Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Neural control of affiliative touch in prosocial interaction

Abstract

The ability to help and care for others fosters social cohesiveness and is vital to the physical and emotional well-being of social species, including humans1,2,3. Affiliative social touch, such as allogrooming (grooming behaviour directed towards another individual), is a major type of prosocial behaviour that provides comfort to others1,2,3,4,5,6. Affiliative touch serves to establish and strengthen social bonds between animals and can help to console distressed conspecifics. However, the neural circuits that promote prosocial affiliative touch have remained unclear. Here we show that mice exhibit affiliative allogrooming behaviour towards distressed partners, providing a consoling effect. The increase in allogrooming occurs in response to different types of stressors and can be elicited by olfactory cues from distressed individuals. Using microendoscopic calcium imaging, we find that neural activity in the medial amygdala (MeA) responds differentially to naive and distressed conspecifics and encodes allogrooming behaviour. Through intersectional functional manipulations, we establish a direct causal role of the MeA in controlling affiliative allogrooming and identify a select, tachykinin-expressing subpopulation of MeA GABAergic (γ-aminobutyric-acid-expressing) neurons that promote this behaviour through their projections to the medial preoptic area. Together, our study demonstrates that mice display prosocial comforting behaviour and reveals a neural circuit mechanism that underlies the encoding and control of affiliative touch during prosocial interactions.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Mice display prosocial comforting behaviour.
Fig. 2: MeA neural dynamics during prosocial interaction.
Fig. 3: MeATac1 neurons underlie allogrooming.
Fig. 4: MPOA-projecting MeATac1∩Vgat neurons drive allogrooming.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

Additional data relating to the paper are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. Source data are provided with this paper.

Code availability

Code for the behavioural analysis (https://github.com/pdollar/toolbox), animal pose tracking (https://github.com/murthylab/sleap/releases/tag/v1.0.9), microendoscopic imaging data analysis (https://github.com/etterguillaume/MiniscopeAnalysis, https://github.com/zhoupc/CNMF_E and https://github.com/flatironinstitute/NoRMCorre) is available at GitHub. The pretrained Google Inception v3 network is available online (https://download.tensorflow.org/models/image/imagenet/inception-2015-12-05.tgz). Additional code relating to the paper is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. de Waal, F. B. M. & Preston, S. D. Mammalian empathy: behavioural manifestations and neural basis. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 498–509 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Dunfield, K. A. A construct divided: prosocial behavior as helping, sharing, and comforting subtypes. Front. Psychol 5, 958 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Rault, J.-L. Be kind to others: prosocial behaviours and their implications for animal welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 210, 113–123 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Morrison, I. Keep calm and cuddle on: social touch as a stress buffer. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 2, 344–362 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Spruijt, B. M., Hooff, J. A. V. & Gispen, W. H. Ethology and neurobiology of grooming behavior. Physiol. Rev. 72, 825–852 (1992).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Jablonski, N. G. Social and affective touch in primates and its role in the evolution of social cohesion. Neuroscience 464, 117–125 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Burkett, J. P. et al. Oxytocin-dependent consolation behavior in rodents. Science 351, 375–378 (2016).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Li, L.-F. et al. Involvement of oxytocin and GABA in consolation behavior elicited by socially defeated individuals in mandarin voles. Psychoneuroendocrinology 103, 14–24 (2019).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Sterley, T.-L. & Bains, J. S. Social communication of affective states. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 68, 44–51 (2021).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Paradiso, E., Gazzola, V. & Keysers, C. Neural mechanisms necessary for empathy-related phenomena across species. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 68, 107–115 (2021).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Kwon, J.-T. et al. An amygdala circuit that suppresses social engagement. Nature 593, 114–118 (2021).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Chen, P. & Hong, W. Neural circuit mechanisms of social behavior. Neuron 98, 16–30 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Raam, T. & Hong, W. Organization of neural circuits underlying social behavior: a consideration of the medial amygdala. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 68, 124–136 (2021).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Unger, E. K. et al. Medial amygdalar aromatase neurons regulate aggression in both sexes. Cell Rep. 10, 453–462 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Li, Y. et al. Neuronal representation of social information in the medial amygdala of awake behaving mice. Cell 171, 1176–1190 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Twining, R. C., Vantrease, J. E., Love, S., Padival, M. & Rosenkranz, J. A. An intra-amygdala circuit specifically regulates social fear learning. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 459–469 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Hong, W., Kim, D.-W. & Anderson, D. J. Antagonistic control of social versus repetitive self-grooming behaviors by separable amygdala neuronal subsets. Cell 158, 1348–1361 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Chen, P. B. et al. Sexually dimorphic control of parenting behavior by the medial amygdala. Cell 176, 1206–1221 (2019).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Allsop, S. A. et al. Corticoamygdala transfer of socially derived information gates observational learning. Cell 173, 1329–1342 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Jeon, D. et al. Observational fear learning involves affective pain system and Cav1.2 Ca2+ channels in ACC. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 482–488 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Sterley, T.-L. et al. Social transmission and buffering of synaptic changes after stress. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 393–403 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Gangopadhyay, P., Chawla, M., Monte, O. D. & Chang, S. W. C. Prefrontal–amygdala circuits in social decision-making. Nat. Neurosci. 24, 5–18 (2021).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Canteras, N. S., Simerly, R. B. & Swanson, L. W. Organization of projections from the medial nucleus of the amygdala: a PHAL study in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 360, 213–245 (1995).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Cádiz-Moretti, B., Otero-García, M., Martínez-García, F. & Lanuza, E. Afferent projections to the different medial amygdala subdivisions: a retrograde tracing study in the mouse. Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 1033–1065 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Mahn, M. et al. High-efficiency optogenetic silencing with soma-targeted anion-conducting channelrhodopsins. Nat. Commun. 9, 4125 (2018).

    Article  ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Fenno, L. E. et al. Targeting cells with single vectors using multiple-feature Boolean logic. Nat. Methods 11, 763–772 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Chen, T.-W. et al. Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature 499, 295–300 (2013).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Dana, H. et al. High-performance calcium sensors for imaging activity in neuronal populations and microcompartments. Nat. Methods 16, 649–657 (2019).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Lim, B. K., Huang, K. W., Grueter, B. A., Rothwell, P. E. & Malenka, R. C. Anhedonia requires MC4R-mediated synaptic adaptations in nucleus accumbens. Nature 487, 183–189 (2012).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Ghosh, K. K. et al. Miniaturized integration of a fluorescence microscope. Nat. Methods 8, 871–878 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Kingsbury, L. et al. Correlated neural activity and encoding of behavior across brains of socially interacting animals. Cell 178, 429–446 (2019).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Pnevmatikakis, E. A. & Giovannucci, A. NoRMCorre: an online algorithm for piecewise rigid motion correction of calcium imaging data. J. Neurosci. Methods 291, 83–94 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Zhou, P. et al. Efficient and accurate extraction of in vivo calcium signals from microendoscopic video data. Elife 7, e28728 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Kingsbury, L. et al. Cortical representations of conspecific sex shape social behavior. Neuron 107, 941–953 (2020).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Hong, W. et al. Automated measurement of mouse social behaviors using depth sensing, video tracking, and machine learning. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, E5351–E5360 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Wiltschko, A. B. et al. Mapping sub-second structure in mouse behavior. Neuron 88, 1121–1135 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Kabra, M., Robie, A. A., Rivera-Alba, M., Branson, S. & Branson, K. JAABA: interactive machine learning for automatic annotation of animal behavior. Nat. Methods 10, 64–67 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Jhuang, H. et al. Automated home-cage behavioural phenotyping of mice. Nat. Commun. 1, 68 (2010).

    Article  ADS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Nilsson, S. R. et al. Simple Behavioral Analysis (SimBA)—an open source toolkit for computer classification of complex social behaviors in experimental animals. Preprint at bioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.04.19.049452 (2020).

  40. LeCun, Y., Bengio, Y. & Hinton, G. Deep learning. Nature 521, 436–444 (2015).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Szegedy, C., Vanhoucke, V., Ioffe, S., Shlens, J. & Wojna, Z. Rethinking the inception architecture for computer vision. Preprint at https://arxiv.org/abs/1512.00567v3 (2015).

  42. Pereira, T. D. et al. SLEAP: multi-animal pose tracking. Preprint at bioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.08.31.276246 (2020).

  43. Hochreiter, S. & Schmidhuber, J. Long short-term memory. Neural Comput. 9, 1735–1780 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Lipton, Z. C., Berkowitz, J. & Elkan, C. A critical review of recurrent neural networks for sequence learning. Preprint at https://arxiv.org/abs/1506.00019 (2015).

  45. Wu, Y. E., Pan, L., Zuo, Y., Li, X. & Hong, W. Detecting activated cell populations using single-cell RNA-seq. Neuron 96, 313–329 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Harris, J. A. et al. Anatomical characterization of Cre driver mice for neural circuit mapping and manipulation. Front. Neural Circuits 8, 76 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Taniguchi, H. et al. A resource of Cre driver lines for genetic targeting of GABAergic neurons in cerebral cortex. Neuron 71, 995–1013 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Daigle, T. L. et al. A suite of transgenic driver and reporter mouse lines with enhanced brain-cell-type targeting and functionality. Cell 174, 465–480 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Hu, R. K. et al. An amygdala-to-hypothalamus circuit for social reward. Nat. Neurosci. 24, 831–842 (2021).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Stuber, G. D. et al. Excitatory transmission from the amygdala to nucleus accumbens facilitates reward seeking. Nature 475, 377–380 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Bankhead, P. et al. QuPath: open source software for digital pathology image analysis. Sci. Rep. 7, 16878 (2017).

    Article  ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Fenno, L. E. et al. Comprehensive dual- and triple-feature intersectional single-vector delivery of diverse functional payloads to cells of behaving mammals. Neuron 107, 836–853 (2020).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank V. Wan and P. B. Chen for technical assistance and contributions to the project; D. Aharoni, P. Golshani and P. Zhao for assistance in setting up the miniscope (v4) imaging system; D. J. Anderson for comments and feedback on the project; and members of the Hong laboratory for valuable comments. Illustrations of mice and mouse brain schematics in Figs. 1a, u–x, 2a, 3a, e, k and 4d, h, l, p and Extended Data Figs. 7b, 8c, h, 9a and 10b were created using BioRender.com. This work was supported in part by NIH grants (R01 NS113124 and U01 NS122124), a Searle Scholars Award, a Packard Fellowship in Science and Engineering, a Keck Foundation Junior Faculty Award, a McKnight Scholar Award, a Vallee Scholar Award, a Mallinckrodt Scholar Award, a NARSAD Young Investigator grant, a Klingenstein-Simons Fellowship Award and a Brain Research Foundation grant (to W.H.) and NIH training grant (T32 NS048004, to J.D.).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Y.E.W. and W.H. designed the study. Y.E.W., J.D., M.Z., F.S. and R.K.H. performed experiments. Y.E.W., W.H., L.K., J.D., M.Z. and F.S. analysed data. Y.E.W. and W.H. wrote the manuscript with inputs from J.D. and L.K. W.H. supervised the entire study.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Ye Emily Wu or Weizhe Hong.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Peer review information Nature thanks Steve Chang and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data figures and tables

Extended Data Fig. 1 Characterization of prosocial allogrooming in female mice.

a, Example raster plots showing allogrooming and self-grooming behaviours exhibited by female subjects when they interact with unstressed (separation only, control) or stressed (foot-shocked) female partners. Each row represents an individual animal. b, Time-course of cumulative duration of allogrooming exhibited by subjects and partners after partners experience foot-shocks. Mean ± s.e.m. c, d, Total duration (c) and onset latency (d) of allogrooming exhibited by subjects toward unstressed (control) or foot-shocked partners during 13 min of interaction. While females exhibit elevated allogrooming toward stressed partners similar to males, the total duration of allogrooming toward stressed partners is shorter in females compared to males (Fig. 1d, Supplementary Note 1). e, f, Total number of allogrooming bouts (e) and average per-bout duration of allogrooming (f) in individual subjects interacting with unstressed (control) or foot-shocked partners. g, Total duration of self-grooming exhibited by subjects during interaction with unstressed (control) or foot-shocked partners. h, i, Total duration of allogrooming (h) and total number of social approaches (i) toward subjects exhibited by partners after separation only or foot-shocks. Boxplots: median with quartiles, 1.5 × IQR and outliers. b-i, n = 10 pairs of female mice. Two-sided Wilcoxon signed-rank test. **P < 0.01, *P < 0.05. ns, not significant. For details of statistical analyses, see Supplementary Table 1

Source data.

Extended Data Fig. 2 Behavioral analysis using convolutional and recurrent neural networks.

a, Schematic of behavior classification using recurrent neural network (RNN) based on CNN-derived spatial features, tracking-based features, and/or head orientation-based features. be, Performance of binary classifiers trained to discriminate between different pairs of behaviors using the CNN + RNN framework. f, Comparison of performance of four-way multi-class classifiers trained to discriminate between allogrooming, sniffing, self-grooming, or other behaviors. Different classifiers used CNN-derived spatial features, tracking-based features, head orientation-based features, or all three types of features to train the recurrent neural network. Data for the “CNN features” and shuffled control groups are the same as those in Fig. 1q and are presented here for comparison. g, PC projections of population vectors associated with different types of behavior bouts from one example microendoscopic imaging session. PCA is first performed using population activity during manually annotated behavior bouts (dots). Population activity during behavior bouts predicted using the CNN-RNN method (circles) was then projected onto this PC space. h, Mean pairwise Euclidean distances (in a space defined by PCs 1-4) between different pairs of behavior events that were either human annotated (“h.a.”) or predicted using the CNN-RNN method during each independent imaging session. Boxplots: median with quartiles, 1.5 × IQR and outliers. bf, n = 9 different partitions of training/validation/test datasets in each group. h, n = 7 independent imaging sessions in 6 subject mice. be, Two-sided Wilcoxon signed-rank test. f, One-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni’s multiple comparisons test. h, Friedman test followed by post hoc Dunn’s multiple comparisons test. ***P < 0.001, **P < 0.01, *P < 0.05. ns, not significant. For details of statistical analyses, see Supplementary Table 1

Source data.

Extended Data Fig. 3 MeA neuronal responses during prosocial interaction.

a, b, Example calcium traces from individual allogrooming-suppressed (a) and sniffing-suppressed (b) neurons during allogrooming or sniffing toward stressed conspecifics. c, d, Heatmaps showing average responses of example neurons with decreased activity during allogrooming (c) or sniffing (d). e, Cumulative distributions of pairwise distances between neurons of the same response type (allogrooming-responsive or sniffing-responsive) and distribution based on shuffled data in which the response type is randomly permuted (100 rounds of shuffling). f, Example calcium traces from single neurons that show increased activity during self-grooming but not allogrooming or sniffing. g, Heatmaps showing average responses of example self-grooming-responsive cells (with either increased or decreased activity) centered around self-grooming onset. h, Fraction of cells activated during both self-grooming and allogrooming or during both self-grooming and sniffing. i, Example image showing retrograde labelling of neurons in the accessory olfactory bulb (AOB) by injecting a retrograde AAV-EGFP virus in the MeA. Scale bar, 200 μm. c, d, g, Time 0 indicates behavior onset. e, n = 338, 1560, and 189800 pairwise distances for allogrooming-encoding cells, sniffing-encoding cells, and cells with shuffled identity, respectively. Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. h, Hypergeometric test. ns, not significant. For details of statistical analyses, see Supplementary Table 1

Source data.

Extended Data Fig. 4 MeA population activity encodes allogrooming and other behaviors during prosocial interaction.

a, Projection of MeA population activity onto the first principal component (PC) overlaid with annotation of social interaction (including allogrooming and sniffing) in an example animal. b, Trial-averaged PC1 activity centered around onset of social interaction across all bouts from all sessions. c, Quantification of area under ROC curve (auROC) characterizing the relationship between PC1 activity and social interaction. d, Pairwise comparisons of the average within- and between-behavior class Euclidean distances (measured on the first 2 PCs within each session) for allogrooming vs. sniffing, allogrooming vs. self-grooming, and sniffing vs. self-grooming. e, Performance of three-way multi-class SVM decoders trained to predict allogrooming, sniffing, or self-grooming behavior. fh, Fraction of time that subjects show different types of behaviors (allogrooming, sniffing, or self-grooming) during the 3 s prior to the onsets of allogrooming (f), sniffing (g), and self-grooming events (h). i, Time-course of behavior decoder performance in discriminating between allogrooming and sniffing centered around onset of behavior. Shuffle control decoders are constructed using time-permuted calcium traces. Note that although a fraction of allogrooming events were preceded by sniffing events (f), the performance of the decoder remains at chance level prior to the onset of behavior (i), suggesting that neural activity during preceding sniffing events is not sufficient to decode allogrooming vs. sniffing. j, ROC curve quantifying performance of a binary decoder to predict whether an allogrooming bout is short (≤ 5s) or long (> 5s) using population activity centered around onset of allogrooming. k, Decoder performance in (j) compared with a null distribution constructed using time-permuted calcium traces. Whiskers indicate the 2.5th and 97.5th percentiles of null distribution. Blue line: auROC from real data. ln, Fraction of time that subjects show allogrooming (l), sniffing (m), or self-grooming (n) during the 3 s prior to the onsets of long or short allogrooming events. Mean ± s.e.m. The ability to predict allogrooming bout duration using population activity is unlikely to be attributable to differences in behaviors preceding allogrooming as there is no difference in the distribution of different behaviors prior to allogrooming onset between the long and short bouts. b, i, Time 0 indicates behavior onset. b, fi, ln, mean ± s.e.m. Boxplots: median with quartiles, 1.5 × IQR and outliers. b, n = 7 independent imaging sessions (from 6 subject mice). c, n = 7 independent imaging sessions (from 6 subject mice) and 70 rounds of shuffling (10 rounds for each imaging session) for control group. Two-sided Wilcoxon rank-sum test. d, e, n = 7 independent imaging sessions (from 6 subject animals). Two-sided Wilcoxon signed-rank test. fh, n = 51, 292, 223 allogrooming (f), sniffing (g), and self-grooming (h) bouts, respectively (in 7 independent imaging sessions in 6 subject animals). k, permutation test (1000 rounds of permutation). ln, n = 51 short and 38 long allogrooming bouts (from 7 independent imaging sessions in 6 subject animals). Two-sided Wilcoxon rank-sum test. ***P < 0.001, **P < 0.01, *P < 0.05. ns, not significant. For details of statistical analyses, see Supplementary Table 1

Source data.

Extended Data Fig. 5 Activation of MeATac1 neurons promotes allogrooming and self-grooming in males and females.

af, in, Probability of allogrooming (ac, ik) and self-grooming (df, ln) (fraction of trials showing a particular behavior at different time points) with respect to stimulation onset during optogenetic activation in male (af) and female (in) Tac1-Cre (a, d, i, l), Sst-Cre (b, e, j, m), and Cck-Cre (c, f, k, n) animals injected with ChR2 in the MeA. Blue areas: duration of light illumination; time 0: stimulation onset. g, h, o, p, Duration of allogrooming (g, o) and self-grooming (h, p) during photostimulations in male (g, h) and female (o, p) Tac1-Cre, Sst-Cre, and Cck-Cre animals injected with ChR2 in the MeA. q, Average trace showing Ca2+ signal changes during allogrooming toward stressed partners in Sst-Cre subjects expressing GCaMP. Mean ± s.e.m. Time 0: allogrooming onset. r, Comparison of Ca2+ signal changes between Sst+ and Tac1+/Vgat+ neurons during allogrooming using mean ∆F/F after behavior onset. Boxplots: median with quartiles, 1.5 × IQR and outliers. ap, Tac1-Cre, n = 35 trials in 2 males and 17 trials 2 females for both allogrooming and self-grooming. Sst-Cre, n = 25 trials in 2 males and 36 trials in 2 females for both allogrooming and self-grooming. Cck-Cre, n = 24 trials in 2 males and 22 trials in 2 females for both allogrooming and self-grooming. Kruskal-Wallis test followed by post hoc Dunn’s multiple comparisons test. r, n = 40 bouts in 7 GCaMP animals for Sst+ neurons. n = 19 bouts in 6 GCaMP animals for Tac1+/Vgat + neurons. Data for Tac1+/Vgat+ neurons are the same as those in Fig. 4k and are presented here for comparison. Two-sided Wilcoxon rank-sum test. ***P < 0.001. **P < 0.01. For details of statistical analyses, see Supplementary Table 1

Source data.

Extended Data Fig. 6 Activation of MeATac1∩Vgat neurons promotes affiliative allogrooming in males and females.

a, d, Probability of allogrooming toward stressed partners (fraction of trials showing allogrooming at different time points) with respect to stimulation onset in male (a) and female (d) ChR2 animals. b, c, e, f, Boxplots of duration (b, e) and onset latency (c, f) of allogrooming during photostimulations in male (b, c) and female (e, f) eYFP control and ChR2 animals. g, No significant difference in the duration of sniffing during photostimulations between eYFP control and ChR2 animals. Blue areas: duration of light illumination; time 0: stimulation onset. eYFP control males, n = 22 trials in 2 mice. ChR2 males, n = 58 trials in 4 mice. eYFP control females, n = 55 trials in 4 mice. ChR2 females, n = 115 trials in 4 mice. In g, trials from males and females are combined. Boxplots: median with quartiles, 1.5 × IQR and outliers. Two-sided Wilcoxon rank-sum test. ***P < 0.001. ns, not significant. For details of statistical analyses, see Supplementary Table 1

Source data.

Extended Data Fig. 7 Activation of MeA Tac1+/Vgat neurons promotes self-grooming but not allogrooming.

a, b, Schematic of an intersectional approach for expression of ChR2 in Tac1+/Vgat neurons in the MeA using a Cre-on and Flp-off AAV virus. c, Example images showing that the majority of eYFP+ cells are glutamatergic (Vglut2+, 64.0 ± 1.8%, mean ± s.e.m.) and Tac1+ (91.8 ± 0.5%, mean ± s.e.m.) in Tac1cre/+/Vgatflp/+ animals injected with the Con/Foff-eYFP virus (n = 3 hemispheres independently injected with the virus from 2 mice (5-7 sections per hemisphere). Scale bar, 25 μm. d, e, Probability of self-grooming (d) and allogrooming (e) toward stressed partners (fraction of trials showing a particular behavior at different time points) with respect to stimulation onset in ChR2 animals. Blue areas: duration of light illumination; time 0: stimulation onset. f, Duration of self-grooming and allogrooming toward stressed partners during photostimulations. Boxplots: median with quartiles, 1.5 × IQR and outliers. df, n = 39 trials in 4 mice (18 trials in 2 females and 21 trials in 2 males) for both self-grooming and allogrooming. The Cre-on/Flp-off virus used in the current study has been reported to lead to residual expression in a minor fraction of Cre+/Flp+ cells, possibly due to insufficiency of Flp relative to Cre (refs. 26, 52). Nonetheless, we found that when using this virus, the majority of eYFP+ cells (64.0 ± 1.8%, mean ± s.e.m.) were Vglut2+. Of note, the observation that the Cre-on/Flp-off animals did not show induction of allogrooming behavior suggests that activation of the small fraction of Tac1+/Vgat+ neurons in these animals (concurrent with activation of Tac1+/Vgat neurons) was not sufficient to drive allogrooming behavior. On the other hand, the observation that activation of Tac1+/Vgat+ neurons in animals injected with the Cre-on/Flp-on virus did not trigger self-grooming behavior suggests that the residual Tac1+/Vgat+ neurons labelled with the Cre-on/Flp-off virus are not responsible for the induction of self-grooming

Source data.

Extended Data Fig. 8 MPOA-projecting MeATac1∩Vgat neurons drive affiliative allogrooming in males and females.

a, b, Example images showing axonal terminals of MeATac1∩Vgat neurons in the MPOA in male (a) and female (b) animals, revealed by immunostaining for eYFP in Tac1cre/+/Vgatflp/+ animals injected with Con/Fon-ChR2-eYFP. Scale bar, 200 μm. (c, h) Schematics of viral injection and fibre implantation strategies for soma stimulation of retrogradely labelled, MPOA-projecting MeATac1∩Vgat neurons (c) or stimulation of the axonal projection of MeATac1∩Vgat neurons in the MPOA (h). dg, Duration (d, f) and onset latency (e, g) of allogrooming during photostimulations in eYFP control and ChR2 males (d, e) and females (f, g) with soma stimulation of MPOA-projecting MeATac1∩Vgat neurons. il, Duration (i, k) and onset latency (j, l) of allogrooming during photostimulations in eYFP control and ChR2 males (i, j) and females (k, l) with stimulation of the MPOA projection of MeATac1∩Vgat neurons. mo, Probability of allogrooming toward stressed partners (fraction of trials showing allogrooming at different time points) with respect to stimulation onset (m, n) and allogrooming duration (o) during photostimulations of the MPOA projection of MeATac1∩Vgat neurons without or with infusion of lidocaine in the MeA. Blue areas: duration of light illumination; time 0: stimulation onset. p, Example image showing axonal projections of MeATac1∩Vgat neurons in the PMv. Scale bar, 200 μm. q, Duration of allogrooming during photostimulation of the MPOA or PMv projection of MeATac1∩Vgat neurons. Data for MPOA projection stimulation are the same as those in Fig. 4r and are presented here for comparison. Boxplots: median with quartiles, 1.5 × IQR and outliers. dg, il, eYFP control, MeA soma stimulation, n = 52 trials in 4 females and 29 trials in 2 males. ChR2, MeA soma stimulation, n = 68 trials in 4 females and 59 trials in 2 males. eYFP control, MPOA projection stimulation, n = 45 trials in 4 females and 22 trials in 2 males. ChR2, MPOA projection stimulation, n = 40 trials in 3 females and 53 trials in 3 males. mo, n = 36 trials in 4 mice (24 trials from 3 females and 12 trials from 1 male) for the “no lidocaine” group. n = 58 trials in 4 mice (39 trials from 3 females and 19 trials from 1 male) for the “lidocaine” group. q, n = 93 trials in 6 mice (40 trials in 3 females and 53 trials in 3 males) for MPOA stimulation, n = 118 trials in 4 mice (65 trials in 2 females and 53 trials in 2 males) for PMv stimulation. Two-sided Wilcoxon rank-sum test. ***P < 0.001. ns, not significant. For details of statistical analyses, see Supplementary Table 1

Source data.

Extended Data Fig. 9 Activation of MeAVgat neurons can promote allogrooming during prosocial interaction.

a, Schematic of ChR2 activation in MeAVgat neurons. b, c, Duration of allogrooming (b) and sniffing (c) toward stressed partners during low-intensity photostimulations in ChR2 and eYFP control animals. The increase in sniffing (~1.5 s) appears to be substantially smaller than that in allogrooming (~5 s), suggesting that increased allogrooming is the predominant behavioral effect. d, Duration of triggered allogrooming when subject animals are in the vicinity of and attending to the partners (“optimal” condition) compared to all stimulations. Boxplots: median with quartiles, 1.5 × IQR and outliers. b, eYFP control, n = 119 trials in 11 mice (74 trials in 6 females and 45 trials in 5 males); ChR2, n = 141 trials in 12 mice (88 trials in 7 females and 53 trials in 5 males). c, eYFP control, n = 119 trials in 11 mice (74 trials in 6 females and 45 trials in 5 males); ChR2, n = 142 trials in 12 mice (89 trials in 7 females and 53 trials in 5 males). d, All condition, n = 78 trials in 5 male mice; optimal condition (subject within half a body-length and facing the partner), n = 53 trials in 5 male mice. bd, Two-sided Wilcoxon rank-sum test. ***P < 0.001, **P < 0.01. For details of statistical analyses, see Supplementary Table 1

Source data.

Extended Data Fig. 10 Activation of MeA Tac1/Vgat+ neurons promotes aggression.

ab, Schematic of an intersectional approach for specific expression of ChR2 in Tac1/Vgat+ neurons in the MeA using a Cre-off and Flp-on AAV. c, Example images showing that eYFP+ cells are predominantly Vgat+ (99.3 ± 1.4%, mean ± s.e.m.) and Tac1 (90.1 ± 1.9%, mean ± s.e.m.) in Tac1cre/+/Vgatflp/+ animals injected with the Coff/Fon-eYFP AAV (n = 4 hemispheres independently injected with the virus from 2 mice (4-5 sections per hemisphere). Scale bar, 25 μm. d, e, Probability of aggression (d) and allogrooming (e) toward stressed partners (fraction of trials showing a particular behavior at different time points) with respect to stimulation onset in ChR2 animals. Blue areas: duration of light illumination; time 0: stimulation onset. f, Duration of aggression and allogrooming toward stressed partners during photostimulations. Boxplots: median with quartiles, 1.5 × IQR and outliers. df, n = 83 and 80 trials in 4 male mice for aggression and allogrooming, respectively

Source data.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Table 1, Supplementary Notes 1–8 and Supplementary References.

Reporting Summary

Supplementary Video 1 Affiliative allogrooming in mice

Naive mice display increased affiliative allogrooming towards stressed partners.

Supplementary Video 2 Microendoscopic imaging during prosocial interaction

Raw calcium fluorescence video (left) from microendoscopic imaging and extracted ROIs (right) showing single-neuron activity changes in the MeA during interaction with stressed conspecifics.

Supplementary Video 3 ChR2 activation of MeA Tac1+Vgat+ neurons triggers allogrooming

Optogenetic activation of MeA Tac1+Vgat+ neurons using ChR2 elicits allogrooming towards stressed partners in a time-locked manner.

Supplementary Video 4 ChR2 activation of the MPOA projection of MeA Tac1+Vgat+ neurons triggers allogrooming

Optogenetic activation of the MPOA projection of MeA Tac1+Vgat+ neurons using ChR2 elicits allogrooming towards stressed partners in a time-locked manner.

Source data

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wu, Y.E., Dang, J., Kingsbury, L. et al. Neural control of affiliative touch in prosocial interaction. Nature 599, 262–267 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-03962-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-03962-w

This article is cited by

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing