Abstract
The elementary stimulus features encoded by the olfactory system remain poorly understood. We examined the relationship between 1,666 physical–chemical descriptors of odors and the activity of olfactory bulb inputs and outputs in awake mice. Glomerular and mitral and tufted cell responses were sparse and locally heterogeneous, with only a weak dependence of their positions on physical–chemical properties. Odor features represented by ensembles of mitral and tufted cells were overlapping but distinct from those represented in glomeruli, which is consistent with an extensive interplay between feedforward and feedback inputs to the bulb. This reformatting was well described as a rotation in odor space. The physical–chemical descriptors accounted for a small fraction in response variance, and the similarity of odors in the physical–chemical space was a poor predictor of similarity in neuronal representations. Our results suggest that commonly used physical–chemical properties are not systematically represented in bulbar activity and encourage further searches for better descriptors of odor space.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Relevant articles
Open Access articles citing this article.
-
Neural manifolds for odor-driven innate and acquired appetitive preferences
Nature Communications Open Access 05 August 2023
-
Odor hedonics coding in the vertebrate olfactory bulb
Cell and Tissue Research Open Access 30 January 2021
-
Olfactory bulb granule cells: specialized to link coactive glomerular columns for percept generation and discrimination of odors
Cell and Tissue Research Open Access 06 January 2021
Access options
Access Nature and 54 other Nature Portfolio journals
Get Nature+, our best-value online-access subscription
$29.99 / 30 days
cancel any time
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 12 print issues and online access
$209.00 per year
only $17.42 per issue
Rent or buy this article
Prices vary by article type
from$1.95
to$39.95
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout





Data availability
All data matrices representing glomerular and MTC odor responses and physical–chemical descriptors for the odors in the panel included in the analyses presented here are available on github at: https://github.com/TeamAlbeanu/mosaic_representations.
Code availability
The code used for analysis is available upon request.
References
Kaas, J. H. Topographic maps are fundamental to sensory processing. Brain Res. Bull. 44, 107–112 (1997).
Amoore, J. E., Palmieri, G. & Wanke, E. Molecular shape and odour: pattern analysis of PAPA. Nature 216, 1084–1087 (1967).
Bushdid, C., Magnasco, M. O., Vosshall, L. B. & Keller, A. Humans can discriminate more than 1 trillion olfactory stimuli. Science 343, 1370–1372 (2014).
Haddad, R., Lapid, H., Harel, D. & Sobel, N. Measuring smells.2 0 Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 18, 438–444 (2008).
Koulakov, A. A., Kolterman, B. E., Enikolopov, A. G. & Rinberg, D. In search of the structure of human olfactory space. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 5, 65 (2011).
Meister, M. On the dimensionality of odor space. eLife 4, e07865 (2015).
Schiffman, S. S. Physicochemical correlates of olfactory quality. Science 185, 112–117 (1974).
Su, C.-Y., Menuz, K. & Carlson, J. R. Olfactory perception: receptors, cells, and circuits. Cell 139, 45–59 (2009).
Zarzo, M. & Stanton, D. T. Understanding the underlying dimensions in perfumers’ odor perception space as a basis for developing meaningful odor maps. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 71, 225–247 (2009).
Tetko, I. V. et al. Virtual computational chemistry laboratory—design and description. J. Comput. Aided Mol. Des. 19, 453–463 (2005).
Haddad, R. et al. A metric for odorant comparison. Nat. Methods 5, 425–429 (2008).
Haddad, R. et al. Global features of neural activity in the olfactory system form a parallel code that predicts olfactory behavior and perception. J. Neurosci. 30, 9017–9026 (2010).
Ma, L. et al. Distributed representation of chemical features and tunotopic organization of glomeruli in the mouse olfactory bulb. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 5481–5486 (2012).
Saito, H., Chi, Q., Zhuang, H., Matsunami, H. & Mainland, J. D. Odor coding by a mammalian receptor repertoire. Sci. Signal. 2, ra9 (2009).
Keller, A. & Vosshall, L. B. Olfactory perception of chemically diverse molecules. BMC Neurosci. 17, 55 (2016).
Secundo, L., Snitz, K. & Sobel, N. The perceptual logic of smell. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 25, 107–115 (2014).
Barlow, H. B. Summation and inhibition in the frog’s retina. J. Physiol. 119, 69–88 (1953).
Mombaerts, P. Axonal wiring in the mouse olfactory system. Annu. Rev. Cell Dev. Biol. 22, 713–737 (2006).
Soucy, E. R., Albeanu, D. F., Fantana, A. L., Murthy, V. N. & Meister, M. Precision and diversity in an odor map on the olfactory bulb. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 210–220 (2009).
Cleland, T. A. Early transformations in odor representation. Trends Neurosci. 33, 130–139 (2010).
Mori, K. & Sakano, H. How is the olfactory map formed and interpreted in the mammalian brain? Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 34, 467–499 (2011).
Murthy, V. N. Olfactory maps in the brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 34, 233–258 (2011).
Bozza, T., McGann, J. P., Mombaerts, P. & Wachowiak, M. In vivo imaging of neuronal activity by targeted expression of a genetically encoded probe in the mouse. Neuron 42, 9–21 (2004).
Uchida, N., Takahashi, Y. K., Tanifuji, M. & Mori, K. Odor maps in the mammalian olfactory bulb: domain organization and odorant structural features. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1035–1043 (2000).
Johnson, B. A. & Leon, M. Chemotopic odorant coding in a mammalian olfactory system. J. Comp. Neurol. 503, 1–34 (2007).
Burton, S. D. & Urban, N. N. Greater excitability and firing irregularity of tufted cells underlies distinct afferent-evoked activity of olfactory bulb mitral and tufted cells. J. Physiol. 592, 2097–2118 (2014).
Fukunaga, I., Berning, M., Kollo, M., Schmaltz, A. & Schaefer, A. T. Two distinct channels of olfactory bulb output. Neuron 75, 320–329 (2012).
Igarashi, K. M. et al. Parallel mitral and tufted cell pathways route distinct odor information to different targets in the olfactory cortex. J. Neurosci. 32, 7970–7985 (2012).
Nagayama, S., Takahashi, Y. K., Yoshihara, Y. & Mori, K. Mitral and tufted cells differ in the decoding manner of odor maps in the rat olfactory bulb. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 2532–2540 (2004).
Otazu, G. H., Chae, H., Davis, M. B. & Albeanu, D. F. Cortical feedback decorrelates olfactory bulb output in awake mice. Neuron 86, 1461–1477 (2015).
Amoore, J. E. Stereochemical and vibrational theories of odour. Nature 233, 270–271 (1971).
Schiffman, S. S. Contributions to the physicochemical dimensions of odor: a psychophysical approach. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 237, 164–183 (1974).
Gschwend, O. et al. Neuronal pattern separation in the olfactory bulb improves odor discrimination learning. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1474–1482 (2015).
Wiechert, M. T., Judkewitz, B., Riecke, H. & Friedrich, R. W. Mechanisms of pattern decorrelation by recurrent neuronal circuits. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 1003–1010 (2010).
Cleland, T. A. & Linster, C. On-center/inhibitory-surround decorrelation via intraglomerular inhibition in the olfactory bulb glomerular layer. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6, 5 (2012).
Arevian, A. C., Kapoor, V. & Urban, N. N. Activity-dependent gating of lateral inhibition in the mouse olfactory bulb. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 80–87 (2008).
Yablonka, A., Sobel, N. & Haddad, R. Odorant similarity in the mouse olfactory bulb. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, E2918–E2919 (2012).
Friedman, J., Hastie, T. & Tibshirani, R. Regularization paths for generalized linear models via coordinate descent. J. Stat. Softw. 33, 1–22 (2010).
Belluscio, L. & Katz, L. C. Symmetry, stereotypy, and topography of odorant representations in mouse olfactory bulbs. J. Neurosci. 21, 2113–2122 (2001).
Friedrich, R. W. & Korsching, S. I. Combinatorial and chemotopic odorant coding in the zebrafish olfactory bulb visualized by optical imaging. Neuron 18, 737–752 (1997).
Mori, K., Takahashi, Y. K., Igarashi, K. M. & Yamaguchi, M. Maps of odorant molecular features in the mammalian olfactory bulb. Physiol. Rev. 86, 409–433 (2006).
Cleland, T. A. & Sethupathy, P. Non-topographical contrast enhancement in the olfactory bulb. BMC Neurosci. 7, 7 (2006).
Dhawale, A. K., Hagiwara, A., Bhalla, U. S., Murthy, V. N. & Albeanu, D. F. Non-redundant odor coding by sister mitral cells revealed by light addressable glomeruli in the mouse. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 1404–1412 (2010).
Maaten, Lvander & Hinton, G. Visualizing data using t-SNE. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 9, 2579–2605 (2008).
Linster, C. & Cleland, T. A. Neuromodulation of olfactory transformations. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 40, 170–177 (2016).
Fantana, A. L., Soucy, E. R. & Meister, M. Rat olfactory bulb mitral cells receive sparse glomerular inputs. Neuron 59, 802–814 (2008).
Björck, Ȧke & Golub, G. H. Numerical methods for computing angles between linear subspaces. Math. Comp. 27, 579–594 (1973).
Zhou, Y., Smith, B. H. & Sharpee, T. O. Hyperbolic geometry of the olfactory space. Sci. Adv. 4, eaaq1458 (2018).
Wang, G., Carey, A. F., Carlson, J. R. & Zwiebel, L. J. Molecular basis of odor coding in the malaria vector mosquito Anopheles gambiae. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 4418–4423 (2010).
Zapiec, B. & Mombaerts, P. Multiplex assessment of the positions of odorant receptor-specific glomeruli in the mouse olfactory bulb by serial two-photon tomography. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, E5873–E5882 (2015).
Adam, Y. & Mizrahi, A. Long-term imaging reveals dynamic changes in the neuronal composition of the glomerular layer. J. Neurosci. 31, 7967–7973 (2011).
Zariwala, H. A., Kepecs, A., Uchida, N., Hirokawa, J. & Mainen, Z. F. The limits of deliberation in a perceptual decision task. Neuron 78, 339–351 (2013).
Chen, T.-W. et al. Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature 499, 295–300 (2013).
Gurden, H., Uchida, N. & Mainen, Z. F. Sensory-evoked intrinsic optical signals in the olfactory bulb are coupled to glutamate release and uptake. Neuron 52, 335–345 (2006).
Vincis, R., Lagier, S., Van De Ville, D., Rodriguez, I. & Carleton, A. Sensory-evoked intrinsic imaging signals in the olfactory bulb are independent of neurovascular coupling. Cell Rep. 12, 313–325 (2015).
Buonviso, N. & Chaput, M. A. Response similarity to odors in olfactory bulb output cells presumed to be connected to the same glomerulus: electrophysiological study using simultaneous single-unit recordings. J. Neurophysiol. 63, 447–454 (1990).
Kikuta, S., Fletcher, M. L., Homma, R., Yamasoba, T. & Nagayama, S. Odorant response properties of individual neurons in an olfactory glomerular module. Neuron 77, 1122–1135 (2013).
Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate—a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B 57, 289–300 (1995).
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to acknowledge members of the Albeanu and Koulakov labs and F. Carnevale for critical discussions, and M. Davis and R. Eifert for technical support. This work was supported by the following funding sources: BBRF 2014 Young Investigator grant to H.C.; WSBS PhD support to D.R.K.; Swartz Fellowship to W.G.B.; R01DC014366 to A.A.K.; NSF IOS-1656830 and NIH R01DC014487-03 to D.F.A.; and R01DC017876-01 to A.A.K. and D.F.A.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
D.F.A. and A.A.K. conceived the study. H.C., D.R.K., W.G.B., A.A.K. and D.F.A. contributed to the design of experiments and analyses. H.C. performed the two-photon imaging experiments. W.G.B. performed the intrinsic optical imaging experiments. D.F.A., V.N.M. and A.A.K. supervised the project and wrote the manuscript with input from all authors.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Peer review information: Nature Neuroscience thanks Nathan Schoppa and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Integrated supplementary information
Supplementary Figure 1 Diversity of glomerular, mitral and tufted cell odor responses in awake head-fixed mice
a. Example minimum projection image of intrinsic optical imaging (IOI) signals on the dorsal aspect of the bulb in response to a panel of 60 stimuli across 4 repeats (Supplementary Table 1) in a head-fixed awake mouse; numbers indicate location of functionally best matched glomeruli across the left and right bulb hemispheres. b. Color maps showing correlation of the average odor-evoked response of each pixel in the field of view with respect to reference ROIs (1-4) chosen from (a). Note glomerular formations of contiguous high correlation values emerge in similar locations across the two bulb hemispheres. c. Odor response spectra (ORS, averaged across four repeats) for the functionally best matched glomerular pairs across the two hemibulbs for the 4 ROIs chosen in (a). d. Average resting fluorescence of an example field of view (FOV) in the external plexiform layer (EPL) (left), and example responses (right, dF/F images) to three odors (ethyl hexanoate, allyl butyrate and 2,3-diethylpyrazine) across 3 trials. e. Single trial responses of four example tufted cells to the odors in (a); shaded area marks duration of odor presentation (4s), black lines the average dF/F across three trials (left). Example odor response spectra (ORS, constructed using average dF/F values across three trials) of the four tufted cells from (left) across a set of 55 odors (right). f. Average resting fluorescence of an example field of view in the mitral cell layer (MCL) (left), and example responses (right, dF/F images) to three odors (ethyl valerate, ethyl butyrate and 1-propanethiol) across 3 trials. g. Single trial responses of four example mitral cells to the odors in (f); shaded area marks duration of odor presentation (4s), black lines the average dF/F across three trials (left). Example odor response spectra (ORS, constructed using average dF/F values across three trials) of the four mitral cells from (left) across a set of 55 odors (right).
Supplementary Figure 2 Odor delivery calibration and functional clusters of mitral and tufted cell odor responses in awake head-fixed mice
a. Normalized Photo-ionization device (PID) measuring the relative odor concentration corresponding to the 1:100 and 1:3,000 dilutions in mineral oil for five example odors (allyl butyrate, ethyl valerate, hexanal, ethyl tiglate and acetal). PID readings are normalized to 1:10 oil dilution for each of the five example odors. b. Example locations of fields of view for M/T cell imaging sessions on the dorsal aspect of the olfactory bulb in 3 example mice. c, d. Odor response types obtained via hierarchical clustering and their relative distribution in the population of tufted cells, TC, (c) and mitral cells, MC (d) for two concentrations (corresponding to 1:3,000 and 1:100 nominal oil dilutions) across the same panel of 55 odors. Shaded area (light gray) marks duration of odor presentation (4s). Shaded area (dark gray) marks standard error across trials (TC 1:3,000 dilution: n=3,313 odor-cell pairs, TC 1:100 dilution: n=2,873 odor-cell pairs, MC 1:3,000 dilution: n=5,740 odor-cell pairs, TC 1:100 dilution: n=5,103 odor-cell pairs).
Supplementary Figure 3 Mitral and tufted cell population responses across two oil dilution series in awake mice
a. Distribution of tufted cell-odor response pairs sorted by response strength for the two set oil dilutions (1:3,000 and 1:100) across 55 odors. b. Scatter plots showing the odor induced change in fluorescence (dF/F) of two example tufted cells for the two dilutions used; each dot indicates the response of a cell to a given odor (cell-odor pair) for low versus high concentration of same odor; only cell-odor pairs that were detected as significant in at least one of the two conditions are shown; dotted line marks slope of 1. c. Distribution of mitral cell-odor response pairs sorted by response strength for the two oil dilutions (1:3,000 and 1:100) across 55 odors. d. Scatter plots showing the odor induced change in fluorescence for two example mitral cells or the two dilutions; each dot indicates the response of a cell to a given odor (cell-odor pair) for the low versus high concentration of the same odor; only cell-odor pairs that were detected as significant in at least one of the two conditions are shown; dotted line marks slope of 1. e. Summary scatter plots showing the odor induced change in tufted cells (left) and mitral cell (right) fluorescence for the two oil dilutions series; each dot indicates the average response of 100 cell-odor pairs for the low versus high oil dilution of same odor (1:3,000 versus 1:100), sorted by the low dilution; only cell-odor pairs that were detected as significant in at least one of the two conditions are included; red bars and blue bars mark standard error of the mean for each bin (TC n=2,708 odor-cell pairs, MC n=13,051 odor-cell pairs. We examined the responses of mitral vs. tufted cells to the same set of odors at two different concentrations (~20 fold change). We found that T cells are more sensitive to odors, on average, than M cells, confirming previous observations28,29. We also found that T cells tend to generally increase activity for higher concentration of odors, but M cells could be modulated bi-directionally. Our data indicate that, at the population level, mitral cells have much flatter response variation to concentrations, including some reduction in activity for higher concentrations. Such bi-directional, cell-specific dependence can arise from the known OB circuitry with extensive, distance-independent lateral inhibition. Despite these important differences in the responses properties of M and T cells to odors and concentrations, all conclusions about the representation of physical-chemical descriptors of odors and spatial layout are applicable to both populations. f. Summary histogram showing change in odor evoked mitral cells (black) and tufted cells (grey) responses in response to 1:100 compared to 1:3,000 oil dilutions for a 55 odor panel; the change for each cell odor response is quantified as the Euclidean distance from the diagonal unity line (interrupted line indicating slope of 1 in e), TC n=2,708 odor-cell pairs, MC n=13,051 odor-cell pairs. g. Cumulative distribution of mitral and tufted cell – odor response pairs as a function of dF/F response amplitude. Continuous lines mark mitral cells, dotted lines tufted cells, black traces 1:3,000 dilution, red traces 1:100 dilution, TC n=2,708 odor-cell pairs, MC n=13,051 odor-cell pairs. Consistent with recent findings, on average, tufted cells were generally more sensitive to odors and more active with increasing concentrations, while mitral cells even showed reduced activity at higher concentrations consistent with increasing inhibition. h. Pairwise odor similarity (uncentered correlation coefficient) of corresponding mitral cell response spectra across the two oil dilution used; each dot corresponds to an odor-odor pair. Pairwise similarity of odors at the level of mitral cell responses was generally low (Avg. = 0.260±0.003 SEM for 1:3,000 dilution and 0.180±0.003 for 1:100 dilution, N=1,485 odor pairs, p<0.001, Sign Rank Test), and varied across the sampled dilutions (Supplementary Fig. 3h, i). i. Histogram of pairwise odor similarity (uncentered correlation coefficient) in terms of mitral cell response spectra across a set of 55 odors for 1:3,000 (black) and 1:100 (red) oil dilutions, (n=1485 pairs).
Supplementary Figure 4 Glomerular and mitral/tufted cells tuning to the molecular properties
a.,c. Average correlation for all imaged glomeruli (a) and M/T cells (c) between the ORS of individual ROIs and the property strength vectors (PSV), for the odors in the panel, across the Dragon properties. For each property, PSV is defined as the corresponding normalized vector of values taken by the property across the monomolecular odors in the panel. Properties that took zero values for all odors in the panel were not included in the analysis (a) (1,320 properties over 871 glomeruli and over 1,711 M/T cells). b.,d. Histogram of the number of ROIs (glomeruli – b and M/T cells - d) significantly tuned for a given physical-chemical property, across the set for all imaged fields of view. e. Histograms of significant ROI-odor response to property correlations (two-sided t-test p<0.05, FDR q<0.1) for glomeruli (green, n=13,446) and M/T cells (blue, n=27,992). f. Dimensionality (90% variance explained) of glomerular (green), mitral cells (blue) odor responses (across all sampled fields of view) and molecular properties strength vectors (PSV) as function of number of odorants included in the analysis. Shaded areas indicate +/- one standard deviation from a mean of 1,000 samples (different combination of odorants) for a given number of odorants included. g. Dimensionality (90% variance explained) of glomerular (green), mitral cells (blue) odor responses (across all 49 monomolecular odorants) as function of number of regions of interests included. Shaded areas indicate +/- one standard deviation from a mean of 1,000 samples (different combination of ROIs) for a set number of ROIs included in the analysis. h. Principal components exchange (PCX) analysis with varying amounts of noise added to glomerular responses. Percent variance explained of glomerular (green), mitral cells (blue) odor responses, molecular property strength vectors (black) shown as function of the number of experimentally observed glomerular responses (no added noise) principal components included in the analysis. Variance of the noise with respect to original signal is shown as signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) coded by the colorbar legend (Right). i. Least absolute shrinkage and selection operator (LASSO) regression based on a subset of on varying the λ parameter (all physical-chemical properties used are included), describing the relationship between odor pairwise similarity across properties versus glomerular (Left) and respectively M/T cells (Right) responses; (light green) all imaged hemibulbs were used as a training set for optimizing the regression; (dark green) half of the fields of view were used for training and the remaining half for cross-validation (FOV cross-validation); (pink) all imaged hemibulbs were used for training, while one pair of odors was iteratively left out during training and added back subsequently for cross-validation (jackknife, odor cross-validation); (red) half of the fields of view were used for training and the remaining half for cross-validation; in addition, one pair of odors was iteratively left out during training and subsequently added back for cross-validation (jackknife, FOV and odor cross-validation). j. Pairwise odor similarity expressed in terms of physical-chemical properties versus neuronal responses. Similarity is calculated as Euclidean distance in odor physical-chemical space and respectively Pearson’s correlation between the odor response spectra (ORS) of cell pairs using a greedy algorithm (yellow line, Methods). Blue line marks FOV jackknife analysis: half of the fields of view were used for training and the remaining half for cross-validation; Red line marks odor pair jackknife analysis: one pair of odors was iteratively left out during training and subsequently added back for cross-validation; Black line marks odor similarity given jackknife analysis performed on both fields of view, as well as by iteratively leaving out one odor pair.
Supplementary Figure 5 Relationship between Dragon physical-chemical properties and glomerular as well as M/T cell responses across different time scales in awake and anaesthetized mice
a.,b.,c. Results of principal component exchange (PCX) analysis for anaesthetized (a), awake (b) glomerular responses and molecular properties (c). Percent of variance explained is shown as a function of the number of included principal components, PCs. (a) Percent variance explained of anaesthetized (red), awake (green) glomerular odor responses, molecular property strength vectors (grey) and random data controls (black) shown as function of the number of anaesthetized glomerular responses principal components, awake glomerular responses principal components (b) and of molecular properties (PSV) principal components (c). d.,e. Least absolute shrinkage and selection operator (LASSO) regression based on a subset of physical-chemical properties selected from the 1,666 set, describing the relationship between odor pairwise similarity across properties versus glomerular responses. Each trace shows the LASSO performance under various cross-validation conditions. (e) Predicting odor-odor response distance using shuffled molecular properties; the properties strength vector associated with each odorant is randomly swapped with the property strength vector of another molecule in the panel. f.,g.,h. Percent variance explained of mitral cell odor responses across different time intervals from stimulus onset (0.5 s to 4.0 s, coded by strength of the shade of blue), molecular property strength vectors (grey) and random data controls (black) shown as function of the number of molecular properties principal components (f) and mitral cell responses (0.5 s vs. 4.0 s) principal components (g, h). i.,j.,k. LASSO regression describing the relationship between odor pairwise similarity across properties versus mitral cell responses sampled across different time intervals: 0.5s (i), 1.0 s (j), 1.5 s (k). Each trace shows LASSO performance under various cross-validation conditions.
Supplementary Figure 6 Local similarity and diversity of mitral cell odor responses
a. Average resting fluorescence multiphoton image in the mitral cell layer (n=112). b. Numbers indicate the relative positions of 112 mitral cell bodies in the imaged field of view; c. Example ORS (average dF/F across 3 trials) of mitral cell bodies within an arbitrarily picked 75µm diameter region circled in (a) sorted according to functional chemical groups (aldehydes, tiglates, ketones, furfuryls, alcohols, acids, ethyl esters, thiazoles); note that neighboring neurons (enclosed in circle from a, bottom) have diverse, as well as similar odor tuning (for example, cells 45 and 84 vs. cell 12). We probed the local diversity further, by rearranging the odor response spectra (ORS) such as to group odors in terms of chemical classes (ketones, acids, etc.). Mitral cells responding to many different classes of chemicals can be found even within a small region of glomerular size. d. ORS (average dF/F across 3 trials) of mitral cells from three example clusters (orange, blue and red) from the FOV in (a), and from a different FOV (black) identified using functional clustering (average linkage) for 1:100 oil dilution (Left) and plotted correspondingly to maintain cell identity for the 1:3,000 oil dilution (Right). Numbers correspond to position of somata shown in (b). The spread of cells belonging to the three clusters is highlighted by the corresponding colored position indices shown in (b). One additional cluster from a different animal is exemplified in black. e. Histogram of pairwise ORS correlations. Red and green traces correspond to putative sister pairs (defined at one dilution) from all, and respectively from those FOVs sampled at both dilutions; Pearson’s correlations for each dilution are included; blue trace corresponds to correlations between putative sister cell pairs defined via functional clustering at one dilution, and sampled at the other dilution. ORS similarity between co-imaged, randomly picked pairs is shown in black. On average, we identified 4 ± 2 s.d. functional clusters per FOV, N=25, and each cluster contained 6 ± 3 s.d. putative sister cells. We asked whether the pairwise similarity and cell identity within these functional clusters varied across the two dilutions. First, keeping track of the cells’ identity, we probed the similarity in odor tuning between cell pairs within a cluster (defined at one nominal dilution) at the other sampled dilution. On average, the pairwise cell similarity decreased (R2=0.41±0.26 vs. 0.20±0.26, N=992 cell pairs tested for the other dilution), but was significantly higher (p<0.001) than for randomly picked cell pairs (R2=0.08±0.01, N=992, 10,000 bootstrapped iterations). Further, if two cells were clustered together by their odor response at one dilution, they were more likely to be part of the same cluster at the other dilution compared to randomly assigned cells (N=6 FOVs sampled at both dilutions). For example, one FOV had 376 putative sister cell pairs in one concentration, and 255 sister cell pairs in the second concentration; 100 cell pairs were shared by both concentrations. After 250,000 iterations of randomly assigning sister cells, 0 had more than 100 shared pairs, with a mean bootstrap overlap of 15±7 shared pairs. f. Average correlation comparisons between mitral/tufted cells in the same FOV (x axis) and cells randomly sampled cells across FOVs within the same animal (y axis). The red line marks unity slope. Error bars show one standard deviation from the mean for 1,000 random samples. Quantifying the relationship between odor tuning and spatial proximity for tufted and mitral cells indicated that odors triggered, on average, low pairwise correlations (uncentered correlation coefficient) in the odor tuning of mitral and tufted cells across the population (Avg. Similarity ~0.08 to ~0.14 across the two dilutions of a 55 odors panel for ~30,000 mitral and 12,000 tufted cell pairs). We observed only a weak relation between functional similarity and spatial location of MTCs (~15% for MCs, ~22% for TCs excess of more similar MC and TC pairs than expected by chance if MTC placement and odor tuning were independent 19).
Supplementary Figure 7 Shuffling glomerular responses decreases the performance of the rotation model in matching experimentally observed mitral cell responses
Model mitral cells responses were generated by taking samples from shuffled glomerular responses (for each glomerulus, the order of responses was shuffled). (Left) Percent variance explained of shuffled glomerular (green), mitral (blue) and simulated mitral (dashed blue line) cells responses shown as function of the number of included shuffled glomerular responses principal components. (Right) Percent variance explained of shuffled glomerular (green), mitral (blue) and simulated mitral (dashed blue line) responses shown as function of the number of included mitral cells response principal components. Note that response variance of mitral cells built by rotating shuffled glomerular responses (dashed blue line) deviates substantially from the experimentally observed mitral cell responses (solid blue).
Supplementary Figure 8 Calculating Jordan principal angles between different subspaces as a function of the number of principal components included
Jordan’s principal angles between different subspaces (selected to maximize the percentage of variance explained) as function of the number of principal components included in the analysis. The principal angles between the subspaces of interest are calculated iteratively including more PCs of the respective subspaces. A 2D histogram is shown with each bin corresponding to the number of counts of specific angles (in radians) calculated for increasing number of included PCs. a. Principal angles between a subspace and itself as function of the number of PCs included. To maintain contrast in the color scheme rendering the number of counts in the 2D histogram, the 0 bin angle entry in the 2D histogram was re-scaled accordingly. b. Expected principal angles between two random subspaces of same dimensionality (embedded in same 50 dimensions) over 1,000 iterations as function of the number of PCs included. c. Principal angles between the glomerular odor responses subspace and itself (plus added noise) are calculated for increasing amounts of added noise (decreasing SNR). d. Principal angles between two spaces which have the same eigenvectors but different eigenvalues (different amounts of variance explained). Numerical error (expected in calculating Jordan’s principal angles) results in a small, non-zero angle. e. Principal component exchange (PCX) analysis identifies differences between overlapping subspaces which have different amounts of signal variance (different eigenvalues) distributed across different orthogonal dimensions (principal components), while overall spanning the same PCs.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Chae, H., Kepple, D.R., Bast, W.G. et al. Mosaic representations of odors in the input and output layers of the mouse olfactory bulb. Nat Neurosci 22, 1306–1317 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0442-z
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0442-z
This article is cited by
-
Neural manifolds for odor-driven innate and acquired appetitive preferences
Nature Communications (2023)
-
Spatial transcriptomic reconstruction of the mouse olfactory glomerular map suggests principles of odor processing
Nature Neuroscience (2022)
-
Olfactory bulb granule cells: specialized to link coactive glomerular columns for percept generation and discrimination of odors
Cell and Tissue Research (2021)
-
Odor hedonics coding in the vertebrate olfactory bulb
Cell and Tissue Research (2021)
-
Structure and flexibility in cortical representations of odour space
Nature (2020)