Abstract
Intelligent behavior involves associations between high-dimensional sensory representations and behaviorally relevant qualities such as valence. Learning of associations involves plasticity of excitatory connectivity, but it remains poorly understood how information flow is reorganized in networks and how inhibition contributes to this process. We trained adult zebrafish in an appetitive odor discrimination task and analyzed odor representations in a specific compartment of the posterior zone of the dorsal telencephalon (Dp), the homolog of mammalian olfactory cortex. Associative conditioning enhanced responses with a preference for the positively conditioned odor. Moreover, conditioning systematically remapped odor representations along an axis in coding space that represented attractiveness (valence). Interindividual variations in this mapping predicted variations in behavioral odor preference. Photoinhibition of interneurons resulted in specific modifications of odor representations that mirrored effects of conditioning and reduced experience-dependent, interindividual variations in odor–valence mapping. These results reveal an individualized odor-to-valence map that is shaped by inhibition and reorganized during learning.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Relevant articles
Open Access articles citing this article.
-
Odor hedonics coding in the vertebrate olfactory bulb
Cell and Tissue Research Open Access 30 January 2021
Access options
Access Nature and 54 other Nature Portfolio journals
Get Nature+, our best-value online-access subscription
$29.99 / 30 days
cancel any time
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 12 print issues and online access
$209.00 per year
only $17.42 per issue
Rent or buy this article
Prices vary by article type
from$1.95
to$39.95
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout








Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding authors upon reasonable request.
Code availability
All codes used in this study are available from the corresponding authors upon reasonable request.
References
Ganguli, S. & Sompolinsky, H. Compressed sensing, sparsity, and dimensionality in neuronal information processing and data analysis. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 485–508 (2012).
Poo, M. et al. What is memory? The present state of the engram. BMC Biol. 14, 40 (2016).
Haberly, L. B. Parallel-distributed processing in olfactory cortex: new insights from morphological and physiological analysis of neuronal circuitry. Chem. Senses 26, 551–576 (2001).
Bolding, K. A. & Franks, K. M. Complementary codes for odor identity and intensity in olfactory cortex. eLife 6, e22630 (2017).
Iurilli, G. & Datta, S. R. Population coding in an innately relevant olfactory area. Neuron 93, 1180–1197.e7 (2017).
Miyamichi, K. et al. Cortical representations of olfactory input by trans-synaptic tracing. Nature 472, 191–196 (2011).
Roland, B., Deneux, T., Franks, K. M., Bathellier, B. & Fleischmann, A. Odor identity coding by distributed ensembles of neurons in the mouse olfactory cortex. eLife 6, e26337 (2017).
Stettler, D. D. & Axel, R. Representations of odor in the piriform cortex. Neuron 63, 854–864 (2009).
Banerjee, A. et al. An interglomerular circuit gates glomerular output and implements gain control in the mouse olfactory bulb. Neuron 87, 193–207 (2015).
Gschwend, O. et al. Neuronal pattern separation in the olfactory bulb improves odor discrimination learning. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1474–1482 (2015).
Kato, H. K., Gillet, S. N., Peters, A. J., Isaacson, J. S. & Komiyama, T. Parvalbumin-expressing interneurons linearly control olfactory bulb output. Neuron 80, 1218–1231 (2013).
Niessing, J. & Friedrich, R. W. Olfactory pattern classification by discrete neuronal network states. Nature 465, 47–52 (2010).
Zhu, P., Frank, T. & Friedrich, R. W. Equalization of odor representations by a network of electrically coupled inhibitory interneurons. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1678–1686 (2013).
Wilson, D. A. & Sullivan, R. M. Cortical processing of odor objects. Neuron 72, 506–519 (2011).
Chapuis, J. & Wilson, D. A. Bidirectional plasticity of cortical pattern recognition and behavioral sensory acuity. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 155–161 (2012).
Roesch, M. R., Stalnaker, T. A. & Schoenbaum, G. Associative encoding in anterior piriform cortex versus orbitofrontal cortex during odor discrimination and reversal learning. Cereb. Cortex 17, 643–652 (2007).
Shakhawat, A. M. et al. Visualizing the engram: learning stabilizes odor representations in the olfactory network. J. Neurosci. 34, 15394–15401 (2014).
Choe, H. K. et al. Oxytocin mediates entrainment of sensory stimuli to social cues of opposing valence. Neuron 87, 152–163 (2015).
Choi, G. B. et al. Driving opposing behaviors with ensembles of piriform neurons. Cell 146, 1004–1015 (2011).
Meissner-Bernard, C., Dembitskaya, Y., Venance, L. & Fleischmann, A. Encoding of odor fear memories in the mouse olfactory cortex. Curr. Biol. 29, 367–380.e4 (2019).
Sacco, T. & Sacchetti, B. Role of secondary sensory cortices in emotional memory storage and retrieval in rats. Science 329, 649–656 (2010).
Hennequin, G., Agnes, E. J. & Vogels, T. P. Inhibitory plasticity: balance, control, and codependence. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 40, 557–579 (2017).
Poo, C. & Isaacson, J. S. Odor representations in olfactory cortex: ‘Sparse’ coding, global inhibition, and oscillations. Neuron 62, 850–861 (2009).
Mueller, T., Dong, Z., Berberoglu, M. A. & Guo, S. The dorsal pallium in zebrafish, Danio rerio (Cyprinidae, Teleostei). Brain Res. 1381, 95–105 (2011).
Miyasaka, N. et al. Olfactory projectome in the zebrafish forebrain revealed by genetic single-neuron labelling. Nat. Commun. 5, 3639 (2014).
Blumhagen, F. et al. Neuronal filtering of multiplexed odour representations. Nature 479, 493–498 (2011).
Yaksi, E., von Saint Paul, F., Niessing, J., Bundschuh, S. T. & Friedrich, R. W. Transformation of odor representations in target areas of the olfactory bulb. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 474–482 (2009).
Jacobson, G. A., Rupprecht, P. & Friedrich, R. W. Experience-dependent plasticity of odor representations in the telencephalon of zebrafish. Curr. Biol. 28, 1–14.e3 (2018).
Rupprecht, P. & Friedrich, R. W. Precise synaptic balance in the zebrafish homolog of olfactory cortex. Neuron 100, 669–683.e5 (2018).
Friedrich, R. W. & Laurent, G. Dynamic optimization of odor representations by slow temporal patterning of mitral cell activity. Science 291, 889–894 (2001).
Miklavc, P. & Valentinčič, T. Chemotopy of amino acids on the olfactory bulb predicts olfactory discrimination capabilities of zebrafish Danio rerio. Chem. Senses 37, 65–75 (2012).
Vitebsky, A., Reyes, R., Sanderson, M. J., Michel, W. C. & Whitlock, K. E. Isolation and characterization of the laure olfactory behavioral mutant in the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Dev. Dyn. 234, 229–242 (2005).
Namekawa, I., Moenig, N. R. & Friedrich, R. W. Rapid olfactory discrimination learning in adult zebrafish. Exp. Brain Res. 236, 2959–2969 (2018).
Sturgill, J. F. & Isaacson, J. S. Somatostatin cells regulate sensory response fidelity via subtractive inhibition in olfactory cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 531–535 (2015).
Aso, Y. et al. Mushroom body output neurons encode valence and guide memory-based action selection in Drosophila. eLife 3, e04580 (2014).
Gradinaru, V. et al. Molecular and cellular approaches for diversifying and extending optogenetics. Cell 141, 154–165 (2010).
Satou, C. et al. Transgenic tools to characterize neuronal properties of discrete populations of zebrafish neurons. Development 140, 3927–3931 (2013).
Mueller, T. & Guo, S. The distribution of GAD67‐mRNA in the adult zebrafish (teleost) forebrain reveals a prosomeric pattern and suggests previously unidentified homologies to tetrapods. J. Comp. Neurol. 516, 553–568 (2009).
Miura, K., Mainen, Z. F. & Uchida, N. Odor representations in olfactory cortex: distributed rate coding and decorrelated population activity. Neuron 74, 1087–1098 (2012).
Kadohisa, M. & Wilson, D. A. Separate encoding of identity and similarity of complex familiar odors in piriform cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 103, 15206–15211 (2006).
Calu, D. J., Roesch, M. R., Stalnaker, T. A. & Schoenbaum, G. Associative encoding in posterior piriform cortex during odor discrimination and reversal learning. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1342–1349 (2007).
Howard, J. D., Plailly, J., Grueschow, M., Haynes, J.-D. & Gottfried, J. A. Odor quality coding and categorization in human posterior piriform cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 932–938 (2009).
Blake, D. T., Strata, F., Churchland, A. K. & Merzenich, M. M. Neural correlates of instrumental learning in primary auditory cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 99, 10114–10119 (2002).
Poort, J. et al. Learning enhances sensory and multiple non-sensory representations in primary visual cortex. Neuron 86, 1478–1490 (2015).
Rutkowski, R. G. & Weinberger, N. M. Encoding of learned importance of sound by magnitude of representational area in primary auditory cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102, 13664–13669 (2005).
Yan, Y. et al. Perceptual training continuously refines neuronal population codes in primary visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 1380–1387 (2014).
Secundo, L., Snitz, K. & Sobel, N. The perceptual logic of smell. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 25, 107–115 (2014).
Hige, T., Aso, Y., Rubin, G. M. & Turner, G. C. Plasticity-driven individualization of olfactory coding in mushroom body output neurons. Nature 526, 258–262 (2015).
Séjourné, J. et al. Mushroom body efferent neurons responsible for aversive olfactory memory retrieval in Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 903–910 (2011).
Khan, A. G. et al. Distinct learning-induced changes in stimulus selectivity and interactions of GABAergic interneuron classes in visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 851–859 (2018).
Cocco, A. et al. Characterization of the γ-aminobutyric acid signaling system in the zebrafish (Danio rerio Hamilton) central nervous system by reverse transcription-quantitative polymerase chain reaction. Neuroscience 343, 300–321 (2017).
Kwan, K. M. et al. The Tol2kit: A multisite gateway-based construction kit forTol2 transposon transgenesis constructs. Dev. Dyn. 236, 3088–3099 (2007).
Distel, M., Wullimann, M. F. & Köster, R. W. Optimized Gal4 genetics for permanent gene expression mapping in zebrafish. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 13365–13370 (2009).
Kawakami, K. et al. A transposon-mediated gene trap approach identifies developmentally regulated genes in zebrafish. Dev. Cell 7, 133–144 (2004).
Suster, M. L., Sumiyama, K. & Kawakami, K. Transposon-mediated BAC transgenesis in zebrafish and mice. BMC Genom. 10, 477 (2009).
Asakawa, K. et al. Genetic dissection of neural circuits by Tol2 transposon-mediated Gal4 gene and enhancer trapping in zebrafish. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 105, 1255–1260 (2008).
Mathieson, W. B. & Maler, L. Morphological and electrophysiological properties of a novel in vitro preparation: the electrosensory lateral line lobe brain slice. J. Comp. Physiol. A 163, 489–506 (1988).
Wullimann, M. Neuroanatomy of the Zebrafish Brain: A Topological Atlas (Birkhäuser Verlag, 1996).
Zhu, P., Fajardo, O., Shum, J., Schärer, Y.-P. Z. & Friedrich, R. W. High-resolution optical control of spatiotemporal neuronal activity patterns in zebrafish using a digital micromirror device. Nat. Protoc. 7, 1410–1425 (2012).
Pologruto, T. A., Sabatini, B. L. & Svoboda, K. ScanImage: flexible software for operating laser scanning microscopes. Biomed. Eng. Online 2, 13 (2003).
Suter, B. A. et al. Ephus: multipurpose data acquisition software for neuroscience experiments. Front. Neural Circuits 4, 100 (2010).
Valentinčič, T., Metelko, J., Ota, D., Pirc, V. & Blejec, A. Olfactory discrimination of amino acids in brown bullhead catfish. Chem. Senses 25, 21–29 (2000).
Duguid, I., Branco, T., London, M., Chadderton, P. & Häusser, M. Tonic inhibition enhances fidelity of sensory information transmission in the cerebellar cortex. J. Neurosci. 32, 11132–11143 (2012).
Vinje, W. E. & Gallant, J. L. Sparse coding and decorrelation in primary visual cortex during natural vision. Science 287, 1273–1276 (2000).
Bathellier, B., Buhl, D. L., Accolla, R. & Carleton, A. Dynamic ensemble odor coding in the mammalian olfactory bulb: sensory information at different timescales. Neuron 57, 586–598 (2008).
Acknowledgements
We thank A. Wanner for custom-made scripts for microscope control, E. Arn Bouldoires for excellent technical assistance and N. Temiz for help during behavioral training. We are grateful to K. Deisseroth (Stanford University, CA, USA) for DNA constructs containing eNpHR3.0YFP and to R. Köster (Technical University Braunschweig, Germany) for DNA constructs containing the 5xUAS cassette. We thank G. Keller, A. Lüthi, C. Meissner-Bernard and P. Rupprecht for critical comments on the manuscript and members of the Friedrich group for helpful discussions. This work was supported by the Novartis Research Foundation, the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant nos. 31003A_135196 and 310030B_1528331), fellowships from HFSPO (no. LT000278/2012-L) and EMBO (no. ALTF 994-2010) to T.F., and the European Research Council under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation (grant no. 742576).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
T.F. conceived the project, designed experiments, generated Tg fish lines, performed all experiments except for behavioral conditioning, analyzed data, interpreted data and wrote the manuscript. N.R.M. performed and analyzed behavioral conditioning experiments and commented on the manuscript. C.S. performed and analyzed behavioral conditioning experiments and commented on the manuscript. S.H. generated Tg fish lines and commented on the manuscript. R.W.F. conceived the project, designed experiments, interpreted data and wrote the manuscript.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Peer review information Nature Neuroscience thanks David Schoppik and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Integrated supplementary information
Supplementary Figure 1 Odor identity classification in dpDp (related to Fig. 1).
a. Classification of odor identity by template matching of activity vectors in naïve (NAV) fish using Pearson correlation distance (1 – Pearson correlation; cf. Fig. 1g). Fills show percentage of correctly decoded odors using a fixed population size of 95 neurons. White dashed line indicates chance level (25 %; McNemar test, n = 156 trials, d.f. = 1, Χ2 = 75.01, P < 10–16); black dashed line shows mean classification success, averaged over all four odors (McNemar test for comparison against 100% correct, Χ2 = 38.03, P = 7 x 10–10). Comparison against classification success using cosine distance (Fig. 1g; McNemar test): Χ2 = 0, P = 1. b. Same odor classification procedure using Euclidean distance as distance metric. Comparison against chance level (25 %; McNemar test): Χ2 = 69.01, P = 1 x 10–16. Comparison against 100% correct (McNemar test): Χ2 = 44.02, P = 3 x 10–11. Comparison against classification success using cosine distance (Fig. 1g; McNemar test): Χ2 = 4.17, P = 0.04. n and d.f. as in (a). ***P < 0.001.
Supplementary Figure 2 Innate odor preference and associative olfactory conditioning (related to Fig. 2).
a. Behavioral setup to analyze innate behavioral responses to odors. Tank water (control) or odor solution (His, Ser, Ala or Trp) were delivered to one side of the tank using a gravity-fed system. b. Experimental paradigm. Following acclimatization, tank water was applied to test for non-specific responses, followed 10 min later by odor application at the same location. Swimming speed was quantified before applications and for 40 s after each application. c. Left: Behavioral discrimination score (CS+ preference score, calculated as ζCS+ – ζCS– over the last nine trials) did not differ significantly between ALA, TRP, and HIS training groups (one-way ANOVA, d.f. = 42, F = 0.72, P = 0.49, N (ALA) = 12 animals, N (TRP) = 16, N (HIS) = 15). Open circles represent individual fish. Multiple comparisons between all groups (Tukey test, two-sided): ALA vs TRP, q = 0.21, P = 0.98; ALA vs HIS, q = 1.27, P = 0.65; TRP vs HIS, q = 1.59, P = 0.50. Right: behavioral preference for Ala vs Trp or His (Trp for ALA, TRP, UNC; His for HIS; ζAla – ζTrp or His; 0: no preference; > 0: preference for Ala; < 0: preference for Trp or His). Ala preference score (calculated over last nine trials) differed between training groups (one-way ANOVA, d.f. = 54, F = 31.83, P = 9 x 10–12, N as before and N (UNC) = 12). Multiple comparisons between all groups (Tukey test, two-sided): ALA vs TRP, q = 12.66, P = 5 x 10–11; ALA vs HIS, q = 10.97, P = 2 x 10–9; ALA vs UNC, q =5.64, P = 0.001; TRP vs HIS, q = 1.63, P = 0.66; TRP vs UNC, q = 6.64, P = 0.0001; HIS vs UNC, q = 5.04, P = 0.004. Box plot: center line, median; box limits, interquartile range; and whiskers, s.d. d. Examples of behavioral responses during appetitive conditioning. Single trial examples of swimming trajectories during the 30 s after odor onset, but prior to food delivery (ALA fish). Trials of the same fish were chosen from the first (left panel) and last (right panel) training day. Top: trajectory plots of one CS+ trial (Ala, red) and one CS− trial (Trp, blue). Brightness encodes z-level in the water column. Center and bottom: histograms of fish position in each video frame extracted from the trajectories above. e. Same plots as in (d) for a TRP fish. f. Mean learning curves for individual components of appetitive behavior (cf. Fig. 2d and ref.33). Lines and shading show the mean (± s.e.m.) of ζ for the first three days of training (nine trials per day). Comparisons between CS+ and CS– (Wilcoxon signed rank test, two-sided, N = 43 animals, ALA, TRP, and HIS). z-level: day 1, T = 293, P = 0.03; day 2, T = 169, P = 0.0001; day 3, T = 111, P = 3 x 10–6. Speed: day 1, T = 291, P = 0.03; day 2, T = 85, P = 3 x 10–7; day 3, T = 121, P =6 x 10–6. Distance: day 1, T = 268, P = 0.01; day 2, T = 208, P = 0.001; day 3, T = 232, P = 0.003. Surface (peaks): day 1, T = 454, P = 0.82; day 2, T = 243, P = 0.005; day 3, T = 56, P = 1 x 10–8. Area: day 1, T = 271, P = 0.01; day 2, T = 297, P = 0.03; day 3, T = 199, P = 0.0007. Circling: day 1, T = 423, P = 0.55; day 2, T = 464, P = 0.92; day 3, T = 404, P = 0.41. ns: P ≥ 0.05; *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001.
Supplementary Figure 3 Experience modifies odor responses in dpDp (related to Figs. 3, 4).
a. Odor-specific response amplitudes expressed as percentage of mean amplitude in NAV fish. Lines and shading show the mean (± s.e.m.). Kruskal-Wallis tests followed by non-parametric multiple comparisons against NAV fish, two-sided: Ala-evoked responses (n = 5654, d.f. = 3, H = 92.04, P < 10–15): ALA, Q = −8.18, P < 10–15, n = 1591; TRP, Q = −2.03, P = 0.09, n = 1839; UNC, Q = 0.005, P = 1, n = 962. Trp-evoked responses (H = 140.48, P < 10–15): ALA, Q = −5.94, P = 6 x 10–9; TRP, Q = −4.77, P = 4 x 10–6; UNC, Q = 5.00, P = 1 x 10–6. His-evoked responses (H = 164.41, P < 10–15): ALA, Q = −11.53, P < 10–15; TRP, Q = −7.38, P = 3 x 10–13; UNC, Q = −1.35, P = 0.36. Ser-evoked responses (H = 142.20, P < 10–15): ALA, Q = −4.11, P = 8 x 10–5; TRP, Q = −6.83, P = 2 x 10–11; UNC, Q = 4.57, P = 1 x 10–5. Number of neurons (n) and d.f. as in Fig. 3a. Pairwise comparisons of Ala- vs Trp-evoked responses (paired t test, two-sided, d.f. = n−1): ALA, t = 4.37, P = 1 x 10–5; TRP, t = −2.81, P = 0.005; UNC, t = 5.78, P = 1 x 10–8. b. Signal-to-background ratio (SBR). Single neuron example illustrating background (grey) and signal (red) analysis windows. c. The SBR ratio of responses to Ala and Trp or His differed significantly between experimental groups (Ala/Trp: Kruskal-Wallis test with NAV, ALA, TRP, UNC: n = 2556, d.f. = 3, H = 21.66, P = 8 x 10–5; Ala/His: Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney test, two-sided: NAV, n = 174, HIS, n = 357, U = 34’751, P = 0.03). Non-parametric multiple comparisons against NAV fish (n = 536), two-sided, for Ala vs Trp: ALA, Q = 0.67, P = 0.83, n = 809; TRP, Q = 3.50, P = 0.0009, n = 780; UNC, Q = −1.34, P = 0.36, n = 431. Analysis included only neurons that responded to both Ala and the second CS (Trp or His). Box plot: center line, median; box limits, interquartile range; and whiskers, s.d. d. Response amplitudes in NAV fish differed between crossings (Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney test, two-sided: NAV #1, n = 1262 neurons (N = 9 animals); NAV #2, n = 528 neurons (N = 4 animals): U = 303’041, P = 0.003). Box plot: as in (c). e. Relative response amplitudes to familiar odors (Ala vs Trp) were not correlated to behavioral odor preference in individual UNC fish. Pearson correlation: r = −0.21 (N = 12 animals, t test of null hypothesis of r = 0, two-sided, d.f. = 10, t = −0.71, P = 0.49). Kendall‘s rank correlation: τ = 0.00 (test of null hypothesis of τ = 0 using a normal approximation, two-sided, z = 0.0, P = 1). f. Correlation of spontaneous activity traces was increased after associative conditioning and odor exposure both in weakly responding neurons (left; mean odor-evoked response amplitude: ∆F/F ≤ 10%; Kruskal-Wallis test, n = 7655, d.f. = 4, H = 331.76, P < 10–15) and in neurons with stronger odor responses (right; mean amplitude: 10 % < ∆F/F < 40%; Kruskal-Wallis test, n = 1302, d.f. = 4, H = 64.25, P = 2 x 10–13). Non-parametric multiple comparisons in weakly responding neurons, n (NAV) = 1573, two-sided: ALA, Q = –14.35, P < 10–15, n = 1240; TRP, Q = –11.04, P < 10–15, n = 1780; HIS, Q = –4.36, P = 3 x 10–5, n = 1807; UNC, Q = –13.86, P < 10–15, n = 1255. Non-parametric multiple comparisons in neurons with stronger responses, n (NAV) = 207, two-sided: ALA, Q = –6.04, P = 3 x 10–9, n = 341; TRP, Q = –4.38, P = 2 x 10–5, n = 374; HIS, Q = –2.97, P = 0.006, n = 230; UNC, Q = –7.31, P = 5 x 10–13, n = 150. Box plot: as in (c). g. Correlation of odor tuning curves (signal correlation) was increased after associative conditioning and odor exposure, both in weakly responding neurons (left; Kruskal-Wallis test, n = 4871, d.f. = 4, H = 264.53, P < 10–15) and neurons with stronger odor responses (right; Kruskal-Wallis test, n = 1297, d.f. = 4, H = 107.65, P < 10–15). Non-parametric multiple comparisons in weakly responding neurons, n (NAV) = 1105, two-sided: ALA, Q = –13.50, P < 10–15, n = 976; TRP, Q = –13.99, P < 10–15, n = 1221; HIS, Q = –10.31, P < 10–15, n = 762; UNC, Q = –6.76, P = 2 x 10–11, n = 807. Non-parametric multiple comparisons in neurons with stronger responses, n (NAV) = 207, two-sided: ALA, Q = –5.20, P = 4 x 10–7, n = 339; TRP, Q = –9.16, P = 2 x 10–10, n = 374; HIS, Q = –6.44, P < 10–15, n = 227; UNC, Q = –8.46, P < 10–15, n = 150. Only neurons that responded to at least one odor were included in this analysis. Box plot: as in (c). ns: P ≥ 0.05; *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001.
Supplementary Figure 4 Mapping odor space onto valence axis (related to Fig. 5).
a. Analysis of coding structures with dimensions representing cosine distance between task-relevant categories. (NAV, ALA, and UNC fish: CS+ was Ala, CS− Trp, Neutral (Neu)1 was Ser, and Neu2 His; TRP: CS+ was Trp, CS− Ala, Neu1 was His, and Neu2 Ser; HIS: CS+ was His, CS− Ala, Neu1 was Trp, and Neu2 Ser; modifying the grouping of odors into these categories, for example by swapping the identities of the two neutral odors, had little effect on the analysis results). Throughout, number of animals and d.f. as in Fig. 4d. Far left: projection of coding structures onto the first two principal components. Each plot symbol represents one fish. Colors show association of each coding structure with the respective experimental group. For ALA, TRP, and HIS fish, larger marker size indicates higher behavioral discrimination score. Quantification for PC 1 in center panel. Left: PC 1 loadings on the six coding structure dimensions: PC 1 neither is dominated by a single task-relevant category nor reflecting the global distance between all category pairs. Center: Projection onto PC 1 was modulated by experience (Kruskal-Wallis test, H = 9.82, P = 0.04). Non-parametric multiple comparisons against NAV fish: ALA, Q = 0.26, P = 1; TRP, Q = 2.43, P = 0.03; HIS, Q = 2.29, P = 0.04; UNC, Q = 1.31, P = 0.38. Right: PC 1 score was correlated to behavioral odor preference in individual UNC fish. Pearson correlation: r = 0.72 (N = 12 animals, t test of null hypothesis of r = 0, two-sided, d.f. = 10, t = 3.26, P = 0.009). Kendall‘s rank correlation: τ = 0.52 (test of null hypothesis of τ = 0 using a normal approximation, two-sided, z = 2.33, P = 0.02). Far right: The correlation between PC 1 score and behavioral odor preference remained significant across all training groups. Pearson correlation: r = 0.52 (N = 55 animals, d.f. = 53, t = 4.44, P = 4 x 10–5). Kendall‘s rank correlation: τ = 0.35 (z = 3.78, P = 0.0002). Box plot: center line, median; box limits, interquartile range; and whiskers, s.d. b. Analysis of coding structures with dimensions representing standardized cosine distance between odor pairs (equivalent to Pearson correlation) of activity patterns (population vectors). Throughout, conventions, number of animals (N), statistical tests, and d.f. as in (a). Projection onto PC 1 was modulated by experience (Kruskal-Wallis test, H = 9.90, P = 0.04). Center: Associative conditioning in TRP fish altered the PC 1 score (non-parametric multiple comparisons against NAV fish): ALA, Q = −0.67, P = 0.90; TRP, Q = −2.42, P = 0.03; HIS, Q = −1.85, P = 0.13; UNC, Q = −2.59, P = 0.02. Right: PC 1 score was correlated to behavioral odor preference in individual UNC fish. Pearson correlation: r = −0.68 (t = −2.94, P = 0.01). Kendall‘s rank correlation: τ = 0.52 (z = 2.33, P = 0.02). Far right: The correlation between PC 1 score and behavioral odor preference remained significant across all training groups. Pearson correlation: r = −0.34 (t = −2.65, P = 0.01). Kendall‘s rank correlation: τ = 0.20 (z = −2.17, P = 0.03). Box plot: as in (a). c. Shuffling rows (observations, fish) before PCA reduced the variance explained by PC 1 to 27.5 ± 1.1 % (mean ± s.d.); bootstrap test, two-sided: P = 0.005 (left panel) and abolished the correlation between PC 1 and behavior. Pearson correlation: r = 0.00 ± 0.14; mean ± s.d.; bootstrap test, two-sided: P = 0.005 (right panel). Grey vertical bars indicate mean of 10’000 shuffling repetitions, red bars indicate mean of data. d. Shuffling columns (coding structure dimensions) before PCA also reduced the variance explained by PC 1 to 55.9 ± 0.6 % (mean ± s.d.); bootstrap test, two-sided: P = 0.005 (left panel) and reduced the correlation between PC 1 and behavior. Pearson correlation: r = −0.50 ± 0.01; mean ± s.d.; bootstrap test, two-sided: P = 0.01 (right panel). Vertical bars as in panel (c). e. Schematic model of learning-induced changes of odor representations in dpDp (ALA and TRP fish). Cube depicts a low-dimensional representation of olfactory coding space in dpDp, with the primary dimension being an axis representing valence (‘good’ to ‘bad’). Different odors are represented as filled circles, and reinforced odors as larger circles. In ALA fish, the main effect of learning was an enhancement of responses, in particular of the CS+ relative to the CS−. As Ala is innately appetitive, reinforcing this odor does not result in a (major) reorganization along the valence axis (‘minor remapping’). In TRP (and HIS) fish, positive valence is assigned to a previously neutral odor and Ala is ‘devalued’ because it is not paired with food, resulting in a ‘major remapping’ of odor representations along the valence axis. Remapping of trained odors also changes representations of related odors (Fig. 5c, Supplementary Fig. 4a). f. Variance explained by PC 1 as a function of coding structure dimensions in a dataset from NAV fish with a larger number of odors (3154 neurons from 15 animals; eight odors: Phe, Trp, Met, Lys [10−5 M each], a mixture Phe-Met, a mixture Trp-Lys, a mixture of three bile acids [glycocholic acid, taurocholic acid, taurodeoxycholic acid; 10−6 M each], and a food extract). Given eight stimuli, the maximum dimensionality of coding structures is 28. We selected random subsets of dimensions (with replacement) and performed PCA to determine the fraction of variance represented by PC 1 as a function of the number of dimensions. Lines and shadings show median and 95% confidence intervals. Results are consistent with assumption that fraction of variance saturates with increasing dimensionality. g. Correlation between scores on the first four PCs and behavioral odor preference. Each plot shows the relationship between one PC and behavioral odor preference scores for all fish tested in behavioral experiments. A significant correlation to behavioral odor preference was observed only for PC 1 (throughout (g), number of animals as in Fig. 5, and statistical tests as in (a)). PC 1: cf. Fig. 5. PC 2: Pearson correlation: r = 0.11 (t = 0.81, P = 0.42). Kendall‘s rank correlation: τ = −0.02 (z = −0.25, P = 0.80). PC 3: Pearson correlation: r = −0.24 (t = −1.78, P = 0.08). Kendall‘s rank correlation: τ = −0.05 (z = −0.50, P = 0.62). PC 4: Pearson correlation: r = −0.06 (t = −0.42, P = 0.67). Kendall‘s rank correlation: τ = 0.03 (z = 0.28, P = 0.78). ns: P ≥ 0.05; *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001. Additional Text. PCA was performed using different data matrices as inputs. In all cases, the dimension of the input matrix was 136 x 6. Columns represented cosine distances between odor-evoked activity patterns, rows represented N = 68 different animals and two different conditions (control, PIN). Very similar results were obtained when PCA was performed independently for each photoinhibition condition (matrices with dimension 68 x 6). The following data matrices were examined: (1) Columns represented odor pairs defined by odor identity (His:Ser, His:Ala, Ser:Ala, His:Trp, Ser:Trp, Ala:Trp; cf. Fig. 5). (2) Columns represented odor pairs defined by task relevance (CS+:CS–, CS+:Neutral1, CS–:Neutral1, CS+:Neutral2, CS–:Neutral2, Neutral1:Neutral2; cf. Supplementary Fig. 4a). (3) Columns represented odor pairs defined by odor identity, calculated as cosine distance between standardized activity patterns (standardization refers to subtraction of the vector mean, followed by division by the vector’s standard deviation). This is equivalent to using correlation distance (1 − Pearson correlation) as a distance metric (Supplementary Fig. 4b). (4) Columns were defined as in (1) and rows (observations) were shuffled (Supplementary Fig. 4c). (5) Columns were defined as in (1) and columns (variables) were shuffled (Supplementary Fig. 4a). When coding structures represented distances between defined odor pairs (1), PC 1 represented 62% of the variance and the Pearson correlation between the PC 1 score and behavioral odor preference was 0.52 (P = 4 x 10−5). When the coding space represented distances between task-relevant odor pairs (2), the variance represented by PC 1 was lower (57%) while the correlation between PC 1 score and behavioral odor preference remained almost unchanged (r = 0.52; P = 5 x 10−5). When fish (observations) were shuffled across experimental groups (4; (c)), or when distances (variables) were shuffled in each fish (5; (d); 10,000 shufflings each), the variance represented by PC 1 and the correlation between PC 1 score and behavioral odor preference decreased significantly. These observations show that experience-dependent modifications of coding structures depend systematically on the odor and task because shuffling of observations and variables both decreased the explained variance and the correlation to behavioral odor preference. However, when distances were shuffled independently in each fish (5), the explained variance remained relatively high and the correlation between the PC 1 score and behavioral odor preference remained significant. When fish were shuffled across groups (4), in contrast, the explained variance decreased substantially and the correlation between the PC 1 score and behavioral odor preference approached zero. These observations lead to the conclusion that modifications of coding structures in different experimental groups include a ‘common mode’ represented by PC 1. The magnitude of this mode was large when associative conditioning resulted in large odor-value reassignments (TRP, HIS) but very small when odor-value reassignments were minor (ALA). Uncoupled odor exposure resulted in a small recruitment of this mode, possibly reflecting a devaluation of Ala, although this effect was not statistically significant. Major modifications of coding structures therefore occurred along an axis (PC 1) that was closely related to valence.
Supplementary Figure 5 Prediction of odor preference across experimental groups (related to Fig. 5).
a. To confirm that coding structures undergo consistent changes along an axis representing valence we tested whether experience-dependent changes in coding structures can predict odor preference across experimental groups. We defined two subsets of fish: (1) the reference group, which consisted of naïve fish and one experimental group (for example, NAV (N = 13 animals) and ALA (N = 12)), and (2) the test group, which contained all other fish (for example, TRP (N = 16), HIS (N = 15), UNC (N = 12)). We then projected coding structures of the test group onto the PC 1 extracted from the reference group and asked whether the projections (‘test scores’) were correlated to behavioral odor preference in the test group. In all cases, correlations between test scores and behavioral odor preference were high and statistically significant. Hence, PC 1 extracted from all animals in any experimental group (and all NAV fish) defined a direction in coding space that reliably predicted behavioral odor preferences in the other, remaining experimental groups. These results directly demonstrate that different odor-reward assignments, as well as uncoupled odor exposure, resulted in modifications of coding structures along an axis that consistently represented valence (attractiveness). Pearson correlation coefficient (r) is reported (P values were determined using a t test of the null hypothesis of r = 0, two-sided, with d.f. = N–2). b. Same analysis as in (a) but the reference group contained NAV fish and two experimental groups (for example, NAV, HIS, UNC) while the test group contained the other two experimental groups (for example, ALA, TRP). As in (a), statistically significant correlations between test scores and behavioral odor preference were observed in all cases (correlations and statistical tests as in (a)). Number of animals in each group as in (a). c. Same analysis as in (a) but ALA fish were excluded from all analyses. The influence of ALA fish on the results was examined because Ala is an innately attractive odor. Excluding ALA modified correlations only minimally (compare to corresponding plots in b). Hence, results can be generalized in this dataset independently of the innate valence of the CS+. Number of animals in each group, correlations, and statistical tests as in (a). d. Correlation between PC 1 and behavioral odor preference when odor responses to Ala were excluded from the analysis in all experimental groups. Excluding Ala as an odor stimulus reduced the number of coding structure dimensions from six to three. Nevertheless, the correlation between PC 1 and behavioral odor preference remained highly significant (left) and the mapping of coding structures onto the first two PCs (right) was similar to the mapping under control conditions (Fig. 5). Hence, a consistent axis representing valence was identified by PCA even when responses to the innately attractive odor Ala were not considered. Number of animals in each group, correlations, and statistical tests as in (a).
Supplementary Figure 6 Role of inhibition in reorganization of neuronal representations (related to Figs. 6, 7, and 8).
a. Location of dpDp (violet). Green square depicts approximate location of images in Fig. 6a. b. Ca2+ signals evoked by PIN in the absence of odor stimulation, averaged over all neurons of all fish from the first crossing (NAV: N = 9; ALA: N = 12; TRP: N = 13; UNC: N = 8). Orange bar depicts light exposure; shading shows s.e.m.. The PIN-evoked amplitude increase was increased after associative conditioning or uncoupled odor exposure, consistent with the effect of PIN on odor-evoked responses (Fig. 6f). c. In the absence of halorhodopsin expression (UAS:NpHR; Tg(UAS:eNpHR3.0YFP), no Gal4 driver), orange laser light had no detectable effect on the mean odor-evoked activity. Graph shows odor-evoked Ca2+ signal averaged over all neurons, trials and odors under control conditions (black; n = 720, N = 2 animals) and during illumination (orange). Shading shows s.e.m.. Red bar indicates approximate duration of odor stimulation; orange bar depicts light exposure. We also observed no obvious effects on the structure of odor-evoked population activity or on spontaneous activity (not shown). d. PIN reduced the signal-to-background ratio (SBR). SBR was averaged over neuron-odor pairs involving neurons that responded to at least one odor (all fish from the same crossing; NAV: N = 9 animals; ALA: N = 12; TRP: N = 13; UNC: N = 8). Mean and standard deviation of background activity were estimated in separate trials without odor application (Methods). PIN vs control comparisons (paired t test, two-sided, d.f. = n−1): NAV, t = 12.42, P = 5 x 10–33, n = 969 neurons; ALA, t = 24.15, P = 8 x 10–108, n = 1384; TRP: t = 18.87, P = 2 x 10–71, n = 1521; UNC, t = 16.46, P = 4 x 10–52, n = 744. Under control conditions, SBR was increased after associative conditioning and uncoupled odor exposure (Kruskal-Wallis test, n = 4618, d.f. = 3, H = 67.29, P = 1 x 10−14). Non-parametric multiple comparisons against NAV fish, two-sided: ALA, Q = –6.19, P = 1 x 10–9; TRP, Q = – 7.61, P = 5 x 10–14; UNC, Q = –2.55, P = 0.02. Black dotted line: median of NAV fish (control). During PIN, SBR was significantly reduced in ALA and UNC fish (Kruskal-Wallis test, H = 58.20, P = 1 x 10−12). Non-parametric multiple comparisons against NAV fish, two-sided: ALA, Q = 4.49, P < 1x10–5; TRP, Q = – 0.06, P = 1; UNC, Q = 5.56, P = 5 x 10–8. Orange dotted line: median of NAV fish (PIN). Box plot: center line, median; box limits, interquartile range; and whiskers, s.d. e. PIN abolished differences in mean pattern distances between experimental groups. Mean pairwise cosine distance of activity patterns (‘pattern separation’) did not differ between experimental groups during PIN (Kruskal-Wallis test, H = 6.89, P = 0.15). Non-parametric multiple comparisons against NAV: ALA, Q = 0.44, P = 0.98; TRP, Q = –1.90, P = 0.11; HIS, Q = –0.28, P = 1; UNC, Q = –0.96, P = 0.67. Number of animals and d.f. as in Fig. 4d. Box plot: as in (d). f. PIN abolished differences in the structure of distance matrices between NAV fish and TRP or HIS fish (cf. Fig. 4e) but had opposing effects in UNC fish (Kruskal-Wallis test, H = 15.35, P = 0.003). Non-parametric multiple comparisons against NAV: ALA, Q = 0.08, P = 1; TRP, Q = –1.18, P = 0.47; HIS, Q = –0.20, P = 1; UNC, Q = –3.24, P = 0.002. Number of animals and d.f. as in Fig. 4d. Box plot: as in (d). g. Projection of coding structures onto the first two principal components during PIN. Colors show association of each coding structure with the experimental group. For ALA, TRP, and HIS fish, larger marker size indicates higher behavioral discrimination score. Distances between coding structures from individual fish (plot symbols) from different experimental groups were reduced as compared to control (Fig. 5b). Number of animals as in Fig. 5b. h. PIN-induced change of PC 2 score as a function of PC 2 score under control conditions. As for PC 1 (Fig. 8e), the effect of PIN was significantly correlated to the control PC 2 score across all animals (Pearson correlation): r = –0.77 (N = 68, t test of null hypothesis of r = 0, two-sided, d.f. = 66, t = –9.77, P = 2 x 10–14). Kendall‘s rank correlation: τ = –0.58 (test of null hypothesis of τ = 0 using a normal approximation, two-sided, z = –6.96, P = 3 x 10–12). Significant negative correlations were also observed in all individual groups except NAV (Pearson correlation, statistical tests as for all fish): NAV, r = –0.50, t = –1.93, P = 0.08; ALA, r = –0.87, t = –5.57, P = 0.0002; TRP, r = –0.80, t = –4.92, P = 0.0002; HIS, r = –0.87, t = –6.40, P = 2 x 10–5; UNC, r = –0.62, t = –2.50 P = 0.03. Kendall‘s rank correlation: NAV, τ = –0.33, z = –1.59, P = 0.11; ALA, τ = –0.82, z = –3.70, P = 0.0002; TRP, τ = –0.67, z = –3.60, P = 0.0003; HIS, τ = –0.77, z = –4.00, P = 6 x 10−5; UNC, τ = –0.55, z = –2.47, P = 0.01. Number of animals as in Fig. 4d. i. PIN abolished differences in PC 1 score between NAV, and TRP and HIS fish (Fig. 5d; Kruskal-Wallis test, H = 6.69, P = 0.16; same data as in h). Non-parametric multiple comparisons against NAV (PIN): ALA, Q = −0.31, P = 0.99; TRP, Q = 1.94, P = 0.11; HIS, Q = 0.27, P = 1; UNC, Q = 1.02, P = 0.62. Number of animals and d.f. as in Fig. 4d. In all experimental groups except NAV, PIN modulated the PC 1 score (PIN vs control comparisons, Wilcoxon signed rank test, two-sided): NAV, T = 21, P = 0.09; ALA, T = 0, P = 0.0004; TRP, T = 18, P = 0.008; HIS, T = 2, P = 0.0002; UNC, T = 11, P = 0.03). Box plot: as in (d). j. PIN decreased the correlation between PC 1 score and behavioral odor preference in UNC fish. Pearson correlation: r = 0.48 (N = 12, t test of null hypothesis of r = 0, two-sided, d.f. = 10, t = 1.72, P = 0.11). Kendall‘s rank correlation: τ = 0.33 (test of null hypothesis of τ = 0 using a normal approximation, two-sided, z = 1.51, P = 0.13). Orange line shows linear fit; dashed grey line shows linear fit to data obtained under control conditions (Fig. 5e). k. PIN decreased the correlation between PC 1 score and behavioral odor preference across all training groups. Pearson correlation: r = 0.35 (N = 55, t test of null hypothesis of r = 0, two-sided, d.f. = 53, t = 2.72, P = 0.009). Kendall‘s rank correlation: τ = 0.22 (test of null hypothesis of τ = 0 using a normal approximation, two-sided, z = 2.36, P = 0.02). Orange line shows linear fit; dashed grey line shows linear fit to data obtained under control conditions (Fig. 5f). ns: P ≥ 0.05; *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001. Dc: central zone of the dorsal telencephalon; Dl: lateral zone of the dorsal telencephalon; OB: olfactory bulb; TeO: optic tectum; V: ventral telencephalon. M: medial; P: posterior.
Supplementary information
Supplementary Information
Supplementary Figs. 1–6.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Frank, T., Mönig, N.R., Satou, C. et al. Associative conditioning remaps odor representations and modifies inhibition in a higher olfactory brain area. Nat Neurosci 22, 1844–1856 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0495-z
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0495-z
This article is cited by
-
A database and deep learning toolbox for noise-optimized, generalized spike inference from calcium imaging
Nature Neuroscience (2021)
-
Odor hedonics coding in the vertebrate olfactory bulb
Cell and Tissue Research (2021)
-
A virtual reality system to analyze neural activity and behavior in adult zebrafish
Nature Methods (2020)