Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Modulation of inhibitory communication coordinates looking and reaching

Abstract

Looking and reaching are controlled by different brain regions and are coordinated during natural behaviour1. Understanding how flexible, natural behaviours such as coordinated looking and reaching are controlled depends on understanding how neurons in different regions of the brain communicate2. Neural coherence in a gamma-frequency (40–90 Hz) band has been implicated in excitatory multiregional communication3. Inhibitory control mechanisms are also required to flexibly control behaviour4, but little is known about how neurons in one region transiently suppress individual neurons in another to support behaviour. How neuronal firing in a sender region transiently suppresses firing in a receiver region remains poorly understood. Here we study inhibitory communication during a flexible, natural behaviour, termed gaze anchoring, in which saccades are transiently inhibited by coordinated reaches. During gaze anchoring, we found that neurons in the reach region of the posterior parietal cortex can inhibit neuronal firing in the parietal saccade region to suppress eye movements and improve reach accuracy. Suppression is transient, only present around the coordinated reach, and greatest when reach neurons fire spikes with respect to beta-frequency (15–25 Hz) activity, not gamma-frequency activity. Our work provides evidence in the activity of single neurons for a novel mechanism of inhibitory communication in which beta-frequency neural coherence transiently inhibits multiregional communication to flexibly coordinate natural behaviour.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Coordinated behaviour and multiregional communication.
Fig. 2: Behavioural performance and coherent neural dynamics.
Fig. 3: State-dependent inhibitory communication.
Fig. 4: Channel modulation predicts LIP firing rates.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

Data for the current study are available on a GitHub repository: https://bitbucket.org/pesaranlab/doublestep/.

Code availability

The MATLAB code for current study is available on a GitHub repository: https://bitbucket.org/pesaranlab/doublestep/.

References

  1. Milner, D. & Goodale, M. The Visual Brain in Action (Oxford Univ. Press, 2006).

  2. Hahn, G., Ponce-Alvarez, A., Deco, G., Aertsen, A. & Kumar, A. Portraits of communication in neuronal networks. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 20, 117–127 (2019).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Buschman, T. J. & Miller, E. K. Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science 315, 1860–1862 (2007).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Anderson, M. C. & Weaver, C. In Encyclopedia of Neuroscience (ed. Squire, L. R.) 153–163 (Academic Press, 2009).

  5. Semedo, J. D., Zandvakili, A., Machens, C. K., Yu, B. M. & Kohn, A. Cortical areas interact through a communication subspace. Neuron 102, 249–259.e4 (2019).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Gregoriou, G. G., Gotts, S. J., Zhou, H. & Desimone, R. High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science 324, 1207–1210 (2009).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Pesaran, B., Nelson, M. J. & Andersen, R. A. Free choice activates a decision circuit between frontal and parietal cortex. Nature 453, 406–409 (2008).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Abrams, R. A., Meyer, D. E. & Kornblum, S. Eye-hand coordination: oculomotor control in rapid aimed limb movements. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 16, 248–267 (1990).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Catani, M. et al. Short parietal lobe connections of the human and monkey brain. Cortex 97, 339–357 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Lewis, J. W. & van Essen, D. C. Corticocortical connections of visual, sensorimotor, and multimodal processing areas in the parietal lobe of the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 428, 112–137 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Kubanek, J., Li, J. M. & Snyder, L. H. Motor role of parietal cortex in a monkey model of hemispatial neglect. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, E2067–E2072 (2015).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Hwang, E. J., Hauschild, M., Wilke, M. & Andersen, R. A. Inactivation of the parietal reach region causes optic ataxia, impairing reaches but not saccades. Neuron 76, 1021–1029 (2012).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Neggers, S. F. & Bekkering, H. Ocular gaze is anchored to the target of an ongoing pointing movement. J. Neurophysiol. 83, 639–651 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Hagan, M. A., Dean, H. L. & Pesaran, B. Spike-field activity in parietal area LIP during coordinated reach and saccade movements. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 1275–1290 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Barash, S., Bracewell, R. M., Fogassi, L., Gnadt, J. W. & Andersen, R. A. Saccade-related activity in the lateral intraparietal area. II. Spatial properties. J. Neurophysiol. 66, 1109–1124 (1991).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Pesaran, B. et al. Investigating large-scale brain dynamics using field potential recordings: analysis and interpretation. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 903–919 (2018).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Dean, H. L., Hagan, M. A. & Pesaran, B. Only coherent spiking in posterior parietal cortex coordinates looking and reaching. Neuron 73, 829–841 (2012).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Pape, A.-A. & Siegel, M. Motor cortex activity predicts response alternation during sensorimotor decisions. Nat. Commun. 7, 13098 (2016).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Kristeva, R., Patino, L. & Omlor, W. Beta-range cortical motor spectral power and corticomuscular coherence as a mechanism for effective corticospinal interaction during steady-state motor output. NeuroImage 36, 785–792 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Chung, J. W., Ofori, E., Misra, G., Hess, C. W. & Vaillancourt, D. E. Beta-band activity and connectivity in sensorimotor and parietal cortex are important for accurate motor performance. NeuroImage 144, 164–173 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Brovelli, A. et al. Beta oscillations in a large-scale sensorimotor cortical network: directional influences revealed by Granger causality. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101, 9849–9854 (2004).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Lee, J. H., Whittington, M. A. & Kopell, N. J. Top-down beta rhythms support selective attention via interlaminar interaction: a model. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9, e1003164 (2013).

    Article  ADS  MathSciNet  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Tan, H., Wade, C. & Brown, P. Post-movement beta activity in sensorimotor cortex indexes confidence in the estimations from internal models. J. Neurosci. 36, 1516–1528 (2016).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Hawellek, D. J., Wong, Y. T. & Pesaran, B. Temporal coding of reward-guided choice in the posterior parietal cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 13492–13497 (2016).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Fiebelkorn, I. C., Pinsk, M. A. & Kastner, S. A dynamic interplay within the frontoparietal network underlies rhythmic spatial attention. Neuron 99, 842–853.e8 (2018).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Wong, Y. T., Fabiszak, M. M., Novikov, Y., Daw, N. D. & Pesaran, B. Coherent neuronal ensembles are rapidly recruited when making a look-reach decision. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 327–334 (2016).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Pesaran, B., Hagan, M., Qiao, S. & Shewcraft, R. Multiregional communication and the channel modulation hypothesis. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 66, 250–257 (2020).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Krakauer, J. W., Ghazanfar, A. A., Gomez-Marin, A., MacIver, M. A. & Poeppel, D. Neuroscience needs behavior: correcting a reductionist bias. Neuron 93, 480–490 (2017).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Bastos, A. M. et al. Visual areas exert feedforward and feedback influences through distinct frequency channels. Neuron 85, 390–401 (2015).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Jensen, O. et al. On the human sensorimotor-cortex beta rhythm: sources and modeling. NeuroImage 26, 347–355 (2005).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Yamawaki, N., Stanford, I. M., Hall, S. D. & Woodhall, G. L. Pharmacologically induced and stimulus evoked rhythmic neuronal oscillatory activity in the primary motor cortex in vitro. Neuroscience 151, 386–395 (2008).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Hall, S. D. et al. The role of GABAergic modulation in motor function related neuronal network activity. NeuroImage 56, 1506–1510 (2011).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Baumgarten, T. J. et al. Beta peak frequencies at rest correlate with endogenous GABA /Cr concentrations in sensorimotor cortex areas. PLoS ONE 11, e0156829 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Pouille, F. & Scanziani, M. Enforcement of temporal fidelity in pyramidal cells by somatic feed-forward inhibition. Science 293, 1159–1163 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Rogal, L. & Fischer, B. Eye-hand-coordination: a model for computing reaction times in a visually guided reach task. Biol. Cybern. 55, 263–273 (1986).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Hoff, B. & Arbib, M. A. Models of trajectory formation and temporal interaction of reach and grasp. J. Mot. Behav. 25, 175–192 (1993).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Wimmer, R. D. et al. Thalamic control of sensory selection in divided attention. Nature 526, 705–709 (2015).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Watanabe, K. & Funahashi, S. Neural mechanisms of dual-task interference and cognitive capacity limitation in the prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 601–611 (2014).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Mitra, P. P. & Pesaran, B. Analysis of dynamic brain imaging data. Biophys. J. 76, 691–708 (1999).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Berens, P. CircStat: a MATLAB toolbox for circular statistics. J. Stat. Softw. 31, 1–21 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank R. Comeau, S. Frey and B. Hynes for custom modifications to the BrainSight system, and N. Price, E. Zavitz, A. Charles and members of the Pesaran laboratory for helpful feedback. This work was supported, in part, by NIH T32 EY007136 (M.A.H.), ARC DE180100344 (M.A.H.), NHMRC APP1185442 (M.A.H.), NSF CAREER Award BCS-0955701 (B.P.), NEI R01-EY024067 (B.P.), the Army Research Office (B.P.), the Simons Foundation (B.P.), McKnight Scholar Award (B.P.), Sloan Research Fellowship (B.P.) and NIH R01-NS104923 (B.P.).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

M.A.H. and B.P. conceptualized the project and designed the experiments. M.A.H. performed the experiments. M.A.H. and B.P. analysed the data and wrote the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bijan Pesaran.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature thanks Martin Giese, Nicholas Hatsopoulos and Lawrence Snyder for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data figures and tables

Extended Data Fig. 1 Coordinated and independent movement tasks.

a, Reach and saccade double-step task (RS) and Saccade double-step task (SS), indicating hand (H) and eye (E) position at each epoch. Dashed lines indicate period of gaze-anchoring in the RS task, and temporally matched epochs in the SS task. The slowing of the second saccade reaction time (SSRT) was tied to the coordinated movement. b, The coordination of the first movement was established by the strong correlation between the reaction times for the reach and saccade in the first movement (Monkey 1 (M1): R = 0.34, slope = 0.21 ms/ms, p = 3 x 10−139, Monkey 2 (M2): R = 0.45, slope = 0.25 ms/ms, p = 0, Pearson pairwise linear correlation). c, SSRT correlated with the difference between the reaction times of the reach and saccade in the first movement (M1: R = 0.20, slope = 0.26 ms/ms, p = 2 x 10−49, M2: R = 0.22, slope = 0.39 ms/ms, p = 2 x 10−78, Pearson pairwise linear correlation). d, Specifically, SSRT correlated with the reaction time of the reach (M1: R = 0.23, slope = 0.29 ms/ms, p = 4 x 10−63, M2: R = 0.20, slope = 0.32 ms/ms, p = 2 x 10−65, Pearson pairwise linear correlation). e, The SSRT was not dependent on the reaction time of the saccade in the RS task (M1: R = 0.05, slope = 0.09 ms/ms, p = 8 x 10−4, M2: R = 0.006, slope = 0.02 ms/ms, p = 0.63, Pearson pairwise linear correlation). f, SSRT did not depend on the duration of the reach (M1: R = 0.07, slope = 0.08 ms/ms, p = 1 x 10−6, M2: R = 0.001, slope = 0.002 ms/ms, p = 0.91, Pearson pairwise linear correlation). g, SSRT only weakly correlated with the SRT in the SS task (M1: R = 0.06, slope = 0.08 ms/ms, p = 1 x 10−3, M2: R = 0.12, slope = 0.17 ms/ms, p = 7 x 10−13, Pearson pairwise linear correlation). Therefore, the slowing of the SSRT was tied to coordinated movement, and primarily the timing of the reach. Monkey 1: RS task, n = 10,324 trials, SS task, n = 8,372 trials; Monkey 2: RS task, n = 12,840 trials, SS task n = 8,452 trials. All error bars show mean ± SEM.

Extended Data Fig. 2 Anatomical locations of recordings.

Recording chambers were placed over the posterior parietal cortex of the hemisphere contralateral to the reaching arm. a, b, Whole brain MRI reconstructions and example coronal slice from Monkey 1 (a) and Monkey 2 (b). Chamber placement and electrode recording sites were registered to the structural MRI (BrainSight, Rogue Research). Recording regions for area LIP (blue) and PRR (black) are indicated by the shaded regions. Dashed lines indicate the plane of example coronal sections shown. Key sulcal landmarks, intraparietal sulcus (ips), lunate sulcus (lu) and superior temporal sulcus (sts), are also indicated.

Extended Data Fig. 3 Phase of spike-β-LFP coherence in each cortical area.

ac, Peri-reach (a), post-reach (b), and saccade (c) trials showing spike-LFP coherence between PRR spiking and PRR LFP phase (y-axis) and LIP LFP phase (x-axis) in the beta-band (\(\beta ,\) 20 Hz, colorscale: proportion of trials). Marginals show the proportion of trials as a function of phase in each area. Peri-reach: PRR \(\beta \)-LFP p = 2 x 10−49, mean phase = 136 ± 75°; LIP \(\beta \)-LFP p = 8 x 10−5, mean phase = 172 ± 79°, Post-reach: PRR \(\beta \)-LFP p = 4 x 10−57, mean phase = 149 ± 74°, LIP \(\beta \)-LFP p = 0.02, mean phase = 164 ± 79°, Saccade trials: PRR \(\beta \)-LFP p = 2 x 10−52, mean phase = 135 ± 73°, LIP \(\beta \)-LFP p = 2 x 10−52, mean phase = 59 ± 78°, Rayleigh’s test of non-uniformity, circular mean ± SD phase). Black triangles indicate mean phase, stars indicate that the distribution is non-uniform (one star, p < 0.05; two stars, p < 0.01, exact p-values above). Peri-reach: n = 4814 trials, Post-reach: n = 2116 trials, Saccade: n = 3583 trials (same dataset shown in Figs. 2, 3). All error bars show mean ± SEM.

Extended Data Fig. 4 Negative-log-likelihood and generalization errors for model fits.

Second saccade reaction time: a, Dual-phase negative-log-likelihood (NLL) = 25124; PRR-only phase NLL = 25158; LIP-only phase NLL = 25157; Null NLL = 25162; ∆NLLDual-PRR = 35; ∆NLLDual-LIP = 33, AIC test. b, Expected generalization error: Dual-coherent: 10.6 ms, R = 0.10; PRR-only: 12.5 ms, R = −0.05; LIP-only: 11.8 ms, R = 0.01; Null: 11.8 ms, where R = 1-(SSE_model/SSE_null). Reach accuracy: c, Dual-phase negative-log-likelihood (NLL)= 6462; PRR-only phase NLL = 6507; LIP-only phase NLL = 6511; Null NLL = 6512; ∆NLLDual-PRR = 45; ∆NLLDual-LIP = 49, AIC test. d, Expected generalization error: Dual-coherent: 0.18 deg, R = 0.20, PRR-only: 0.27 deg, R = −0.14, LIP-only: 0.24 deg, R = −0.02, Null: 0.24 deg). Non-parametric analyses: Peri-reach trials: SSRT: dual-coherent: resultant = 6.2 x 10−3, p < 10−6, LIP-only: p = 0.28, PRR-only: resultant = 2.3 x 10−6, p = 0.02, RA: dual-coherent: resultant = 1.6 x 10−4, p = 0. LIP-only: p = 0.33. PRR-only: resultant = 5.3 x 10−5,p = 1.6 x 10−3. Post-reach trials for dual coherent phase: SSRT: resultant = 5.7 x 10−3,p = 0. post-reach RA: resultant = 2.1 x 10−4, p = 1.2 x 10−3. Saccade trials: SSRT: p = 0.89. Peri-reach: n = 4814 trials, Post-reach: n = 2116 trials, Saccade: n = 3583 trials (same dataset shown in Figs. 2, 3).

Extended Data Fig. 5 Dual coherent β-LFP phase aligned to reach onset.

a, Second saccade reaction time (SSRT) against dual-coherent \(\beta \)-LFP phase for each RS task trial type (Peri-reach: light green. Post-reach: dark green) during the gaze anchoring epoch when aligned to reach onset, instead of second target onset. Solid lines present changes in SSRT fitted by von Mises function (peri-reach: p = 0, preferred phase = 41°. post-reach: p = 6 x 10−5, preferred phase = 153°). Downward triangle presents the mean of the von-Mises fit dual-coherent \(\beta \)-LFP phase at maximum SSRT on peri-reach trials. b, c, Phase of spike-\(\beta \)-LFP coherence in each cortical area (PRR \(\beta \)-LFP coherence, y axis; LIP \(\beta \)-LFP coherence, x axis) and influence on SSRT (colorscale). Marginals show SSRT against \(\beta \)-LFP phase coherence in each area alone (Peri-reach: PRR-only p = 0.53, LIP-only p = 2 x 10−3, preferred phase = 48°. Post-reach: PRR-only p = 0.53, LIP-only p = 0.48) . Dashed lines (b) indicate the corresponding dual-coherent phase shown by the downward triangle in (a). Note that confounds due to the onset of the Go cue were not a concern because the LFP phase calculation during gaze-anchoring epoch rarely overlapped cue delivery (3.7% of peri-reach trials, 0% of post-reach trials and 2.5% of saccade trials). All p-values report likelihood-ratio test. Peri-reach: n = 4814 trials, Post-reach: n = 2116 trials, Saccade: n = 3583 trials (same dataset shown in Figs. 2, 3). All error bars show mean ± SEM.

Extended Data Fig. 6 β-LFP phase difference alone did not predict gaze anchoring.

The circular mean \(\beta \)-LFP phase was taken across the gaze-anchoring epoch, irrespective of spike timing. a, Mean phase difference of the \(\beta \)-LFP (20 Hz) across cortical areas for each task trial type (Saccade: pink. Peri-reach: light green. Post-reach: dark green). Solid lines present changes in SSRT fitted by von Mises function (Peri-reach: p = 0.23. Post-reach: p = 1 x 10−3, preferred phase = 53°. Saccade: p = 0.83. likelihood-ratio test). bd, Mean \(\beta \)-LFP phase in each cortical area (PRR \(\beta \)-LFP phase, y axis; LIP \(\beta \)-LFP phase x axis) and influence on SSRT (colorscale). Marginals show SSRT as a function of mean \(\beta \)-LFP phase in each area alone (Peri-reach: PRR-only p = 0.89, LIP-only p = 0.24. Post-reach: PRR-only p = 8 x 10−3, preferred phase = 32° LIP-only p = 0.20. Saccade: PRR-only p = 8 x 10−4, preferred phase = −143; LIP-only p = 0.12. likelihood-ratio test). eg, \(\beta \)-LFP phase in PRR (y-axis) and LIP (x-axis, colorscale: proportion of trials). Downward triangles show the circular mean phase. Peri-reach: n = 4814 trials, Post-reach: n = 2116 trials, Saccade: n = 3583 trials (same dataset shown in Figs. 2, 3). All error bars show mean ± SEM.

Extended Data Fig. 7 Spike timing dependence of dual coherent β-LFP phase.

al, For each trial, spike times were jittered according to a Gaussian distribution with standard deviation 2 ms (ac), 5 ms (df), 10 ms (gi) and 20 ms (jl). Second saccade reaction time (SSRT) as a function of dual-coherent \(\beta \)-LFP phase for each task trial type (Saccade, pink; Peri-reach, light green; Post-reach, dark green) during the gaze anchoring epoch was recomputed with the jittered spike times. Solid lines present changes in SSRT fitted by von Mises function (a, Peri-reach: p = 1 x 10−13, preferred phase = 82°. Post-reach: p = 5 x 10−5, preferred phase = 126°. Saccade: p = 0.68. d, Peri-reach: p = 3 x 10−3, preferred phase = 73°. Post-reach: p = 7 x 10−5, preferred phase = 108°. Saccade: p = 0.23. g, Peri-reach: p = 0.22. Post-reach: p = 0.29. Saccade: p = 0.90. j, Peri-reach: p = 0.11. Post-reach: p = 1. Saccade: p = 0.38. likelihood-ratio test). For peri-reach trials, the phase of spike-\(\beta \)-LFP coherence in each cortical area were computed for the jittered spike times (PRR \(\beta \)-LFP coherence, y axis; LIP \(\beta \)-LFP coherence x axis) and influence on SSRT (colorscale). Marginals show SSRT as a function of \(\beta \)-LFP phase coherence in each area alone (b, PRR-only p = 0.10, LIP-only, p = 0.10, e, PRR-only p = 0.04, preferred phase = −127°, LIP-only, p = 0.47, h, PRR-only p = 0.68, LIP-only, p = 0.63, k, PRR-only p = 0.52, LIP-only, p = 0.71, likelihood-ratio test). Spike-LFP coherence between PRR spiking and each cortical area alone for jittered spike times on peri-reach trials (PRR LFP phase, y-axis, LIP LFP phase x-axis, colorscale: proportion of trials). Marginals show the proportion of trials as a function of phase in each area (c, PRR p = 2 x 10−47, mean = 137°, LIP p = 8 x 10−4, mean = 175°, f, PRR p = 2 x 10−31, mean = 139°, LIP p = 1 x 10−3, mean = 167°, i, PRR p = 3 x 10−10, mean = 132°, LIP p = 0.04, mean = −140°, l, PRR p = 0.84, LIP p = 0.79, Rayleigh’s test of non-uniformity, circular mean phase). Black triangles indicate mean phase, stars indicate that the distribution is non-uniform (one star, p < 0.05; two stars, p < 0.01, exact p-values above). Peri-reach: n = 4814 trials, Post-reach: n = 2116 trials, Saccade: n = 3583 trials (same dataset shown in Figs. 2, 3). All error bars show mean ± SEM.

Extended Data Fig. 8 Dual coherent γ-LFP phase.

Dual-coherent phase was calculated between PRR spiking and the \(\gamma \)-LFP phase (40 Hz) in each cortical area. a, Second saccade reaction time (SSRT) against dual-coherent \(\gamma \)-LFP phase for each RS task trial type (Peri-reach = light green; Post-reach = dark green). Solid lines present changes in SSRT fitted by von Mises function (Peri-reach: p = 8 x 10−4, preferred phase = −6°. Post-reach: p = 0.04, preferred phase = 26°. Saccade: p = 0.1likelihood-ratio test). bd, Mean \(\gamma \)-LFP phase in each cortical area (PRR \(\gamma \)-LFP phase, y axis; LIP \(\gamma \)-LFP phase x axis) and their influence on SSRT (colorscale). Marginals show SSRT against mean \(\gamma \)-LFP phase in each area alone (Peri-reach: PRR-only p = 0.55, LIP-only p = 0.02, preferred phase = 112°. Post-reach: PRR-only p = 0.02, preferred phase = −25°. LIP-only p = 4 x 10−6, preferred phase = 78°. Saccade: PRR-only p = 0.13, LIP-only p = 0.04, preferred phase = 17°, likelihood-ratio test). eg, \(\gamma \)-LFP phase in PRR (y-axis) and LIP (x-axis, colorscale: proportion of trials). Marginals show the proportion of trials against phase in each area (Peri-reach: PRR p = 4x10−23, mean =  −177°, LIP p = 0.02, mean = −125°. Post-reach: PRR p = 2 x 10−6, mean = 148°, LIP p = 3 x 10−4, mean = −159°. Saccade: PRR p = 1 x 10−4, mean = 153°, LIP p = 0.33, Rayleigh’s test of non-uniformity). Black triangles indicate mean phase, stars indicate that the distribution is non-uniform (one star, p < 0.05; two stars, p < 0.01, exact p-values above). Peri-reach SSRT slowing with gamma-frequency phase was significantly reduced compared to that with beta-frequency dual-coherent phase (resultant = 2.9 x 10−3, p = 1 x 10−4. permutation test). Peri-reach: n = 4814 trials, Post-reach: n = 2116 trials, Saccade: n = 3583 trials (same dataset shown in Figs. 2, 3). All error bars show mean ± SEM.

Extended Data Fig. 9 LIP-spike dual coherent β-LFP phase.

Dual-coherent phase was calculated between LIP spiking and the \(\beta \)-LFP phase (20 Hz) in each cortical area. a, Second saccade reaction time (SSRT) as a function of LIP dual-coherent \(\beta \)-LFP phase for each RS task trial type (peri-reach trials, light green; post-reach trials, dark green) and SS task trials (saccade trials). Solid lines present changes in SSRT fitted by von Mises function (peri-reach: p = 1 x 10−4, preferred phase = −122°. post-reach: p = 0.21. saccade: p = 1 x 10−3, preferred phase = −157°. likelihood-ratio test). SSRT varied with PRR-spike dual-coherent phase significantly more than with LIP-spike dual-coherent phase (resultant = 1.2 x 10−3, p < 10−6, permutation test). bd, Mean LIP-spike \(\beta \)-LFP phase in each cortical area (PRR \(\beta \)-LFP phase, y axis; LIP \(\beta \)-LFP phase x axis) and their influence on SSRT (colorscale). Marginals show SSRT as a function of mean LIP-spike \(\beta \)-LFP phase in each area alone (Peri-reach: PRR-only p = 0.23. LIP-only p = 0.49. Post-reach: PRR-only p = 6 x 10−4, preferred phase = 109°. LIP-only p = 0.48. Saccade: PRR-only p = 5 x 10−5 preferred phase = −25°. LIP-only p = 0, preferred phase = −142°, likelihood-ratio test). eg, LIP-spike \(\beta \)-LFP phase in PRR (y-axis) and LIP (x-axis, colorscale: proportion of trials). Marginals show the proportion of trials as a function of phase in each area (Peri-reach: PRR: p = 2 x 10−5, mean =  160°, LIP: p = 1 x 10−7, mean = 97°. Post-reach: PRR p = 2 x 10−5, mean = 11°, LIP p = 5 x 10−4, mean = 67°. Saccade: PRR p = 2 x 10−5, mean = 100°, LIP p = 4 x 10−12, mean = 103°, Rayleigh’s test of non-uniformity, circular mean). Black triangles indicate mean phase, stars indicate that the distribution is non-uniform (one star, p < 0.05; two stars, p < 0.01, exact p-values above). Peri-reach: n = 7782 trials, Post-reach: n = 3604 trials, Saccade: n = 8603 trials. All error bars show mean ± SEM.

Extended Data Fig. 10 Dual-coherent β-LFP phase is weakly correlated with PRR firing rate.

ac, PRR firing rate and a function of dual-coherent \(\beta \)-LFP phase for each peri-reach (a), post-reach (b), and saccade (c) trials. Solid lines present changes in SSRT fitted by von Mises function (Peri-reach: p = 0, preferred phase = −121°. Post-reach: p = 0.23. Saccade: p = 0, preferred phase = 19°, likelihood-ratio test). Peri-reach: n = 4814 trials, Post-reach: n = 2116 trials, Saccade: n = 3583 trials (same dataset shown in Figs. 2, 3). All error bars show mean ± SEM.

Supplementary information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hagan, M.A., Pesaran, B. Modulation of inhibitory communication coordinates looking and reaching. Nature 604, 708–713 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-022-04631-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-022-04631-2

This article is cited by

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing