating a plus maze. eNeuro https://doi.org/ENEURO.0089-22.2022 (2022).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank D. Scott for providing the AAV1/2-muGFP and V. Bahr, J. Kremkow and R. Sachdev for custom software for measuring online pupil diameter. This work was supported by the NHMRC (grant nos. APP1086082 and APP1063533, L.M.P.), the Sylvia and Charles Viertel Charitable Foundation (L.M.P.), the Australian Research Council (grant no. DP160103047, L.M.P.), IBRO (Return Home Fellowship and Early Career Award, R.G.) and an MJJ (Marius Jakulis Jason) fellowship (R.G.). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

R.G. and L.M.P. conceptualized and designed the study. R.G. performed and analyzed all experiments. L.G. performed control opto-inactivation experiments. R.G. and L.M.P. interpreted the results and wrote the paper.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Robertas Guzulaitis or Lucy Maree Palmer.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature Neuroscience thanks the anonymous reviewers for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data

Extended Data Fig. 1 Performance in tactile association task.

(a) The tactile-association task design. Mice were trained to lick a water port within 1 s of a tactile stimulation delivered to the forepaw (200 Hz, 100 ms). Correct responses (HIT) were rewarded with sucrose water (5–10 µl, 10% sucrose). Go-trials (tactile stimulus) were randomly interleaved with Catch-trials (no tactile stimulus). (b) Rate of HIT (green) and false alarm (FA; fawn) trials throughout learning of the tactile-asociation task (n = 32 mice). Data presented from individual mice (lines) and as mean (circles). (c) Discriminability index (d’) throughout learning (n = 32 mice). Data presented as individual mice (lines) and mean (circles).

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 2 Improvement in performance during the cued sensory association task following the introduction of an auditory cue.

(a) HIT (correct), (b) False-alarm (FA), (c) Discriminability index (d’), and (d) Early-lick (EL) rate in the first and last day after introducing the auditory cue during the cued-sensory association task (HIT, P < 0.0001; FA, P < 0.0001; d’, P < 0.0001; EL, P = 0.82; two tailed Wilcoxon tests, n = 32 mice). ns, P > 0.05; ****, P < 0.0001.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 3 Performance in the cued-sensory association task during cue omission.

(a) The cued sensory association task design. Mice were trained to lick a water port within 1 s of a tactile stimulation (200 Hz, 100 ms). Tactile stimulus was presented either in isolation (non-cued trials; TAC) or preceded by an auditory cue (cued trials; CueTAC; 1 s, 7 kHz, 60 dB). Trials were randomly interleaved. (b) Rate of HIT during CueTAC and TAC trials (P = 0.69, two tailed Wilcoxon test, n = 6 mice). Data presented as individual mice (lines) and mean of all mice (bars). (c) The average onset of the licking response during CueTAC and TAC trials (P = 0.03, two tailed Wilcoxon test, n = 6 mice). Data presented as individual mice (lines) and mean of all mice (bars). ns, P > 0.05; *, p < 0.05.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 4 Performance in the cued sensory association task during variable cue duration.

(a) The cued sensory association task design. Mice were trained to lick a water port within 1 s of a tactile stimulation (200 Hz, 100 ms). Tactile stimulus was preceded with an auditory cue (7 kHz, 60 dB) with variable duration (0.75 s, 1 s and 1.25 s). (b) Rate of EL performance during trials with different cue duration (1 s versus 0.75 s, P = 0.03; 1 s versus 1.25 s, P = 0.03; two tailed Wilcoxon test, n = 6 mice). (c) The average onset of the EL licking response during trials with different cue duration (1 s versus 0.75 s, P = 0.03; 1 s versus 1.25 s, P = 0.03; two tailed Wilcoxon test, n = 6 mice). (d) Rate of HIT performance during trials with different cue duration (1 s versus 0.75 s, P = 0.25; 1 s versus 1.25 s, P = 0.63; two tailed Wilcoxon test, n = 6 mice). (e) The average onset of the HIT licking response during trials with different cue duration (1 s versus 0.75 s, P = 0.99; 1 s versus 1.25 s, P = 0.84; two tailed Wilcoxon test, n = 6 mice). Data presented from individual mice (lines) and as mean (bars). ns, P > 0.05; *, P < 0.05.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 5 Performance in the cued sensory association task during omission of the tactile stimulus.

(a) The cued sensory association task design. Mice were initially trained to lick a water port in response to tactile stimulation (200 Hz, 100 ms) which was preceded by an auditory cue (1 s, 7 kHz, 60 dB; CueTAC). Then, in a subset of randomly interleaved trials, the tactile stimulus was omitted and only the auditory cue was presented to the mouse (Cue). Therefore, mice were presented with either the cue with (CueTAC) or without (Cue) the tactile stimulus. (b) HIT rate and (c) average onset of the licking response during CueTAC and Cue trials (HIT rate, P = 0.03; HIT Onset, P = 0.03; two tailed Wilcoxon test, n = 6 mice). Data presented as individual mice (lines) and mean of all mice (bars). *, P < 0.05.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 6 Voltage response during correct HIT performance in the cued sensory association task and its relation to timing of the licking response.

(a) Overlay of individual voltage responses during HIT trials (grey) and average of all HIT trials (black) in an example L2/3 pyramidal neuron. (b) Overlay of the average voltage responses during HIT trials of individual (grey) and average of all (black) L2/3 pyramidal neurons (n = 11 neurons, 4 mice) during HIT performance (light green) and its average (dark green). (c) Average voltage response (top) and licking response (bottom) during all HIT trials in the example L2/3 pyramidal neuron shown in (a). (d) Onset of the voltage occured before the first lick in HIT trials (P = 0.004, two tailed Wilcoxon test, n = 9 neurons, 4 mice). **, P < 0.01.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 7 Voltage responses in ALM neurons during the auditory cue.

(a) Overlay of the grand average (dark) and sem (light) voltage response for all recorded neurons during HIT (green) and MISS (grey) trials. Insert, zoom of voltage during the auditory cue. Auditory cue separated into 1st (0–500 ms) and 2nd (500–1000 ms) portion. (b) Amplitude of the voltage response in HIT trials during the 1st and 2nd portion of the auditory cue compared to baseline (baseline versus 1st portion of the cue, P = 0.0001; baseline versus 2nd portion of the cue, P = 0.0002; two tailed Wilcoxon test, n = 14 neurons, 4 mice). (c) Amplitude of the voltage response in MISS trials during the 1st and 2nd portion of the auditory cue compared to baseline (baseline versus 1st portion of the cue, P = 0.03; baseline versus 2nd portion of the cue, P = 0.44; two tailed Wilcoxon test, n = 6 neurons, 4 mice). ns, P > 0.05; *, P < 0.05; ***, P < 0.001.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 8 Nuchal EMG activity during HIT and early-lick trials.

(a) Example EMG activity during HIT (top) and early-lick (bottom; EL) trials. (b) Grand average (dark) and sem (light) EMG activity during HIT (green) and early-lick (red) trials from example in (a). (c) Average EMG activity during the beginning of the cue prior to licking responses (0–0.3 s) in HIT and early-lick trials (P = 0.47, two tailed Wilcoxon test, n = 7 sessions, 3 mice). ns, P > 0.05.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 9 Ramping baseline during early-lick trials.

(a) Example voltage responses recorded from a layer 2/3 pyramidal neuron within ALM during early-lick (EL) and correct (HIT) trials. Average baseline voltage at the start (0–1.5 s) and end (1.5–2 s) of the baseline epoch during (b) early-lick trials (P = 0.02, two tailed Wilcoxon test, n = 11 neurons, 4 mice) and (c) HIT trials (P = 0.32, two tailed Wilcoxon test, n = 11 neurons, 4 mice). ns, P > 0.05; *, P < 0.05.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 10 Firing rates of ALM pyramidal neurons during and after photo-activation of PV interneurons.

(a) Experimental design. PV+  Cre transgenic mice were previously injected with the light sensitive opsin ChR2 (pAAV-EF1a-doublefloxed-hChR2(H134R)-EYFP-WPRE-HGHpA) into the ALM (AP 2.5, ML 1.5 from Bregma). After 2 weeks expression, whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were performed in pyramidal neurons within the ALM during photo-activation of PV interneurons by blue LED pulses (470 nm, 40 Hz, 10 ms, 2–5 mW) delivered to the cortical surface. (b) Average (black) and sem (grey) firing rate during ChR2 activation for all recorded ALM neurons (n = 21 neurons, 5 mice). (c) Firing rate of ALM pyramidal neurons before, during and after photoactivation of PV interneurons (P = 0.29, two tailed Wilcoxon test, n = 21 neurons, 5 mice). ns, P > 0.05.

Source data

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Figs. 1–3.

Reporting Summary

Supplementary Data

Supplementary Data 1. Data of Supplementary Figs. 2 and 3.

Source data

Source Data Fig. 1

Statistical source data.

Source Data Fig. 2

Statistical source data.

Source Data Fig. 3

Statistical source data.

Source Data Fig. 4

Statistical source data.

Source Data Fig. 5

Statistical source data.

Source Data Fig. 6

Statistical source data.

Source Data Fig. 7

Statistical source data.

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 1

Statistical source data.

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 2

Statistical source data.

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 3

Statistical source data.

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 4

Statistical source data.

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 5

Statistical source data.

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 6

Statistical source data.

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 7

Statistical source data.

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 8

Statistical source data.

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 9

Statistical source data.

Source Data Extended Data Fig. 10

Statistical source data.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Guzulaitis, R., Godenzini, L. & Palmer, L.M. Neural basis of anticipation and premature impulsive action in the frontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 25, 1683–1692 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-022-01198-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-022-01198-z

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing