Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Cortex-dependent corrections as the tongue reaches for and misses targets

Abstract

Precise tongue control is necessary for drinking, eating and vocalizing1,2,3. However, because tongue movements are fast and difficult to resolve, neural control of lingual kinematics remains poorly understood. Here we combine kilohertz-frame-rate imaging and a deep-learning-based neural network to resolve 3D tongue kinematics in mice drinking from a water spout. Successful licks required corrective submovements that—similar to online corrections during primate reaches4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11—occurred after the tongue missed unseen, distant or displaced targets. Photoinhibition of anterolateral motor cortex impaired corrections, which resulted in hypometric licks that missed the spout. Neural activity in anterolateral motor cortex reflected upcoming, ongoing and past corrective submovements, as well as errors in predicted spout contact. Although less than a tenth of a second in duration, a single mouse lick exhibits the hallmarks of online motor control associated with a primate reach, including cortex-dependent corrections after misses.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: CSMs within licks are important for spout contact.
Fig. 2: ALM inactivation impairs CSMs.
Fig. 3: ALM activity reflects upcoming, ongoing and past CSMs.
Fig. 4: ALM activity is necessary for online corrections and is associated with double-step performance.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Code availability

The acquisition and behavioural software code that support the findings of this study are available at https://github.com/GoldbergLab/Bollu-Ito-et-al-2021.

References

  1. Kier, W. M. & Smith, K. K. Tongues, tentacles and trunks: the biomechanics of movement in muscular-hydrostats. Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 83, 307–324 (1985).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Chartier, J., Anumanchipalli, G. K., Johnson, K. & Chang, E. F. Encoding of articulatory kinematic trajectories in human speech sensorimotor cortex. Neuron 98, 1042–1054. e1044 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Arce-McShane, F. I., Hatsopoulos, N. G., Lee, J. C., Ross, C. F. & Sessle, B. J. Modulation dynamics in the orofacial sensorimotor cortex during motor skill acquisition. J. Neurosci. 34, 5985–5997 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Meyer, D. E., Abrams, R. A., Kornblum, S., Wright, C. E. & Smith, J. E. Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychol. Rev. 95, 340–370 (1988).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Spijkers, W. A. & Lochner, P. Partial visual feedback and spatial end-point accuracy of discrete aiming movements. J. Mot. Behav. 26, 283–295 (1994).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Khan, M. A. & Franks, I. M. Online versus offline processing of visual feedback in the production of component submovements. J. Mot. Behav. 35, 285–295 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Gordon, J., Ghilardi, M. F. & Ghez, C. Accuracy of planar reaching movements. I. Independence of direction and extent variability. Exp. Brain Res. 99, 97–111 (1994).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Ghez, C., Gordon, J., Ghilardi, M. F. & Sainburg, R. in The Cognitive Neurosciences (ed. Gazzaniga, M. S.) 549–564 (The MIT Press, 1995).

  9. Dickey, A. S., Amit, Y. & Hatsopoulos, N. G. Heterogeneous neural coding of corrective movements in motor cortex. Front. Neural Circuits 7, 51 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Ames, K. C., Ryu, S. I. & Shenoy, K. V. Neural dynamics of reaching following incorrect or absent motor preparation. Neuron 81, 438–451 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Ames, K. C., Ryu, S. I. & Shenoy, K. V. Simultaneous motor preparation and execution in a last-moment reach correction task. Nat. Commun. 10, 2718 (2019).

    Article  ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Komiyama, T. et al. Learning-related fine-scale specificity imaged in motor cortex circuits of behaving mice. Nature 464, 1182–1186 (2010).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Goard, M. J., Pho, G. N., Woodson, J. & Sur, M. Distinct roles of visual, parietal, and frontal motor cortices in memory-guided sensorimotor decisions. eLife 5, e13764 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Welsh, J. P., Lang, E. J., Suglhara, I. & Llinás, R. Dynamic organization of motor control within the olivocerebellar system. Nature 374, 453–457 (1995).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Crochet, S., Lee, S. H. & Petersen, C. C. H. Neural circuits for goal-directed sensorimotor transformations. Trends Neurosci. 42, 66–77 (2019).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Svoboda, K. & Li, N. Neural mechanisms of movement planning: motor cortex and beyond. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 49, 33–41 (2018).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Coddington, L. T. & Dudman, J. T. The timing of action determines reward prediction signals in identified midbrain dopamine neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 1563–1573 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Gaffield, M. A. & Christie, J. M. Movement rate is encoded and influenced by widespread, coherent activity of cerebellar molecular layer interneurons. J. Neurosci. 37, 4751–4765 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Xu, D. et al. A functional cortical network for sensorimotor sequence generation. Preprint at https://doi.org/10.1101/783050 (2019).

  20. Ronneberger, O., Fischer, P. & Brox, T. U-net: convolutional networks for biomedical image segmentation. In International Conference on Medical Image Computing and Computer-Assisted Intervention (ed. Navab, N. et al.) 234–241 (Springer, 2015).

  21. Beatus, T., Guckenheimer, J. M. & Cohen, I. Controlling roll perturbations in fruit flies. J. R. Soc. Interface 12, 20150075 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Elliott, D. & Madalena, J. The influence of premovement visual information on manual aiming. Quat. J. Exp. Psychol. 39, 541–559 (1987).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. McIntyre, J., Stratta, F. & Lacquaniti, F. Viewer-centered frame of reference for pointing to memorized targets in three-dimensional space. J. Neurophysiol. 78, 1601–1618 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Heath, M. & Westwood, D. A. Can a visual representation support the online control of memory-dependent reaching? Evidence from a variable spatial mapping paradigm. Motor Control 7, 349–365 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Westwood, D. A., Heath, M. & Roy, E. A. No evidence for accurate visuomotor memory: systematic and variable error in memory-guided reaching. J. Mot. Behav. 35, 127–133 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Woods, J. W. Behavior of chronic decerebrate rats. J. Neurophysiol. 27, 635–644 (1964).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. McElvain, L. E. et al. Circuits in the rodent brainstem that control whisking in concert with other orofacial motor actions. Neuroscience 368, 152–170 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Li, N., Chen, T. W., Guo, Z. V., Gerfen, C. R. & Svoboda, K. A motor cortex circuit for motor planning and movement. Nature 519, 51–56 (2015).

    Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Allen, W. E. et al. Global representations of goal-directed behavior in distinct cell types of mouse neocortex. Neuron 94, 891–907.e6 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Desmurget, M. et al. Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 563–567 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Rohrer, B. et al. Movement smoothness changes during stroke recovery. J. Neurosci. 22, 8297–8304 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Megaw, E. D. Possible modification to a rapid on-going programmed manual response. Brain Res. 71, 425–441 (1974).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Archambault, P. S., Ferrari-Toniolo, S. & Battaglia-Mayer, A. Online control of hand trajectory and evolution of motor intention in the parietofrontal system. J. Neurosci. 31, 742–752 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Georgopoulos, A. P., Kalaska, J. F., Caminiti, R. & Massey, J. T. Interruption of motor cortical discharge subserving aimed arm movements. Exp. Brain Res. 49, 327–340 (1983).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Goodale, M. A., Pelisson, D. & Prablanc, C. Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement. Nature 320, 748–750 (1986).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Diedrichsen, J., Hashambhoy, Y., Rane, T. & Shadmehr, R. Neural correlates of reach errors. J. Neurosci. 25, 9919–9931 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Pruszynski, J. A., Johansson, R. S. & Flanagan, J. R. A rapid tactile-motor reflex automatically guides reaching toward handheld objects. Curr. Biol. 26, 788–792 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Rico-Guevara, A. & Rubega, M. A. The hummingbird tongue is a fluid trap, not a capillary tube. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 9356–9360 (2011).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Harper, C. J., Swartz, S. M. & Brainerd, E. L. Specialized bat tongue is a hemodynamic nectar mop. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 8852–8857 (2013).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Reis, P. M., Jung, S., Aristoff, J. M. & Stocker, R. How cats lap: water uptake by Felis catus. Science 330, 1231–1234 (2010).

    Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. de Groot, J. H. & van Leeuwen, J. L. Evidence for an elastic projection mechanism in the chameleon tongue. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 271, 761–770 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Wei, J. et al. Sucking or lapping: facultative feeding mechanisms in honeybees (Apis mellifera). Biol. Lett. 16, 20200449 (2020).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Travers, J. B., Dinardo, L. A. & Karimnamazi, H. Motor and premotor mechanisms of licking. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 21, 631–647 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Hayar, A., Bryant, J. L., Boughter, J. D. & Heck, D. H. A low-cost solution to measure mouse licking in an electrophysiological setup with a standard analog-to-digital converter. J. Neurosci. Methods 153, 203–207 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Dayan, P. & Abbott, L. F. Theoretical neuroscience: computational and mathematical modeling of neural systems. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 154–155 (2003).

    Google Scholar 

  46. Sauerbrei, B. A. et al. Cortical pattern generation during dexterous movement is input-driven. Nature 577, 386–391 (2020).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank J. Fetcho, M. Warden, N. Hogan, C. Pandarinath and members of the laboratory of J.H.G. for comments on the manuscript, and B. Bari, A. Finkelstein, N. Prasad and J. Walker for technical advice. Funding to J.H.G. was provided by the NIH (grant no. DP2 HD087952), the Dystonia Medical Research Foundation, the Pew Charitable Trust, J. Sheng and the Klingenstein Neuroscience Foundation. Funding to B.S.I. was provided by the NSF Graduate Research Fellowship Program.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

T.B., B.S.I. and J.H.G. designed the experiments, analysed data and wrote the manuscript. T.B. and B.S.I. carried out the experiments. T.B., S.C.W. and J.R. developed tongue-tip tracking algorithms. T.B. and B.K. built the hardware and software necessary for data acquisition; T.B., B.S.I., and M.H.L. carried out mouse training, care and histology.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jesse H. Goldberg.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Peer review information Nature thanks the anonymous reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data figures and tables

Extended Data Fig. 1 Method for extracting 3D tongue tip kinematics.

a, Architecture of the artificial neural network (U-NET) used to segment the tongue from the background image. U-NET has characteristic symmetrical contraction and expansion paths that simultaneously capture localization and image context. Each box corresponds to a multi-channel feature map and numbers above each layer indicate the number of channels; colour-coded arrows indicate sequential processing steps. b, Pipeline for tongue segmentation. Left to right, top, side view of the tongue as the input image to U-NET, the identified tongue mask and the mask plus the input image. Bottom, process is repeated separately for the bottom view of the tongue. c, An example of the process used to generate a 3D voxel hull from the two views of the mouse tongue. The walls of the diagrams are stills taken from the high-speed video, with the segmented tongue mask highlighted in red. The final hull (rightmost diagram) is obtained by intersecting the projections of the side- and bottom-view tongue masks. d, A 2D illustration of the tip coordinate search. With the voxels (grey circle) and centroid (black circle) identified, the first search step is performed, in which candidate voxels (blue) are found via the intersection of voxels satisfying the two search criteria (yellow)—namely, thresholds on the maximum angle made with an initial search vector (blue arrow) and the minimum distance from the tongue centroid. These first candidate voxels are then used to generate a refined search vector (red arrow, second row) for the second step of the search. Using this refined search vector, a similar set of angle and distance thresholds are applied to determine a refined set of candidate voxels, which are then averaged to determine the tip location. e, Example of the tip search process with real data in 3D. The grey object is the 3D tongue hull, with the centroid labelled by a black circle. The first search step identifies a set of candidate voxels (blue) that are used to generate a refined search vector for the second search step (red). Using the second-step candidate voxels, the tongue tip location is estimated (green ‘x’). f, Average power spectral density plot of tongue tip trajectories from five representative mice. More than 90% of power was at frequencies less than 50 Hz. g, Two time-points of a single lick (left and right) are shown with tongue tip estimated with key points (red) and with volume reconstruction (blue). Though key-point tracking appears to work well from the side view, it fails in the bottom view as the true tongue tip does not always lie at the edge of the image of the tongue as seen from the bottom. This is because in most licks the tongue exhibits a ‘c’ shape at full extension. In these frames the tip is mislabelled by key points in the bottom view. Importantly, the error cannot be accounted for systematically because it varies dynamically within a lick according to the convexity of the tongue.

Extended Data Fig. 2 Water retrieval and cue-evoked licks exhibit distinct kinematics.

ac, Water-retrieval licks, defined as those initiated after spout contact. a, Six overlaid tongue tip trajectories during retrieval licks. A single lick is bold for clarity. b, Protrusion, CSM, SSM and retraction phases of the trajectories from a are separately plotted. The ‘x’ symbols denote the absence of CSMs and/or SSMs. c, Three-dimensional trajectory of the highlighted lick shown in a, with protrusion (green) and retraction (purple) lick phases indicated. df, Data plotted as in ac for cue-evoked licks. Note the prominent CSMs. g, Tongue tip speed profiles for retrieval (blue) and cue-evoked (black) trajectories shown in a, d. hk, Probability of CSMs (h) and durations (i), peak speeds (j) and path lengths (k) of distinct lick phases during cue-evoked (black) and retrieval (blue) licks. l, m, Kinematics (l) and entropy (m) of lick durations, path lengths, peak speeds and number of acceleration peaks. Data are median ± IQR. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001, two-sided paired Wilcoxon signed rank test; all data from sessions with spout at 3.2 mm. n = 17 mice. Exact statistics are in Supplementary Table 1.

Extended Data Fig. 3 Individual mice exhibit stereotyped tongue tip positions at retraction onset and spout contact.

a, Side and bottom views of the tongue at the moment of retraction onset from a representative lick. b, Scatter plots of tongue tip positions at retraction onset for side (top) and bottom (bottom) views during successful cue-evoked licks from a single session. Probability distributions are projected along the axes at top and right (bin size, 120 μm). Right, 2D standard deviations of tongue tip positions at retraction onset for nine representative mice (each mouse independently colour-coded). Each mouse exhibits a ‘preferred’ target location for retraction onset. Similarly, tongue CSMs terminated at precisely clustered tongue tip positions beneath the spout in a way that was unique for each mouse. c, Tongue tip positions at moment of retraction onset plotted as in b for retrieval licks. df, Data plotted as in ac for tongue tip positions at the moment of spout contact, for the same nine mice. g, Probability of spout contact as a function of the distinct lick phases for cue-evoked and water-retrieval licks (blue and black, respectively, median ± IQR, n = 17 mice). h, i, The number of acceleration peaks per lick predicts latency to spout contact. h, The latency to spout contact relative to protrusion onset is plotted against the number of acceleration peaks per lick from a single spout-far session. Red line, linear fit. i, Box plot showing r2 for linear fits across 17 mice (red line, median; box edges, IQR; whiskers, 95% confidence interval).

Extended Data Fig. 4 Tongue protrusions remain aimed during ALM inactivation.

a, Left, Five example tongue protrusions (from bottom view) from a single session with the spout placed to the left (blue, ALM inactivated; black, ALM intact). Green ellipse denotes 95% confidence interval of the tongue tip location at the moment of retraction onset. Centre, scatter plot of tongue tip positions at tongue protrusion offsets. Probability distributions of ALM intact (black) and inactivated (blue) dots are projected along the axes at top and right (bin size, 120 μm). Green line, midline. b, c, Data plotted in a for sessions with centred (b) and right (c) spout placements. d, Left, the lateral placement of the tongue tip at the moment of protrusion offset is plotted across left, straight and right sessions (black, laser off; blue, ALM inactivated). Right, the average difference in lateral displacement between ALM-intact and ALM-inactivated trials. Data in d are median ± IQR across n = 13, 15 and 12 mice for spout left, centre and right, respectively; *P < 0.05 for a one sample two-sided Wilcoxon signed rank test. Exact statistics are in Supplementary Table 2.

Extended Data Fig. 5 CSMs are directionally biased towards remembered spout locations.

ae, CSM kinematics for spout-left sessions. a, Time-dependent velocity vector for the protrusion (green) and CSM (orange) phase of a single cue-evoked lick. The origin of each vector is the tongue tip position at 5-ms intervals of the lick, the amplitude is the speed and the arrow points in the direction of motion. Inset, polar plot with direction distribution of all CSMs produced in a single spout-left session. Dashed circle, the null distribution of unbiased CSM directions. b, Scatter plot of CSM directions plotted against protrusion directions for all cue-evoked licks in the session. c, Position-dependent average velocity vectors for all cue-evoked licks from a single session, colour-coded by highest likelihood lick phase to pass through the binned space (250-μm grid). Grey shading intensity of each bin is proportional to the probability of a tongue tip trajectory passing through the space. d, Scatter plot of tongue tip positions at protrusion offset (green) and retraction onset (orange), indicating CSM start and end points, respectively. Probability distributions of the CSM start and end points are projected along the axes at top and right, respectively (bin size, 120 μm). e, Example of a single CSM path and its speed profile (orange). The initial direction of the CSM (VCSM, black dotted line) was computed from the vector connecting the CSM starting point to its position at the first speed minimum (upward black triangle in speed and path plots). The dot products between this CSM direction vector and three additional vectors from CSM starting point to left, centre and right targets (dotted red, green, and blue lines, respectively) were computed to quantify the ‘direction bias’, the extent to which the initial direction of a given CSM was aimed at each of the three candidate targets (targets defined independently for each mouse and each session as its median tongue tip position at moment of retraction onset (Extended Data Fig. 3, Methods)). f, Left, cumulative distributions of directional biases for all CSMs produced in a single session to the three candidate targets (coloured as in b). CSMs were reliably aimed to the left target. Right, directional biases of CSMs to left, centre and right targets in spout-left sessions(n = 13 mice). gl, CSM kinematics for spout-centre sessions, plotted as in af (n = 17 mice). mr, CSM kinematics for spout-right sessions, plotted as in af (n = 12 mice). Data in f, l and r are median ± IQR.* P < 0.05, **P <0.01, two-sided Wilcoxon ranked-sum test. No corrections for multiple comparisons were made. Exact statistics are in Supplementary Table 3.

Extended Data Fig. 6 Uncertainty in spout position is associated with the need for corrections.

ad, The first spout contact transforms the kinematics of subsequent licks in a bout. a, Tongue volumes as a function of time during three trials in which first spout contact (black dashed line) occurred on the first, second or third lick. Licks initiated before spout contact exhibited substantial CSMs, whereas those initiated after spout contact did not. b, CSM probability as a function of lick number in cases in which first spout contact happened on first, second or third licks (n = 17 mice). Spout contact reliably transformed the kinematics of subsequently initiated licks. Data in b from sessions in which water was dispensed on first spout contact. c, d, CSMs when water was dispensed on the second spout contact (n = 12 mice). c, In sessions in which water was dispensed on second contact, both spout contact on L1 and water dispensed on L2 contact reduced CSMs on ensuing licks. d, CSM duration on first cue-evoked licks (L1) did not depend on water dispensation on first contact. e, f, Mixed-effects models were used to predict the duration (e) and probability (f) of CSMs on first licks of a bout (Methods). CSM durations were significantly predicted by trial number in session and time since previous spout contact (tp_sp), but not reaction time (tRT). CSM probabilities were predicted by tp_sp and by tRT (n = 8 mice, 1,507 trials). g, CSM probability scales with spout distance. n = 17, 11 and 13 mice for spout at 3.2, 2.4 and 1.6 mm, respectively. Data in bd, g are median ± IQR. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, two-sided Wilcoxon rank-sum test. Data in e, f are mean ± s.e.m. of the model estimates of the coefficients; **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001, two-sided t-test. No corrections for multiple comparisons were made. Exact statistics are in Supplementary Tables 46.

Extended Data Fig. 7 Electrophysiological validation of photoinhibition in Vgat-ChR2-EYFP mice.

a, Voltage waveforms of putative pyramidal neuron (top) and interneuron (bottom) during one second illumination of 40-Hz sinusoidal wave at 10 mW, the same power and waveform generated in behavioural experiments. b, Spike rasters and corresponding rate histograms of the neurons from a. c, The z-scored firing rates of 71 ALM neurons before, during and after optogenetic activation of inhibitory interneurons in Vgat-ChR2-EYFP mice (n = 2 sessions, n = 2 mice) (Methods).

Extended Data Fig. 8 Effects of ALM and PMM inactivation on lick kinematics.

ac, Inactivation of PMM does not affect task performance or lick kinematics. a, Cumulative probability of tongue–spout contact relative to cue onset during laser-off and PMM-photoinactivated trials. Right, probability of spout contact within a trial across mice (n = 9 mice). b, Data plotted as in a for tongue protrusions. c, Median durations, path lengths and peak speeds for lick phases with PMM intact (black) and PMM inactivated (blue), d, Effect of ALM photoinhibition on the duration, path length speed and number of acceleration peaks in cue-evoked licks. e, ALM photoinhibition reduced the variability of L1 kinematics. Data in d, e (n = 12 mice) are from trials in which L1 protrusions existed during control (black) and ALM-inactivated (blue) trials with a minimum of 10 data points for each lick phase. fh, Effect of ALM photoinhibition on water-retrieval licks. ALM photoinhibition reduced the probabilities of spout contact (f) and CSM generation (g) (blue) (control trials in black). h, Median duration, path lengths and peak speeds of retrieval lick phases produced with ALM intact and inactivated. Data (n = 12 mice) of the first retrieval lick that followed cue-evoked licks that made contact during ALM photoinhibition. ik, Proximal spout placement rescues ALM-inactivation-associated spout contact deficits. Cumulative probability of spout contact relative to cue onset for control (black) and ALM-inactivated (blue) trials in sessions in which the spout was 1.6 mm (i) and 3.2 mm (j) from the incisors. k, Median probability of spout contact across mice from spout-close and spout-far sessions (n = 13 mice). Data in ah, k are median ± IQR. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001, two-sided paired Wilcoxon signed-rank test. Exact statistics are in Supplementary Tables 710.

Extended Data Fig. 9 Online corrections on L3 of double-step trials have neural correlates in ALM.

a, Tongue volumes, spike rasters and corresponding rate and double-step-selectivity histograms for two example ALM neurons (example neuron 1 (a) and example neuron 2 (f)). Neural activity aligned to L3 protrusion onset. Raster colour codes are as in Fig. 4. Bottom (k), ALM population double-step selectivity, defined as the normalized difference in firing rate from control and double-step trials (Methods). Only neurons with significant trial selectivity are shown (n = 234 out of 465 neurons). be, gj, lo, Data plotted as in a for the following conditions: premature bout termination following L3 (n = 147 out of 438 neurons) (b, g, l), L3 spout misses resulting in bout continuation (n = 85 out of 418 neurons) (c, h, m), CSMs on L3 misses (n = 10 out of 79 neurons) (d, i, n), and spout location on L3 contacts (n = 18 out of 167 neurons) (e, j, o). Histograms are bootstrapped mean ± s.e.m.

Extended Data Fig. 10 Centroid-based tracking confirms the presence of CSMs on cue-evoked licks and their reduction during retrieval licks and ALM inactivation.

ac, The speed of the tongue centroid (Extended Data Fig. 1) plotted above the absolute values of rate of tongue volume change for an example cue-evoked lick with ALM intact (a), with ALM inactivated (b) and a retrieval lick (c). dg, Data plotted as in Fig. 2m–p for the same mice and sessions but with lick phase kinematics computed from centroid-based tongue tracking, in which the first and last minima in centroid speeds defined protrusion offset and retraction onset. Data are median ± IQR, n = 12 mice, *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001, two-sided paired Wilcoxon signed-rank test. No corrections for multiple comparisons were made. Exact statistics are in Supplementary Table 16.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

This file contains Supplementary Discussion with additional references and Supplementary Tables 1-16.

Reporting Summary

Supplementary Video 1

Cue-evoked licks and water retrieval licks exhibited distinct kinematics. Side and bottom views of the mouse tongue during a lick bout at full speed and slowed 1/100x. Left to right: Raw video, video with tongue labeled by a convolutional artificial neural network, and the estimated tongue tip trajectory during a cue-evoked lick and the subsequent water retrieval lick. Lick phases are color coded as in Fig. 1.

Supplementary Video 2

Spout contact altered kinematics of subsequent licks. Representative example showing the absence of corrective submovements on a water retrieval lick following successful spout contact.

Supplementary Video 3

ALM inactivation at cue onset abolished the generation of corrective submovements. Example of a cue-evoked lick from the same behavioral session as Video 1 with ALM bilaterally photoinhibited. Note absence of CSMs in the tongue tip trajectory.

Supplementary Video 4

Example cue-evoked and water retrieval licks from a session with left spout position.

Supplementary Video 5

Example cue-evoke lick from a session with left spout position in an ALM-inactivated trial. Note that ALM inhibition at cue onset abolished the generation of corrective submovements but did not affect aiming of protrusions.

Supplementary Video 6

ALM inactivation at protrusion (not cue) onset impaired spout contact but not the probability of generating a corrective submovement.

Supplementary Video 7

Video from a double-step trial, in which the spout was retracted by 1 mm immediately after the offset of the first lick’s spout contact. Note both within-lick and across lick corrections. After L2 protrusion missed the more distant spout, CSMs were generated that resulted in contact. After the L2 contact at the more distant site the pathlength of L3 was increased.

Supplementary Video 8

Video from a double-step trial in which ALM was inactivated at the moment of L1 spout contact offset. ALM inactivation impaired the generation of corrective submovements and spout contact.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bollu, T., Ito, B.S., Whitehead, S.C. et al. Cortex-dependent corrections as the tongue reaches for and misses targets. Nature 594, 82–87 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-03561-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-03561-9

This article is cited by

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing