Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Causal role for the primate superior colliculus in the computation of evidence for perceptual decisions

Abstract

Trained monkeys performed a two-choice perceptual decision-making task in which they reported the perceived orientation of a dynamic Glass pattern, before and after unilateral, reversible, inactivation of a brainstem area—the superior colliculus (SC)—involved in preparing eye movements. We found that unilateral SC inactivation produced significant decision biases and changes in reaction times consistent with a causal role for the primate SC in perceptual decision-making. Fitting signal detection theory and sequential sampling models to the data showed that SC inactivation produced a decrease in the relative evidence for contralateral decisions, as if adding a constant offset to a time-varying evidence signal for the ipsilateral choice. The results provide causal evidence for an embodied cognition model of perceptual decision-making and provide compelling evidence that the SC of primates (a brainstem structure) plays a causal role in how evidence is computed for decisions—a process usually attributed to the forebrain.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Decision and selection tasks before and after unilateral SC inactivation.
Fig. 2: Unilateral inactivation of the SC biases perceptual decision-making.
Fig. 3: Decision, but not selection, accuracy is altered after SC inactivation.
Fig. 4: Comparison of aspects of SDT and DDM.
Fig. 5: Unilateral SC inactivation alters the drift rate offset.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The data reported in this manuscript are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Code availability

MATLAB, Python, R and JAGS analysis code is available at https://gitlab.com/fuster-lab-cognitive-neuroscience/sc-inactivation-project upon publication.

References

  1. Brody, C. D. & Hanks, T. D. Neural underpinnings of the evidence accumulator. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 37, 149–157 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Shadlen, M. N. & Kiani, R. Decision making as a window on cognition. Neuron 80, 791–806 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Roitman, J. D. & Shadlen, M. N. Response of neurons in the lateral intraparietal area during a combined visual discrimination reaction time task. J. Neurosci. 22, 9475–9489 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Kim, J. N. & Shadlen, M. N. Neural correlates of a decision in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the macaque. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 176–185 (1999).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Ding, L. & Gold, J. I. The basal ganglia’s contributions to perceptual decision making. Neuron 79, 640–649 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Ding, L. & Gold, J. I. Caudate encodes multiple computations for perceptual decisions. J. Neurosci. 30, 15747–15759 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Yartsev, M. M., Hanks, T. D., Yoon, A. M. & Brody, C. D. Causal contribution and dynamical encoding in the striatum during evidence accumulation. eLife 7, e34929 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Hanks, T. D., Ditterich, J. & Shadlen, M. N. Microstimulation of macaque area LIP affects decision-making in a motion discrimination task. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 682–689 (2006).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Ding, L. & Gold, JoshuaI. Separate, causal roles of the caudate in saccadic choice and execution in a perceptual decision task. Neuron 75, 865–874 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Horwitz, G. D., Batista, A. P. & Newsome, W. T. Representation of an abstract perceptual decision in macaque superior colliculus. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 2281–2296 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Horwitz, G. D. & Newsome, W. T. Separate signals for target selection and movement specification in the superior colliculus. Science 284, 1158–1161 (1999).

  12. Horwitz, G. D. & Newsome, W. T. Target selection for saccadic eye movements: prelude activity in the superior colliculus during a direction-discrimination task. J. Neurophysiol. 86, 2543–2558 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Felsen, G. & Mainen, Z. F. Neural substrates of sensory-guided locomotor decisions in the rat superior colliculus. Neuron 60, 137–148 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Glass, L. The Moire effect from random dots. Nature 223, 578–580 (1969).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Smith, M. A., Bair, W. & Movshon, J. A. Signals in macaque striate cortical neurons that support the perception of glass patterns. J. Neurosci. 22, 8334–8345 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Ratcliff, R., Cherian, A. & Segraves, M. A comparison of macaque behavior and superior colliculus neuronal activity to predictions from models of two-choice decisions. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 1392 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Cho, S.-H., Crapse, T., Grimaldi, P., Lau, H. & Basso, M. A. Variable statistical structure of neuronal spike trains in monkey superior colliculus. J. Neurosci. 41, 3234–3253 (2021)..

  18. Britten, K. H., Newsome, W. T., Shadlen, M. N., Celebrini, S. & Movshon, J. A. A relationship between behavioral choice and the visual responses of neurons in macaque MT. Vis. Neurosci. 13, 87 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Kim, B. & Basso, M. A. Saccade target selection in the superior colliculus: a signal detection theory approach. J. Neurosci. 28, 2991–3007 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Hikosaka, O. & Wurtz, R. H. Effects on eye movements of a GABA agonist and antagonist injected into monkey superior colliculus. Brain Res. 272, 368–372 (1983).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Lovejoy, L. P. & Krauzlis, R. J. Inactivation of primate superior colliculus impairs covert selection of signals for perceptual judgments. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 261–266 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. McPeek, R. M. & Keller, E. L. Deficits in saccade target selection after inactivation of superior colliculus. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 757–763 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Lee, C., Rohrer, W. H. & Sparks, D. L. Population coding of saccadic eye movements by neurons in the superior colliculus. Nature 332, 357–360 (1988).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Nummela, S. U. & Krauzlis, R. J. Inactivation of primate superior colliculus biases target choice for smooth pursuit, saccades, and button press responses. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 1538–1548 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Sedaghat-Nejad, E., Herzfeld, D. J. & Shadmehr, R. Reward prediction error modulates saccade vigor. J. Neurosci. 39, 5010–5017 (2019).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Gold, J. I. & Shadlen, M. N. Representation of a perceptual decision in developing oculomotor commands. Nature 404, 390–394 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Cisek, P. & Pastor-Bernier, A. On the challenges and mechanisms of embodied decisions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 369, 20130479 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Ratcliff, R. A theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Rev. 85, 59–108 (1978).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Ratcliff, R., Smith, P. L., Brown, S. D. & McKoon, G. Diffusion decision model: current issues and history. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 260–281 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Fan, Y., Gold, J. I. & Ding, L. Frontal eye field and caudate neurons make different contributions to reward-biased perceptual decisions. eLife 9, e60535 (2020).

  31. Perugini, A., Ditterich, J. & Basso, M. A. Patients with Parkinson’s disease show impaired use of priors in conditions of sensory uncertainty. Curr. Biol. 26, 1902–1910 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Ratcliff, R. & McKoon, G. The diffusion decision model: theory and data for two-choice decision tasks. Neural Comput. 20, 873–922 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Kloosterman, N. A. et al. Humans strategically shift decision bias by flexibly adjusting sensory evidence accumulation. eLife 8, e37321 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Crapse, T. B., Lau, H. & Basso, M. A. A role for the superior colliculus in decision criteria. Neuron 97, 181–194.e186 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Cisek, P., Puskas, G. A. & El-Murr, S. Decisions in changing conditions: the urgency-gating model. J. Neurosci. 29, 11560–11571 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Evans, N. J., Hawkins, G. E., Boehm, U., Wagenmakers, E. J. & Brown, S. D. The computations that support simple decision-making: a comparison between the diffusion and urgency-gating models. Sci. Rep. 7, 16433 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Ratcliff, R., Hasegawa, Y. T., Hasegawa, R. P., Smith, P. L. & Segraves, M. A. Dual diffusion model for single-cell recording data from the superior colliculus in a brightness-discrimination task. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 1756–1774 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Churchland, A. K. & Ditterich, J. New advances in understanding decisions among multiple alternatives. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 22, 920–926 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Churchland, A. K., Kiani, R. & Shadlen, M. N. Decision making with multiple alternatives. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 693 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Zhou, Y. & Freedman, D. J. Posterior parietal cortex plays a causal role in perceptual and categorical decisions. Science 365, 180–185 (2019).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Cisek, P. & Kalaska, J. F. Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 33, 269–298 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. de Lafuente, V., Jazayeri, M. & Shadlen, M. N. Representation of accumulating evidence for a decision in two parietal areas. J. Neurosci. 35, 4306–4318 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Gandhi, N. J. & Katnani, H. A. Motor functions of the superior colliculus. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 34, 205–231 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Basso, M. A. & May, P. J. Circuits for action and cognition: a view from the superior colliculus. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 3, 197–226 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Katz, L. N., Yates, J. L., Pillow, J. W. & Huk, A. C. Dissociated functional significance of decision-related activity in the primate dorsal stream. Nature 535, 285–288 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Licata, A. M. et al. Posterior parietal cortex guides visual decisions in rats. J. Neurosci. 37, 4954–4966 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Erlich, J. C., Brunton, B. W., Duan, C. A., Hanks, T. D. & Brody, C. D. Distinct effects of prefrontal and parietal cortex inactivations on an accumulation of evidence task in the rat. eLife 4, e05457 (2015).

  48. Hanks, T. D. et al. Distinct relationships of parietal and prefrontal cortices to evidence accumulation. Nature 520, 220–223 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Ferrera, V. P., Yanike, M. & Cassanello, C. Frontal eye field neurons signal changes in decision criteria. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 1458–1462 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Ditterich, J., Mazurek, M. & Shadlen, M. N. Microstimulation of visual cortex affects the speed of perceptual decisions. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 891 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Doi, T., Fan, Y., Gold, J. I. & Ding, L. The caudate nucleus contributes causally to decisions that balance reward and uncertain visual information. eLife 9, e56694 (2020).

  52. Judge, S., Richmond, B. & Chu, F. Implantation of magnetic search coils for measurement of eye position: an improved method. Vis. Res. 20, 535–538 (1980).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Crist, C. F., Yamasaki, D. S. G., Komatsu, H. & Wurtz, R. H. A grid system and a microsyringe for single cell recording. J. Neurosci. Methods 26, 117–122 (1988).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Hays, A. V., Richmond, B. J. & Optican, L. M. A UNIX-based multiple process system for real-time data acquisition and control. WESCON Conf. Proc. 2, 1–10 (1982).

    Google Scholar 

  55. Fuchs, A. F. & Robinson, D. A. A method for measuring horizontal and vertical eye movement chronically in the monkey. J. Appl. Physiol. 21, 1068–1070 (1966).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Ferster, C. B. & Skinner, B. F. Schedules of Reinforcement (Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1957).

  57. Ditterich, J. Evidence for time-variant decision making. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, 3628–3641 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Efron, B. & Tibshirani, R. J. An Introduction to the Bootstrap (Chapman and Hall/CRC, 1998).

  59. Macmillan, N. A. & Creelman, C. D. Detection Theory: A User’s Guide (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2004).

  60. O’Connell, R. G., Shadlen, M. N., Wong-Lin, K. & Kelly, S. P. Bridging neural and computational viewpoints on perceptual decision-making. Trends Neurosci. 41, 838–852 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  61. Voss, A., Rothermund, K. & Voss, J. Interpreting the parameters of the diffusion model: an empirical validation. Mem. Cognition 32, 1206–1220 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Plummer, M. JAGS: A program for analysis of Bayesian graphical models using Gibbs sampling. In Proc. 3rd International Workshop on Distributed Statistical Computing (eds Hornik, K. et al.) 10 (Vienna, Austria, 2003).

  63. Prins, N. & Kingdom, F. A. A. Applying the model-comparison approach to test specific research hypotheses in psychophysical research using the Palamedes toolbox. Front. Psychol. 9, 1250 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Wabersich, D. & Vandekerckhove, J. Extending JAGS: a tutorial on adding custom distributions to JAGS (with a diffusion model example). Behav. Res. Methods 46, 15–28 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Chandrasekaran, C. & Hawkins, G. E. ChaRTr: an R toolbox for modeling choices and response times in decision-making tasks. J. Neurosci. Methods 328, 108432 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Shen, Y. & Richards, V. M. A maximum-likelihood procedure for estimating psychometric functions: thresholds, slopes, and lapses of attention. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 132, 957–967 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Gold, J. I. & Ding, L. How mechanisms of perceptual decision-making affect the psychometric function. Prog. Neurobiol. 103, 98–114 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to J. Fuster for all of his support. We thank J. Ditterich for many helpful discussions and suggesting the 2D model with gain and A. Huk for comments on a previous version of the manuscript. We thank P. Grimaldi for help with the initial injection experiments, M. Lenoir, D. Tokuda, J. Garcia and K. Britchford for monkey care, A. Fabro for programming support and R. Krauzlis for monkey illustrations. This work was supported by EY013692 to M.A.B. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

M.A.B. conceived the study. E.J.J., A.R.B. and M.A.B. designed the study. E.J.J., A.R.B., J.H.T., E.A. and D.C.A. collected the data with guidance from M.A.B. E.J.J., A.R.B. and M.D.N. analyzed the data with guidance from M.A.B. E.J.J., A.R.B., M.D.N., D.C.A. and M.A.B. interpreted the results and wrote the paper.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michele A. Basso.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Peer review information Nature Neuroscience thanks Alexander Huk and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data

Extended Data Fig. 1 Estimates of muscimol spread in the SC.

(Associated with Fig. 1 main text) (a-j) The reduction in peak velocity from pre- to postmuscimol for 10 muscimol injections (four from monkey B, six from monkey S), one to two hours post injection. The percent change in peak velocity after muscimol injection (postmuscimol saccade velocity minus premuscimol saccade velocity divided by premuscimol saccade velocity multiplied by 100), is plotted for the target positions indicated by the white circles and linearly interpolated on the visual field in Cartesian coordinates. Cooler colors indicate slower saccadic velocities postmuscimol. (a-e) shows five injections with more concentrated effects of muscimol (color bar scaled from -60% to 60%), whereas (f-j) shows five injections with smaller but more diffuse effects of muscimol (color bar scaled from -30% to 30%), showing the range in the efficacy of our muscimol injections based on changes in saccade velocity at least one hour post injection. Red Xs show the site of injection based on the RF determined electrophysiologically (Supplementary Table 1). The peak velocity maps highlighted by the colored boxes in (a-j) had a uniform and homogenous sampling of positions in the visual field that allowed us to calculate the estimated spread across the SC map as shown in (k-n) (Quaia, C., Aizawa, H., Optican, L.M. & Wurtz, R.H. Reversible inactivation of monkey superior colliculus: II. Maps of saccadic deficits. Journal of Neurophysiology 79, 2097-2110 (1998).). (k-n) show the same percent change in peak velocity after muscimol injections plotted on the SC map (top) and the visual field in polar coordinates (bottom) for the injections in the corresponding colored boxes in (a-j). o, shows the locations of muscimol injections and spread estimates plotted onto the SC map. Red circles show injection locations for monkey B and red triangles show injections from monkey S. Each injection’s estimated muscimol spread is represented by two concentric circles. The darker shaded circles show 0.5 mm radius and the lighter shaded circles shows 1.5 mm radius from the center of the injection site based on estimates from (Allen, T.A., et al. Imaging the spread of reversible brain inactivations using fluorescent muscimol. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 171, 30-38 (2008).). There were three injections in which muscimol may have spread into the pretectal region and thus also the foveal region of the rostral SC, as evidenced by the occurrence of ocular nystagmus about an hour after the injection. In these cases, we aborted the experiment and omitted the data from analyses upon appearance of nystagmus. One example appears in b and k (maps highlighted by purple boxes). The Glass pattern decision and selection task data before the occurrence of nystagmus are included in the analysis in the main text. The effect on the psychometric function from this example was the largest that we observed (Fig. 2a, rightmost transparent orange psychometric function).

Extended Data Fig. 2 AIC and BIC scores for the two, three and four parameter logistic function fits.

(Associated with Fig. 2 of the main text) a, AIC scores for each pre- and post-injection and recovery sessions for all muscimol and saline injections (n = 87 sessions) for two, three, and four parameter logistic fits to the performance data. The circles show the AIC score of the logistic fit to each individual session from the n = 87 total sessions (pre-, post- and recovery * 29 injections), and the black, horizontal bars show the mean AIC score. The dotted lines connect the same data sessions that were fit across the two, three, and four parameter fits to see if there were any changes in AIC score between the fits with different number of parameters. The two-parameter logistic model has two parameters: α (decision bias) and β (sensitivity) following the equation p(IF) = 1/(1 + exp(-β (k-α))) (Eq 1 in Methods), which was used to fit the psychometric functions in Fig. 2 and Extended Data MEP_L_fig4Fig. 4. The three parameter logistic model includes: α, β, and ʎ (lapse rate or the difference between perfect performance and the top and bottom asymptotes) following the equation p(IF) = ʎ + (1-2ʎ)/(1 + exp(-β (k-α))). The four parameter logistic model includes: α, β and ʎ (lapse rate or the difference in perfect performance and asymptotic performance for toIF decisions) and γ (lapse rate or the difference in perfect performance and asymptotic performance for awayIF decisions) following the equation p(IF) = γ + (1-γ-λ)/(1 + exp(-β (k-α))). When looking at the AIC scores for the two, three, and four parameter fits (lower AIC scores indicate a better fit given model complexity), we see that the data are explained equally well or better with the models without lapse rates, with mean scores of 638.80 for the two-parameter fit, 640.51 for the three parameter fit, and 641.47 for the four parameter fit. Therefore, we selected the simpler, two-parameter model to fit the performance data. b, Same as in a for the BIC scores, with mean of 639.93 for the two-parameter fit, 642.20 for the three parameter fit, and 643.73 for the four parameter fit. The lack of difference in the quality of the fits with or without the lapse rate parameters is consistent with the parameter estimation results of lapse rates in the hierarchical DDM (Extended Data Fig. 6i,j).

Extended Data Fig. 3 Decision criterion but not sensitivity, is impacted by unilateral SC inactivation during one-interval, two-choice perceptual decision-making.

(Associated with Fig. 2 main text) a, Sensitivity, as measured by d’ is plotted against coherence for all experiments from both monkeys premuscimol (black circles and lines), postmuscimol (orange circles and lines) and 24 hour recovery (green circles and lines). Dashed lines show data from monkey B and solid lines show data from monkey S. Note that for monkey S, there is an additional 50% coherence condition (Methods). Qualitatively, monkey B showed a higher sensitivity for the same Glass pattern coherences than monkey S. b, d’ collapsed over coherence and plotted for premuscimol (gray circles), postmuscimol (orange circles) and recovery (green circles) for all experiments from both monkeys. Dashed lines show data from monkey B (n = 11 injections) and solid lines show data from monkey S (n = 12 injections). The horizontal lines indicate the mean d’ across sessions. On average there were no significant changes in d’ with muscimol in either monkey (monkey S, t(11) = -1.54, p = 0.152, 95% CI = [-0.23, 0.07]; monkey B, t(10) = -1.51, p = 0.161, 95% CI = [-0.21, 0.07]). (c-d) Same as in a and b for the saline injections. Because we only had two saline injections in monkey B, we collapsed the data across monkeys (n = 6 injections) for statistical analysis, but the data are shown separated by monkey. We found no significant differences in d’ with saline (t(5) = 1.20, p = 0.283, 95% CI = [-0.1, 0.19]). Note that there are no d’ or criterion (c) values for monkey B for the 24% and 36% coherences due to a lack of errors for the awayIF postmuscimol 24% and 36% coherence trials. e, Criterion (c) plotted against coherence for premuscimol (black), postmuscimol (orange), and recovery (green) for all experiments from both monkeys. Dashed lines are from monkey B and solid lines are from monkey S. This plot is shown for symmetry with the d’ plot although criterion changes across coherences are not particularly meaningful as monkeys are not expected to change their criterion across coherences as the coherences were randomized from trial to trial and there was no way for the monkeys to know which coherence was impending. f, Criterion collapsed over coherence plotted for premuscimol, postmuscimol and recovery for all experiments (n = 12 injections for monkey S, n = 11 injections for monkey B). For both monkeys, c changed significantly with muscimol (monkey S, t(11) = -9.34, p = 1.46 ×10−6, 95% CI = [-0.7, -0.38], monkey B, t(10) = -7.48, p = 2.10 ×10−5, 95% CI = [-0.75, -0.33]). (g-h) Same as in e-f for the saline experiments from both monkeys (n = 6 injections). We found no significant differences in c with saline injections (w(5) = 18, p = 0.156). Consistent with the psychometric function results shown in Fig. 2, unilateral inactivation of SC with muscimol produced changes in decision bias and not perceptual sensitivity.

Extended Data Fig. 4 Decision-making behavior 24 hours after muscimol.

(Associated with Fig. 2 of the main text) a, Proportion of choices to the inactivated field (toIF) is plotted as a function of Glass pattern coherence. Black circles show premuscimol performance data and green circles show 24-hour recovery performance data. The black and green lines show the two-parameter logistic fits to the performance data. n = 23 injections. b, Same as in a for the pre-saline (black circles and lines) and the 24-hour recovery from saline (green circles and lines). n = 6 injections. c, α parameters from the logistic fits for the recovery data (rec-muscimol) plotted against α parameters from the fits for the premuscimol data. On average, the α parameter shifted leftward during the recovery period compared to the premuscimol control (w(22) = 230, p = 0.005). Note that this was opposite to the direction of the shift that occurred postmuscimol as seen in the main Fig. 2a, as if the monkeys over-compensated for the effect of muscimol during recovery. d, β parameters from the logistic fits for the recovery data plotted against the β parameters from the fits from the premuscimol data. On average, there were no significant differences in the β parameter (t(22) = -1.31, p = 0.20, 95% CI = [-0.02, 6.0 ×10−3]). (e-f) Same as in c and d for the saline experiments. g, Reaction time (RT) plotted against coherence for the premuscimol data (black circles) and recovery data (green circles) from the RT version of the decision task (n = 9 injections). The lines show linear fits to the RT data. The RT was shorter for the recovery data compared to the premuscimol data for all coherences. Similar to the results of the α parameter comparisons, the RT finding suggests a compensatory response to the muscimol injections 24 hours earlier. h, Same as in g for the saline experiments. i, The slope parameter from the linear fits to the RT data for the recovery data plotted against the premuscimol data. Cyan circles show the parameter of the linear fits of the RT data for toIF decisions (positive coherences) and magenta circles show the RT data for awayIF decisions (negative coherences). There were no significant differences on average (RT slope awayIF, t(8) = 1.37, p = 0.21, 95% CI = [-0.87, 2.59]; RT slope toIF, t(8) = -0.87, p = 0.41, 95% CI = [-3.10, 1.61]). j, same as in i for the intercept parameter. There were significant changes in the intercept on average for the toIF side (RT intercept, t(8) = 3.61, p = 0.007, 95% CI = [32.55, 240.50]) but not the awayIF side (RT intercept, t(8) =2.63, p = 0.03 n.s. Bonferroni correction, 95% CI = [-4.89, 216.53]). (k-l) Same as in i and j for the saline experiments. There were no significant differences in slope or intercept for these experiments (RT slope awayIF, t(3) = -0.02, p = 0.98, 95% CI = [-6.26, 6.19]; RT slope toIF, t(3) = 0.62, p = 0.58, 95% CI = [-0.88, 1.19]; RT intercept awayIF, t(3) = 0.37, p = 0.74, 95% CI = [-71.20, 85.05]; RT intercept toIF, w(3) = 9, p = 0.25). Note that four saline experiments were performed in the RT task and the other two were performed using the delayed version of the task so only four observations appear in this plot. Note that the darker shaded symbols show the median values and the 95% confidence intervals are from the means.

Extended Data Fig. 5 DDM model simulations for changes in model parameters.

(Associated with Figs. 4 and 5 of the main text). Panels a-p are the same as those shown in Fig. 5 of the main text. a, RT distribution from the 0% coherence condition (density approximated through kernel smoothing) predicted by a DDM simulation with only decrease in proportionality factor between coherence and drift rate postmuscimol (orange). Pre-muscimol shown in black. Below the RT distributions, the relative evidence for toIF decisions is plotted over time since the Glass pattern onset and the short arrows show drift rates for toIF decisions (positive) and awayIF decisions (negative) pre- and postmuscimol, for the 0%, 10%, and 36% coherence conditions. The longer arrows show the mean drift rate across both toIF and awayIF directions and all coherences, termed drift rate offset28 b, The psychometric function, plotted as a proportion of toIF choices over coherences, predicted by the DDM variant simulation with a decrease in proportionality factor between coherence and drift rate which changes the slope (without a shift) of the psychometric function. A change in the slope of the psychometric function was not observed in the data (Fig. 5r, v, shaded), making the decrease in proportionality factor between coherence and drift rate an unlikely explanation for the observed data. c, Mean RT predictions for correct trials for each coherence condition for the DDM simulation with a decrease in proportionality factor between coherence and drift rate, for pre- (black) and postmuscimol (orange). d, Same as in c but for error trials. (e-h) Same as in a-d but for the DDM variant simulation with only a change in proportional start-point of the evidence accumulation path away from the IF (often interpreted as an initial bias away from the IF). A decrease in proportional start point away from the IF predicts a shift in the psychometric function as observed in the real data (Fig. 5r,v, shaded), making a change in the proportional start point a possibility in explaining the decision bias we observed in the postmuscimol data. However, a start point change away from the IF also predicts a decrease in error toIF RTs which we did not observe in the data (Fig. 5t,x, shaded). (i-l) Same as in a-d but for the DDM variant with an increase in the upper boundary but no absolute start point change (start point proportionally decreased away from the IF). This parameter change also predicts a lateral shift in the psychometric function away from IF decisions as we observed in the data (Fig. 5r,v, shaded). However, this parameter change cannot explain the magnitude of the psychometric function shift we observed (Fig. 5r,v, shaded) with similar changes in simulated and observed mean RTs (Fig. 5s,t,w,x, shaded). (m-p) Same as in a-d but for the DDM variant with a change in drift rate offset favoring awayIF decisions. The psychometric function predictions of the model simulation with a change in the drift rate offset predict a lateral shift in the psychometric function that is observed in the data (Fig. 5r,v, shaded). The increases in correct mean RT for toIF decisions are predicted and shown for both monkeys (Fig. 5s,w, shaded). Overall, a change in drift rate offset is most likely to explain the data we obtained after muscimol inactivation of the SC. (q-t) Same as in a-d but for the DDM model variant that describes RT distributions and performance with only an increase in the symmetric boundaries. This parameter change predicts only slight steepening of the slope of the psychometric function and no changes in the shift of the psychometric function as observed in the data (Fig. 5r,v, shaded), making the symmetric boundary change an unlikely possibility for explaining the effects of SC inactivation. (u-x) Same as in a-d but for the DDM variant that describes RT distributions and performance with only an increase in non-decision time. Non-decision time changes do not explain any changes in performance and thus cannot explain a shift in the psychometric function observed in the data from both monkeys (Fig. 5r,v, shaded), making a change in non-decision time unlikely to explain the effects of SC inactivation on decision-making.

Extended Data Fig. 6 Parameter estimates for HDDM, DDM, and UGM.

(Associated with Fig. 5 of the main text). (a-j) Estimates from the full HDDM of hierarchical parameters (μ) for each monkey (solid lines in the muscimol experimental condition; dotted lines for monkey S in the saline experimental condition, we did not collect data from the RT task for monkey B in the saline condition). 95% credible intervals with 2.5th and 97.5th quantile boundaries of hierarchical parameters provided by shading for the muscimol condition and smaller dot-dashed lines for the saline condition. Also shown are individual session parameter estimates for monkey S’s muscimol data (upward-pointing triangles), monkey B’s muscimol data (circles), and monkey S’s saline data (downward-pointing triangles). Estimates were obtained from the median posterior distributions of each parameter. a, Estimates of the HDDM session-level drift rate offset (Δ) and hierarchical drift rate offset (\(\mu _{\Delta}\)) for monkey S (pre BF = 0.08, post BF = 3.19 ×106, 99.7% probability of decrease pre to post). b, Same as in a but for monkey B (pre Bayes factor BF = 0.14, post BF = 17.87, 99.0% probability of decrease pre to post). c, Estimates of the HDDM session-level start point (w) and hierarchical start point (\(\mu _w\)) for monkey S (post BF-1 = 11.51, 95.1% probability of a proportional start point bias away from the IF from pre to post). d, Same as in c but for monkey B (post BF-1 = 2.82, 70.6% probability of a proportional start point bias towards the IF from pre to post). e, Estimates of the session-level non-decision time (τ) and hierarchical non-decision time (\(\mu _\tau\)) for monkey S (94.5% probability of an increase from pre to post). f, Same as in e for monkey B (97.0% probability of increase pre to post). g, Estimates of the session-level symmetric boundary (a) and hierarchical symmetric boundary (\(\mu _a\)) for monkey S (78.9% probability of an increase from pre to post). h, Same as in g but for monkey B (95.2% probability of increase pre to post). i, Estimates of the session-level lapse proportion (λ) and hierarchical lapse proportion (\(\mu _\lambda\)) for monkey S (72.5% probability of increase from pre to post). j, Same as in i but for monkey B (54.0% probability of increase pre to post). (k-l) The parameter estimates obtained from fitting the DDM and the UGM to the pre- and postmuscimol data for monkey S (panel k) and monkey B (panel l). The first row describes the model that was fit (DDM or UGM) and which data session (pre or post) was used to fit the model. The next 11 rows represent the drift rate parameter estimates (\(\delta _k\)) in evidence units/sec for the DDM or evidence units/ms for the UGM, for the k = 11 conditions (-24%, -17%, -10%, -3%, -5%, 0%, 5%, 3%, 10%, 17%, 24% coherences). The next row shows the drift rate offset (Δ). This parameter was not explicitly fit in the non-hierarchical DDM and UGM, but rather calculated as the mean of the all the drift rates across all coherences for toIF and awayIF directions that were estimated from fits. The drift rate offset decreased from pre- to postmuscimol for both DDM and UGM and for both monkeys (difference in monkey S, 0.53 evidence units/sec decrease for DDM, 2.19 evidence units/ms decrease for UGM; monkey B, 0.83 evidence units/sec decrease for DDM, 3.30 evidence units/ms decrease for UGM). The next row shows the proportional start point parameter w, defined as the proportion of the distance between the upper and lower bound. For monkey S, the start point parameter had slightly decreased from pre- to postmuscimol in both the DDM (0.06 decrease) and UGM (0.02 decrease), indicating the start point moved closer to the awayIF decision bound, and for monkey B, the start point parameter slightly increased in the DDM (0.04 increase) and UGM (0.02 increase), indicating the start point moved closer to the toIF decision bound. The next row shows the bound height parameter a, defined as the distance between the upper and lower bounds. For both monkeys, but more prominent in monkey B, the bound parameter had slightly increased from pre to post in the DDM (monkey S, pre to post increase of 0.03 decision units; monkey B, pre- to postmuscimol increase of 0.07 decision units), whereas the bound was fixed in the UGM (Supplementary Note). The row after shows the non-decision time 𝜏, in seconds, where we see a slight increase in the DDM (0.03 sec increase) and UGM (0.001 sec increase) for monkey S and a greater increase in the DDM for monkey B (0.11 sec increase), but not for the UGM (0.03 sec decrease). The last row shows the urgency slope estimates for the UGM, m, decreasing slightly with muscimol for monkey S (0.07 urgency units/ms), and decreasing more for monkey B (0.23 urgency units/ms).

Extended Data Fig. 7 Model predictions versus data for RT distributions and psychometric functions.

(Associated with Fig. 5 of the main text). Column a, shows the predicted RT distributions (0% coherence, density approximated through kernel smoothing) from the DDM, HDDM and UGM model variants (dashed lines) together with the actual data (solid lines), premuscimol (black) and postmuscimol (orange), for monkey S. We observed a rightward skew of the RT distribution, consistent with a fixed bound model of decision-making and captured by the DDM rather than the UGM as was also indicated by the R2pred, AIC, and BIC goodness of fit values (Supplementary Table 4). Column b, shows the same as in a but for psychometric functions (performance data, four parameter logistic model using equation shown in Extended Data Fig. 2). Column c, shows the same as in a for monkey B’s data and model fits. The RT distributions from monkey B were more normally distributed compared to the skewed RT distributions of monkey S, suggesting that the UGM rather than the DDM would explain monkey B’s data, consistent with the goodness of fit values (Supplementary Table 4). Column d, shows the same as in c but for psychometric function (performance data). Each row indicates the results of each model’s prediction compared to data for both monkey S and monkey B. The models from top to bottom are the full HDDM, HDDM with a free-to-vary drift rate offset (HDDM-Δ), HDDM with a free-to-vary proportional start point (HDDM-w), HDDM with free-to-vary non-decision time (HDDM-τ), HDDM with both a proportional start point and bound free to vary (HDDM-a,w), the non-hierarchical DDM, the full UGM, the UGM with free-to-vary drift rates (UGM-δ), and UGM with a free-to-vary urgency slope (UGM-m). Note that only the postmuscimol data are shown for the UGM with a single free parameter since we only fit the post data with those models where we let only one parameter free to vary while the rest of the parameters were fixed to premuscimol parameter estimates (Supplementary Note). Also for the DDM and UGM fits, note that there are only 11 conditions (-24 to 24 % coherence) for the psychometric functions because only 11 conditions were fitted (Supplementary Note). For the HDDM, out of all the variants (first five rows), the full HDDM predictions visually match the data for both performance and RT. The prediction of the HDDM-Δ captures the decision bias from the data almost equally well for both monkeys. The prediction for the HDDM-w and HDDM-a,w also predicts a decision bias, but is insufficient to explain the magnitude of the shift in decision bias that we observed in the data. The HDDM-τ fails to capture any decision bias (RT and performance predictions for pre and post are overlapping). The predictions of the simple DDM also capture the 0% RT distribution well, more so for monkey S than for monkey B, and also capture the choice data well. The opposite is true for the full UGM predictions, where the RT predictions capture monkey B’s data more than monkey S (see goodness of fit values in Supplementary Table 4), but also captures performance data well for both monkeys. The UGM-δ captures the shift in decision bias from the postmuscimol data from both monkeys, consistent with the findings from the HDDM, whereas the UGM-m fails to capture the decision bias in the post data.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Note, Supplementary Figs. 1–3 and Supplementary Tables 1–5.

Reporting Summary

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Jun, E.J., Bautista, A.R., Nunez, M.D. et al. Causal role for the primate superior colliculus in the computation of evidence for perceptual decisions. Nat Neurosci 24, 1121–1131 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-021-00878-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-021-00878-6

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing