Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Locus coeruleus activity improves cochlear implant performance

Subjects

Abstract

Cochlear implants (CIs) are neuroprosthetic devices that can provide hearing to deaf people1. Despite the benefits offered by CIs, the time taken for hearing to be restored and perceptual accuracy after long-term CI use remain highly variable2,3. CI use is believed to require neuroplasticity in the central auditory system, and differential engagement of neuroplastic mechanisms might contribute to the variability in outcomes4,5,6,7. Despite extensive studies on how CIs activate the auditory system4,8,9,10,11,12, the understanding of CI-related neuroplasticity remains limited. One potent factor enabling plasticity is the neuromodulator noradrenaline from the brainstem locus coeruleus (LC). Here we examine behavioural responses and neural activity in LC and auditory cortex of deafened rats fitted with multi-channel CIs. The rats were trained on a reward-based auditory task, and showed considerable individual differences of learning rates and maximum performance. LC photometry predicted when CI subjects began responding to sounds and longer-term perceptual accuracy. Optogenetic LC stimulation produced faster learning and higher long-term accuracy. Auditory cortical responses to CI stimulation reflected behavioural performance, with enhanced responses to rewarded stimuli and decreased distinction between unrewarded stimuli. Adequate engagement of central neuromodulatory systems is thus a potential clinically relevant target for optimizing neuroprosthetic device use.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Behavioural assessment of CI learning.
Fig. 2: Dynamic LC activity during CI learning.
Fig. 3: LC pairing enhances CI learning.
Fig. 4: Auditory cortical responses in CI rats.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are further available on Zenodo (https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7226424) or the NYU Data Catalogue (https://datacatalog.med.nyu.edu/dataset/10584).  Source data are provided with this paper.

Code availability

Custom code used in this study is available on Github at https://github.com/ErinGlennon/CI_rat_analysis.git.

References

  1. Merzenich, M. M., Michelson, R. P., Pettit, C. R., Schindler, R. A. & Reid, M. Neural encoding of sound sensation evoked by electrical stimulation of the acoustic nerve. Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. 82, 486–503 (1973).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Chang, S. A. et al. Performance overtime on adults with simultaneous bilateral cochlear implants. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 21, 35–43 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Tang, L. et al. Rehabilitation and psychosocial determinants of cochlear implant outcomes in older adults. Ear Hear. 38, 663–671 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Nourski, K. V. et al. Direct recordings from the auditory cortex in a cochlear implant user. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 14, 435–450 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Fallon, J. B., Irvine, D. R. & Shepherd, R. K. Neural prostheses and brain plasticity. J. Neural Eng. 6, 065008 (2009).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  6. Reiss, L. A., Turner, C. W., Karsten, S. A. & Gantz, B. J. Plasticity in human pitch perception induced by tonotopically mismatched electro-acoustic stimulation. Neuroscience 256, 43–52 (2014).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Svirsky, M. A., Silveira, A., Neuburger, H., Teoh, S. W. & Suarez, H. Long-term auditory adaptation to a modified peripheral frequency map. Acta. Otolaryngol. 124, 381–386 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Johnson, L. A., Della Santina, C. C. & Wang, X. Selective neuronal activation by cochlear implant stimulation in auditory cortex of awake primate. J. Neurosci. 36, 12468–12484 (2016).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Johnson, L. A., Della Santina, C. C. & Wang, X. Representations of time-varying cochlear implant stimulation in auditory cortex of awake marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). J. Neurosci. 37, 7008–7022 (2017).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Klinke, R., Kral, A., Heid, S., Tillein, J. & Hartmann, R. Recruitment of the auditory cortex in congenitally deaf cats by long-term cochlear electrostimulation. Science 285, 1729–1733 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Fallon, J. B., Shepherd, R. K. & Irvine, D. R. Effects of chronic cochlear electrical stimulation after an extended period of profound deafness on primary auditory cortex organization in cats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 39, 811–820 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Isaiah, A., Vongpaisal, T., King, A. J. & Hartley, D. E. Multisensory training improves auditory spatial processing following bilateral cochlear implantation. J. Neurosci. 34, 11119–11130 (2014).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Blamey, P. et al. Factors affecting auditory performance of postlinguistically deaf adults using cochlear implants: an update with 2251 patients. Audiol. Neurootol. 18, 36–47 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Moore, D. R. & Shannon, R. V. Beyond cochlear implants: awakening the deafened brain. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 686–691 (2009).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Glennon, E., Svirsky, M. A. & Froemke, R. C. Auditory cortical plasticity in cochlear implant users. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 60, 108–114 (2020).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Lu, W., Xu, J. & Shepherd, R. K. Cochlear implantation in rats: a new surgical approach. Hear. Res. 205, 115–122 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Hancock, K. E., Noel, V., Ryugo, D. K. & Delgutte, B. Neural coding of interaural time differences with bilateral cochlear implants: effects of congenital deafness. J. Neurosci. 30, 14068–14079 (2010).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Rosskothen-Kuhl, N. & Illing, R. B. Nonlinear development of the populations of neurons expressing c-Fos under sustained electrical intracochlear stimulation in the rat auditory brainstem. Brain Res. 1347, 33–41 (2010).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Tillein, J. et al. Cortical representation of interaural time difference in congenital deafness. Cereb. Cortex 20, 492–506 (2010).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Hancock, K. E., Chung, Y. & Delgutte, B. Congenital and prolonged adult-onset deafness cause distinct degradations in neural ITD coding with bilateral cochlear implants. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 14, 393–411 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Chung, Y., Hancock, K. E. & Delgutte, B. Neural coding of interaural time differences with bilateral cochlear implants in unanesthetized rabbits. J. Neurosci. 36, 5520–5531 (2016).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. King, J., Shehu, I., Roland, J. T. Jr, Svirsky, M. A. & Froemke, R. C. A physiological and behavioral system for hearing restoration with cochlear implants. J. Neurophysiol. 116, 844–858 (2016).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Tillein, J., Hubka, P. & Kral, A. Monaural congenital deafness affects aural dominance and degrades binaural processing. Cereb. Cortex 26, 1762–1777 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Chung, Y., Buechel, B. D., Sunwoo, W., Wagner, J. D. & Delgutte, B. Neural ITD sensitivity and temporal coding with cochlear implants in an animal model of early-onset deafness. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 20, 37–56 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Rosskothen-Kuhl, N., Buck, A. N., Li, K. & Schnupp, J. W. Microsecond interaural time difference discrimination restored by cochlear implants after neonatal deafness. eLife 10, e59300 (2021).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Martins, A. R. & Froemke, R. C. Coordinated forms of noradrenergic plasticity in the locus coeruleus and primary auditory cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1483–1492 (2015).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Glennon, E. et al. Locus coeruleus activation accelerates perceptual learning. Brain Res. 1709, 39–49 (2019).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Holden, L. K. et al. Factors affecting open-set word recognition in adults with cochlear implants. Ear Hear. 34, 342–360 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Edeline, J. M., Manunta, Y. & Hennevin, E. Induction of selective plasticity in the frequency tuning of auditory cortex and auditory thalamus neurons by locus coeruleus stimulation. Hear. Res. 274, 75–84 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Devilbiss, D. M., Page, M. E. & Waterhouse, B. D. Locus ceruleus regulates sensory encoding by neurons and networks in waking animals. J. Neurosci. 26, 9860–9872 (2006).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Sara, S. J. The locus coeruleus and noradrenergic modulation of cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 211–223 (2009).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Aston-Jones, G. & Cohen, J. D. An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28, 403–450 (2005).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Sugiyama, D. et al. In vivo patch-clamp recording from locus coeruleus neurones in the rat brainstem. J. Physiol. 590, 2225–2231 (2012).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Schultz, W., Dayan, P. & Montague, P. R. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275, 1593–1599 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Watabe-Uchida, M., Eshel, N. & Uchida, N. Neural circuitry of reward prediction error. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 40, 373–394 (2017).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Kral, A. & Tillein, J. Brain plasticity under cochlear implant stimulation. Adv. Otorhinolaryngol. 64, 89–108 (2006).

    Google Scholar 

  37. Giraud, A. L., Truy, E. & Frackowiak, R. Imaging plasticity in cochlear implant patients. Audiol. Neurootol. 6, 381–393 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Irvine, D. R., Fallon, J. B. & Kamke, M. R. Plasticity in the adult central auditory system. Acoust. Aust. 34, 13–17 (2006).

    Google Scholar 

  39. Carcea, I., Insanally, M. N. & Froemke, R. C. Dynamics of auditory cortical activity during behavioural engagement and auditory perception. Nat. Commun. 8, 14412 (2017).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Bledsoe, S. C., Nagase, S., Miller, J. M. & Altschuler, R. A. Deafness-induced plasticity in the mature central auditory system. Neuroreport 7, 225–229 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Abbott, S. D., Hughes, L. F., Bauer, C. A., Salvi, R. & Caspary, D. M. Detection of glutamate decarboxylase isoforms in rat inferior colliculus following acoustic exposure. Neuroscience 93, 1375–1381 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Vale, C. & Sanes, D. H. The effect of bilateral deafness on excitatory and inhibitory synaptic strength in the inferior colliculus. Eur. J. Neurosci. 16, 2394–2404 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Argence, M., Vassias, I., Kerhuel, L., Vidal, P.-P. & de Waele, C. Stimulation by cochlear implant in unilaterally deaf rats reverses the decrease of inhibitory transmission in the inferior colliculus. Eur. J. Neurosci. 28, 1589–1602 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Scholl, B. & Wehr, M. Disruption of balanced cortical excitation and inhibition by acoustic trauma. J. Neurophysiol. 100, 646–656 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Rosskothen-Kuhl, N., Hildebrandt, H., Birkenhäger, R. & Illing, R. B. Astrocyte hypertrophy and microglia activation in the rat auditory midbrain is induced by electrical intracochlear stimulation. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 12, 43 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Dorrn, A. L., Yuan, K., Barker, A. J., Schreiner, C. E. & Froemke, R. C. Developmental sensory experience balances cortical excitation and inhibition. Nature 465, 932–936 (2010).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Froemke, R. C. et al. Long-term modification of cortical synapses improves sensory perception. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 79–88 (2013).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. Witten, I. B. et al. Recombinase-driver rat lines: tools, techniques, and optogenetic application to dopamine-mediated reinforcement. Neuron 72, 721–733 (2011).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Carter, M. E. et al. Tuning arousal with optogenetic modulation of locus coeruleus neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 1526–1533 (2010).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Muller, M. Frequency representation in the rat cochlea. Hear. Res. 51, 247–254 (1991).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Paxinos, G. & Watson, C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates 7th edn (Academic Press, 2013).

  52. Feldkamp, L. A., Davis, L. C. & Kress, J. W. Practical cone-beam algorithm. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1, 612–619 (1984).

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  53. Barrett, J. F. & Keat, N. Artifacts in CT: recognition and avoidance. Radiographics 24, 1679–1691 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Duerinckx, A. J. & Macovski, A. Polychromatic streak artifacts in computed tomography images. J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr. 2, 481–487 (1978).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  55. Joseph, P. M. & Spital, R. D. A method for correcting bone induced artifacts in computed tomography scanners. J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr. 2, 100–108 (1978).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  56. Botros, A., van Dijk, B. & Killian, M. AutoNR: an automated system that measures ECAP thresholds with the Nucleus Freedom cochlear implant via machine intelligence. Artif. Intell. Med. 40, 15–28 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank M. Azadpour, N. Capach, I. Carcea, M. Chesler, M. Donegan, P. Gibson, Z. Gironda, A.E. Hight, M. Insanally, J. Kirk, D. Lin, K.A. Martin, O. Mishkit, J. Multani, J. Neukam, J.T. Roland Jr., E. Sagi, D. Sanes, S. Sara, J.K. Scarpa, J. Schiavo, M. Semerkant, I. Shehu, S. Shokat Fadaei, D. Smyth, J. Tranos, C. Treaba, N. Tritsch and S. Waltzman for comments, discussions and technical assistance; Cochlear for technical support; the Genotyping Core Laboratory of NYU Langone Health for help with genotyping transgenic rats; CILcare for cochleogram analysis; the Stanford Neuroscience Gene Vector and Virus Core and the Deisseroth laboratory for AAVDJ-ef1α-DIO-GCaMP6s (Fig. 2 and Extended Data Figs. 6 and 7); C. Schaulsohn for artwork in Figs. 1a, 2a and 3b. This work was funded by a Vilcek Scholar Award (to E.G.); a Howard Hughes Medical Institute Medical Research Fellowship Award (to A.Z.), a Hirschl/Weill-Caulier Career Award (to R.C.F.); and the National Institutes of Health (grant number F30-DC017351 to E.G., T32GM007308 to E.G., R01-DC003937 to M.A.S., and R01-DC012557 to R.C.F.). Partial support was also received from a research contract from Cochlear to J. T. Roland Jr. In vivo imaging was performed under the DART Preclinical Imaging Core partially funded by the NYU Laura and Isaac Perlmutter Cancer Center Support Grant, NIH/NCI P30CA016087. The Center for Advanced Imaging Innovation and Research (CAI2R, www.cai2r.net) at NYU School of Medicine is supported by NIH/NIBIB P41 EB017183.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

E.G. conducted extracellular electrophysiological recordings, in vivo optogenetics, fibre photometry and CI training. E.G. and A.Z. conducted behavioural testing, IHC and CT/MRI co-registration analysis. S.V. conducted in vivo whole-cell recordings. All other analysis was done by E.G. Y.Z.W. designed co-registration analysis. E.G, M.A.S. and R.C.F designed the study and wrote the paper.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Mario A. Svirsky or Robert C. Froemke.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature thanks the anonymous reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer review reports are available.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data figures and tables

Extended Data Fig. 1 Auditory conditioning on go/no-go task in normal-hearing rats.

a, Normal-hearing behavioral response curves from three example rats that reached training criteria. Arrowhead, target tone was 4 kHz for all animals. Error bars, response rate ± 95% confidence intervals. b, Average initiation rates for final five days of normal-hearing behavioral performance (N = 16 rats). Error bars, mean ± s.e.m. c, Average hits and false positive rates for final five days (N = 16 rats). d, Average behavioral performance (d’) for final five days (N = 16 rats). Error bars, mean±s.e.m. e, Days to d’ ≥ 1.0 (N = 16 rats). Error bars, median±interquartile range.

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 2 CI programming.

a, Depiction of center frequencies for individual CI channels. b, Frequency allocation tables used to select tones for behavioral conditioning based on center frequency of channels with different electrode configurations. c, Example electrodograms, showing that only the CI channel for selected center frequency was activated by the tone.

Extended Data Fig. 3 Deafened animals used the CI to perform the auditory task.

a-p, Behavioral performance for all 16 rats from Fig. 1. Each subpanel is a separate animal. Upper left, behavioral response rates across CI channels with the CI turned on (black) or turned off (red). Arrowhead, target tone programmed to activate channel 3 or 4. Error bars, response rate ± 95% confidence interval. Upper right, d’ over days on stage two. Lower left, hit rate over time. Lower right, false positive rate over time. No difference in d’ for rats with 7-8 active channels (N = 8) vs 3 active channels (N = 8) on testing day for implant on vs off (7-8 active channel implant on d’: 1.8 ± 0.2, 3 active channel implant on d’: 1.6 ± 0.2, p = 0.61, unpaired two-tailed t-test; 7-8 active channel implant off d’: 0.0 ± 0.1, 3 active channel implant off d’: −0.1 ± 0.1, p = 0.39)

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 4 Behavioral and electrophysiological confirmation of deafness in implanted rats.

a, Hit rates were lower and false positives were higher in rats when the CI was off (N = 16 rats, on vs off, hits: p < 0.0001; false positives: p = 0.02; paired Wilcoxon signed-rank test). b, Initiation rates decreased when the CI was turned off (N = 16 rats, on vs off, p = 0.0002; paired Wilcoxon signed-rank test). c, Hairs cells were lesioned by the deafening process. Representative immunohistochemistry from normal-hearing cochlea from the right ear (‘NH’) and deafened cochlea from the left ear (‘Deafened’) stained with hair cell marker Myo7a. Scale bar, 100 µm. Summary of hair cell counts showed that deafening significantly reduced the number of OHCs (‘NH’, 2746 ± 116 OHCs in normal-hearing animals; ‘Deaf’ 52 ± 46 OHCs in deafened animals; 98.1% loss of OHCs, N = 4, p = 0.0001, Student’s one-tailed paired t-test) and IHCs (‘NH’ 787 ± 34 IHCs; ‘Deaf’ 387 ± 118 IHCs; 50.8% loss of IHCs, N = 4, p = 0.04, Student’s one-tailed paired t-test). d, ABRs were gone both acutely (immediately after deafening) and weeks later. Example waveforms from the same rat with 4 kHz stimuli at 70 dB SPL, 80 dB SPL, and 90 dB SPL; chronic post-deafening ABRs measured 41 days post-deafening. e, EABRs intact both acutely and weeks later. Stimulation was at ECAP threshold. f, Standard deviation (SD) of acoustically-evoked ABRs (solid lines) and baseline noise (dashed lines) across frequencies from sample rat displayed in d,e. Red, acute deafness; blue, chronic deafness 41 days later. g, Summary of ABR/EABR recordings with stimuli of 4 kHz at 90 dB SPL or ECAP threshold in 14 rats pre- and post-deafening (6 pre and acute post; 4 chronic post; 4 pre, acute, and chronic post). ABRs were equivalent to baseline noise after deafening (‘Pre’, before deafening noise SD: 1.1 ± 0.2 µV, before deafening ABR SD: 3.2 ± 0.3 µV, N = 10, p = 0.0003,; ‘Acute’ just after deafening noise SD: 1.2 ± 0.1 µV, just after deafening ABR SD: 1.2 ± 0.3 µV, N = 10, p = 0.61; ‘Chronic’ weeks after deafening noise SD: 1.6 ± 0.2 µV, weeks after deafening ABR SD: 1.6 ± 0.1 µV, N = 8, p = 0.55; Student’s paired two-tailed t-tests). EABRs were significantly evoked (‘Acute’ just after deafening noise SD: 1.0 ± 0.1 µV, just after deafening EABR SD: 3.9 ± 0.7 µV, N = 10, p = 0.001; ‘Chronic’ weeks after deafening noise SD: 1.2 ± 0.1 µV, weeks after deafening EABR SD: 4.6 ± 0.8 µV, N = 8, p = 0.002, Student’s paired two-tailed t-test). Chronic measurements made between 13–42 days after deafening. h-k, Behavioral performance of four animals from g showing behavioral responses ± 95% confidence interval and d’ values post-deafening on stage 2 when CI is on (black) vs off (red) (upper left), d’ over time (upper right), hit rates and false positive over time (middle), and sample ABR and EABR traces (bottom). *, p < 0.05; **, p < 0.01

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 5 Individual variability with CI use was related to false positive rate but not insertion depth, impedance, ECAP thresholds, hit rates, or normal-hearing performance.

a, Example x-rays of full insertion (8 channels) and partial insertion (4–7 channels). b, Days to d’ ≥ 1.0 did not differ based on CI insertion depth (full insertion: N = 9 rats vs partial insertion: N = 7 rats, p = 0.32, unpaired two-tailed Mann–Whitney test). Error bars, median±interquartile range. c, Average impedance of active CI channels over time. Grey dashed lines, individual rats (N = 16). Black, mean±s.e.m. d, Days to d’ ≥ 1.0 did not correlate with initial impedance values (N = 16 rats, Pearson’s r: 0.23, p = 0.40). e, Days to d’ ≥ 1.0 did not correlate with ECAP threshold (N = 16 rats, Pearson’s r: −0.21, p = 0.44). f, CI learning days to d’≥1.0 did not correlate normal-hearing learning days to d’ ≥ 1.0 (N = 16 rats, Pearson’s r: 0.07, p = 0.79). g, Days to d’ ≥ 1.0 did not correlate with hit rate (N = 16 rats, Pearson’s r: 0.16, p = 0.56). h, Days to d’ ≥ 1.0 correlated with false positives (N = 16 rats, Pearson’s r: 0.61, p = 0.01). i, During normal-hearing training, days to d’ ≥ 1.0 did not correlate with maximum d’ performance (N = 16 rats, Pearson’s r: −0.37, p = 0.16). j, Hit rates on first CI day were uncorrelated with hit rates on last normal-hearing day (N = 16 rats, Pearson’s r: 0.02, p = 0.93). k, False positive rates on first CI day were uncorrelated with hit rates on last normal-hearing day (N = 16 rats, Pearson’s r: 0.03, p = 0.90). l, d’ values on first CI day were uncorrelated with hit rates on last normal-hearing day (N = 16 rats, Pearson’s r: −0.03, p = 0.92)

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 6 Fiber photometry miss/withhold analysis.

a, Example LC activity aligned to tone onset during stage one CI training on miss trials, showing dF/F in high-miss rate behavioral session 1 (top) and in later low-miss behavioral session 5 (bottom). Error bars, mean ±s.e.m. b, In stage one, LC dF/F signals during miss trials were highest in sessions where the miss rates were highest (N = 4 rats, n = 21 sessions, Pearson’s r: 0.61, p = 0.003). c, LC signals were not predictive of false positive trials in stage two (tone-aligned: N = 4 rats, n = 39 sessions, Pearson’s r: −0.03, p = 0.83; response-aligned: N = 4 rats, n = 38 sessions, Pearson’s r: −0.30, p = 0.06). d, Example LC activity aligned to tone onset during stage two (foil and target training). Miss and withhold trials in high-false positive (F+) behavioral session 2 (top); miss and withhold trials in low-F+ behavioral session 12 (bottom). Error bars, mean ±s.e.m. e, Tone-aligned normalized dF/F LC signals during miss trials over all stage two sessions (N = 4 rats, n = 36 sessions, Pearson’s r: −0.01, p = 0.97). f, Tone-aligned normalized dF/F LC signals during withhold trials over all sessions (N = 4 rats, n = 40 sessions, Pearson’s r: 0.24, p = 0.14)

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 7 LC activity in normal-hearing reversal learning.

a, Schematic of go/no-go auditory behavioral task in normal-hearing rats when target tone is changed to a different frequency. After training to response to one target tone (black) while withholding from foil tones (green/red), one of the previously unrewarded tones (green) became the rewarded tone and the previously rewarded tone (black) became unrewarded. b, Example of animal performance on this task to first and second rewarded tones. Black arrowhead, first rewarded tone; green arrowhead, second rewarded tone. Error bars, response rates ± 95% confidence intervals. c, Tone-aligned LC activity, response-aligned LC activity, and miss rates across behavioral sessions in an example animal. Black, miss rates. Green, dF/F responses either tone-aligned (filled symbols, solid lines) or behavioral response-aligned (open symbols, dashed lines). d, Miss rates across all sessions were not correlated with tone-aligned normalized LC dF/F (N = 2 rats, n = 23 behavioral sessions, Pearson’s r: −0.05, p = 0.8). e, Miss rates across all sessions were correlated with dF/F when aligned to behavioral response (N = 2 rats, n = 23 behavioral sessions, Pearson’s r: 0.45, p = 0.03). f, Tone-aligned LC activity, response-aligned LC activity, and false positive rates across behavioral sessions in an example animal. Black, false positives. Green, dF/F responses either tone-aligned (filled symbols, solid lines) or behavioral response-aligned (open symbols, dashed lines). G, False positive rates across all sessions were not correlated with tone-aligned normalized LC dF/F (N = 2 rats, n = 23 behavioral sessions, Pearson’s r: −0.34, p = 0.1). h, False positive rates across all sessions were negatively correlated with dF/F when aligned to behavioral response (N = 2 rats, n = 23 sessions, Pearson’s r: −0.45, p = 0.03). i, Tone-aligned LC activity, response-aligned LC activity, and d’ across behavioral sessions in an example animal. Black, d’. Green, dF/F responses either tone-aligned (filled symbols, solid lines) or behavioral response-aligned (open symbols, dashed lines). j, d’ across all sessions correlated with tone-aligned normalized LC dF/F (N = 2 rats, n = 23 behavioral sessions, Pearson’s r: 0.52, p = 0.01). k, d’ across all sessions was not correlated with dF/F when aligned to behavioral response (N = 2 rats, n = 23 behavioral sessions, Pearson’s r: −0.11, p = 0.6)

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 8 LC targeting and behavioral comparison between sham-paired YFP-injected animals vs fiber-mistargeted animals.

a, Surgical approach for targeting LC. Multi-unit recordings were conducted to locate LC and then viral injection and optic fiber placement were based on these coordinates. b, Example LC multi-unit activity evoked by toe pinch. c, Optical fiber placement based on histology and μ-CT/ μ-MRI co-registration. Top, fiber placement in LC-paired animals. Bottom, fiber placement in sham-paired animals (red, mis-targeted fibers outside of LC; black, YFP-injected controls). Scale bar, 1 mm. d, In fiber mis-targeted animals, there was no significant correlation between distance of probe tip for optical stimulation and days to d’ ≥ 1.0 (N = 11 rats, Pearson’s r: 0.17, p = 0.63) e, In fiber mis-targeted animals with at least six days of CI training, there was no significant correlation between distance of probe tip for optical stimulation and maximum performance with CI (N = 10 rats, Pearson’s r: −0.02, p = 0.95). f, Days to d’ ≥ 1.0 was similar between the two sub-groups of sham-paired animals with either YFP-only expression in LC or when fiber was mis-targeted outside LC (YFP: N = 5 rats, mis-targeted: N = 11 rats, p = 0.38, unpaired two-tailed Mann–Whitney test). Error bars, median±interquartile range. g, Sham-paired animals in each subgroup with at least six days of CI training had similar maximum d’ (YFP: N = 4 rats vs mis-targeted: N = 10 rats, p = 0.71, unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-test). h, CI performance (d’) over time in YFP: N = 5 rats vs mis-targeted: N = 11 rats. One YFP animal in f,h and one mis-targeted animal shown in d,f,h did not reach the six-day requirement for maximum performance analysis; these animals are not displayed in e,g. i, Hit rates over time in YFP: N = 5 rats vs mis-targeted: N = 11 rats. j, False positives over time in YFP: N = 5 rats vs mis-targeted: N = 11 rats. Data are mean±s.e.m. except in f. One YFP rat and one mis-targeted rat did not reach the six day performance requirement to calculate maximum d’. This mis-targeted animal is displayed in d, but excluded from e, and both are displayed in f, but excluded from g

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 9 LC-paired vs sham-paired animals had comparable CI insertions, impedances, ECAPs, behavioral initiation rates, and lack of residual hearing.

a, Number of intracochlear electrodes as assessed by x-ray was similar between LC-paired rats and sham-paired rats (LC-paired, N = 8 rats vs sham-paired, N = 16 rats, p = 0.93, unpaired two-tailed Mann–Whitney test). Blue, LC-paired animals. Black, sham-paired animals. Error bars, median±interquartile range. b, Degree of insertion did not predict performance across sham-paired (black) and LC-paired (blue) rats (full insertion: 8-channels, N = 14 rats vs partial insertion:4–7 channels, N = 10 rats, p = 0.36, unpaired two-tailed Mann–Whitney test). Measure of center, median. C, No significant correlation between estimated frequency mismatch and learning rate in both sham-paired (black, N = 16 rats, Pearson’s r: 0.39, p = 0.1) and LC-paired (blue, N = 8 rats, Pearson’s r: −0.37, p = 0.4) animals. Two LC-paired animals self-explanted their CIs prior to x-ray assessment of insertion; these animals are not displayed in a,b,c. d, Average impedances of CI channels over time in LC-paired (blue, N = 10) and sham-paired rats (black, N = 16). Dashed lines, individual rats. Solid lines, mean±s.e.m. e, Initial and final impedance values were similar in LC-paired and sham-paired rats (LC-paired: N = 10 rats vs sham-paired: N = 16 rats, initial: p = 0.32; final: p = 0.27; unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-test). f, ECAP thresholds during stage one and stage two training did not differ between LC-paired and sham-paired rats (LC-paired: N = 10 rats vs sham-paired: N = 16 rats, stage one: p = 0.91; stage two: p = 0.64; unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-test). g, Initiation rates were similar between LC-paired and sham-paired rats (LC-paired: N = 10 rats vs sham-paired: N = 16 rats, p = 0.35; unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-test). h, Learning rates with CIs (days to d’ ≥ 1) were not significantly correlated with initiation rates across LC-paired (blue, N = 10 rats, Pearson’s r: −0.49, p = 0.2) or sham-paired rats (black, N = 16 rats, Pearson’s r: −0.23, p = 0.4). i, Behavioral response rates were comparable in sham-paired (N = 16 rats) vs LC-paired (N = 10 rats) animals for sessions when the CI was turned off. (Blue, LC-paired: N = 10 rats vs black, sham-paired: N = 16 rats, hit rate: p = 0.72; false positives: p = 0.61; unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-test). j, d’ values were ~0 for both sham-paired (black, N = 16 rats) and LC-paired (blue, N = 10 rats) animals when the CI was turned off (p = 0.84; unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-test). Animals with high hit rates in I tended to also have high false positive rates; similarly, animals with low hit rates tended to have low false positive rates. k-m, Relating performance before and after deafening on CI performance vs acoustic normal-hearing (NH) task for last NH day vs first CI day in LC-paired animals. No significant correlation between hit rates (N = 10 rats, Pearson’s r: −0.05, p = 0.9) (k), false positives (N = 10 rats, Pearson’s r: −0.26, p = 0.5) (l), or d’ (N = 10 rats, Pearson’s r: −0.27, p = 0.4) (m). Data are error bars, mean±s.e.m. except in a, b

Source data

Extended Data Fig. 10 Electrophysiological recordings from the auditory periphery and auditory cortex of implanted rats.

a, Example ECAPs in CN VIII from an LC-paired rat (left), a sham-paired rat (middle), and an untrained rat (right). b, Average ECAP amplitudes (P1-N1) were similar across groups and target/foil channels (LC-paired target ECAP amplitude: 120.0 ± 5.8 µV, LC-paired foil: 94.0 ± 5.2 µV, N = 4 rats; sham-paired target: 124.9 ± 19.6 µV, sham-paired foil: 111.9 ± 16.8 µV, N = 8 rats; untrained: 114.5 ± 3.9 µV, N = 4 rats). There was no significant difference between LC-paired, sham-paired, and untrained animal ECAPs (comparing LC-paired target vs foil, p = 0.27; sham-paired target vs foil, p = 0.55; LC-paired vs sham-paired target, p = 0.59; LC-paired vs sham-paired foil, p = 0.99; untrained vs LC-paired target, p = 0.99; untrained vs LC-paired foil, p = 0.61; untrained vs sham-paired target, p = 0.92; untrained vs sham-paired foil, p = 0.41; two-way ANOVA across all groups with Tukey’s multiple comparisons correction). c, Relative fraction of unresponsive multi-unit sites was greater in untrained vs trained rats (fraction of unresponsive sites in: untrained rat auditory cortex, N = 4 rats, 21.3 ± 10.1% vs trained rat auditory cortex, N = 12 rats 5.5 ± 2.1%, p = 0.03, Student’s unpaired two-tailed t-test). Total number of sites recorded from was comparable in untrained rats (18.5 ± 1.0 sites/animal, N = 4 rats), LC-paired trained rats (17.5 ± 2.8 sites/animal, N = 4 rats), and sham-paired trained rats (20.0 ± 2.4 sites/animal, N = 8 rats). d, Neural vs behavioral d’ values across animals as in Fig. 4h, but with neural d’ values computed using only sites tuned to the target channel (N = 12 rats, Pearson’s r: 0.36, p = 0.25). e, As d, but using only sites where foils were the best channel (N = 12 rats, Pearson’s r: 0.85, p = 0.0005)

Source data

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

This file contains the Supplementary Discussion, Supplementary Table 1 and Supplementary References.

Reporting Summary

Peer Review File

Supplementary Video 1

Example of a deaf rat using CI to respond to presentation of target tones but not foil tones.

Source data

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Glennon, E., Valtcheva, S., Zhu, A. et al. Locus coeruleus activity improves cochlear implant performance. Nature 613, 317–323 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-022-05554-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-022-05554-8

This article is cited by

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing