Abstract
The spread of misinformation online is a global problem that requires global solutions. To that end, we conducted an experiment in 16 countries across 6 continents (N = 34,286; 676,605 observations) to investigate predictors of susceptibility to misinformation about COVID-19, and interventions to combat the spread of this misinformation. In every country, participants with a more analytic cognitive style and stronger accuracy-related motivations were better at discerning truth from falsehood; valuing democracy was also associated with greater truth discernment, whereas endorsement of individual responsibility over government support was negatively associated with truth discernment in most countries. Subtly prompting people to think about accuracy had a generally positive effect on the veracity of news that people were willing to share across countries, as did minimal digital literacy tips. Finally, aggregating the ratings of our non-expert participants was able to differentiate true from false headlines with high accuracy in all countries via the ‘wisdom of crowds’. The consistent patterns we observe suggest that the psychological factors underlying the misinformation challenge are similar across different regional settings, and that similar solutions may be broadly effective.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Access options
Access Nature and 54 other Nature Portfolio journals
Get Nature+, our best-value online-access subscription
$29.99 / 30 days
cancel any time
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 12 digital issues and online access to articles
$119.00 per year
only $9.92 per issue
Buy this article
- Purchase on SpringerLink
- Instant access to full article PDF
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout
Similar content being viewed by others
Data availability
Data are accessible through this link: https://osf.io/g65qu/.
Code availability
Code and materials are accessible through this link: https://osf.io/g65qu/.
Change history
18 July 2023
A Correction to this paper has been published: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-023-01676-9
References
Lazer, D. et al. The science of fake news. Science 9, 1094–1096 (2018).
Bradshaw, S. & Howard, P. N. The Global Disinformation Order: 2019 Global Inventory of Organised Social Media Manipulation (Univ. Oxford, 2019).
Whitten-Woodring, J., Kleinberg, M. S., Thawnghmung, A. & Thitsar, M. T. Poison if you don’t know how to use it: Facebook, Democracy, and Human Rights in Myanmar. Int. J. Press. 25, 407–425 (2020).
Mozur, P. A genocide incited on Facebook, with posts from Myanmar’s military. The New York Times (15 October 2018).
Arun, C. On WhatsApp, rumours, and lynchings. Econ. Polit. Wkly 54, 7–8 (2019).
Khandelwal, D., Gildejeva, K. & Miller, E. Covid lies are tearing through India’s family WhatsApp groups. Wired (14 April 2021).
Lederer, E. UN chief says misinformation about COVID-19 is new enemy. AP News (28 March 2020).
Roozenbeek, J. et al. Susceptibility to misinformation about COVID-19 around the world: susceptibility to COVID misinformation. R. Soc. Open Sci. 7, 201199 (2020).
Basch, C. H., Meleo-Erwin, Z., Fera, J., Jaime, C. & Basch, C. E. A global pandemic in the time of viral memes: COVID-19 vaccine misinformation and disinformation on TikTok. Hum. Vaccin Immunother. 17, 2373–2377 (2021).
Loomba, S., de Figueiredo, A., Piatek, S. J., de Graaf, K. & Larson, H. J. Measuring the impact of COVID-19 vaccine misinformation on vaccination intent in the UK and USA. Nat. Hum. Behav. 5, 337–348 (2021).
Pennycook, G., McPhetres, J., Bago, B. & Rand, D. G. Beliefs about COVID-19 in Canada, the United Kingdom, and the United States: a novel test of political polarization and motivated reasoning. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 48, 750–765 (2022).
Bursztyn, L., Rao, A., Roth, C. & Yanagizawa-Drott, D. Misinformation during a pandemic. Becker Friedman Inst. Work. Pap. https://bfi.uchicago.edu/working-paper/2020-44/ (2020).
Brennen, J. S., Simon, F., Howard, P. N. & Kleis Nielsen, R. Types, sources, and claims of COVID-19 misinformation. Reuters Institute https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac.uk/types-sources-and-claims-covid-19-misinformation (2020).
Fleming, N. Coronavirus misinformation, and how scientists can help to fight it. Nature 583, 155–156 (2020).
Jain, S. India’s healthcare workers are busting misinformation on WhatsApp. The Verge https://www.theverge.com/22535642/covid-misinformation-india-asha-whatsapp (2021).
Simonov, A., Sacher, S., Dube, J.-P. & Biswas, S. The persuasive effect of Fox News: non-compliance with social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic. Natl. Bur. Econ. Res. https://www.nber.org/system/files/working_papers/w27237/w27237.pdf (2020).
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. & Norenzayan, A. The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 61–83 (2010).
Fawzi, N. et al. Concepts, causes and consequences of trust in news media—a literature review and framework. Ann. Int. Commun. Assoc. 45, 154–174 (2021).
Mardikyan, S., Yıldız, E., Ordu, M. & Şimşek, B. Examining the global digital divide: a cross-country analysis. Commun. IBIMA https://doi.org/10.5171/2015.592253 (2015).
Sanou, B. ICT facts and figures 2016. International Telecommunication Union https://www.itu.int/en/ITU-D/Statistics/Documents/facts/ICTFactsFigures2016.pdf (2016).
Kemp, S. Digital 2021: Global Overview Report. DataReportal https://datareportal.com/reports/digital-2021-global-overview-report (2021).
Allen, J., Arechar, A. A., Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. Scaling up fact-checking using the wisdom of crowds. Sci. Adv. 7, 36 (2021).
Fazio, L. K., Rand, D. G. & Pennycook, G. Repetition increases perceived truth equally for plausible and implausible statements. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 26, 1705–1710 (2019).
Pennycook, G., Binnendyk, J., Newton, C. & Rand, D. G. A practical guide to doing behavioural research on fake news and misinformation. Collabra Psychol. 7, 25293 (2021).
Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. The psychology of fake news. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 388–402 (2021).
Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. Lazy, not biased: susceptibility to partisan fake news is better explained by lack of reasoning than by motivated reasoning. Cognition 188, 39–50 (2019).
Frederick, S. Cognitive reflection and decision making. J. Econ. Perspect. 19, 25–42 (2005).
Bago, B., Rand, D. & Pennycook, G. Fake news, fast and slow: deliberation reduces belief in false (but not true) news headlines. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 149, 1608–1613 (2020).
Osmundsen, M., Bor, A., Vahlstrup, P. B., Bechmann, A. & Petersen, M. B. Partisan polarization is the primary psychological motivation behind ‘fake news’ sharing on Twitter. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 115, 999–1015 (2021).
Arceneaux, K. et al. Some people just want to watch the world burn: the prevalence, psychology and politics of the ‘Need for Chaos’. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 376, 20200147 (2021).
Grant, A. M. & Shandell, M. S. Social motivation at work: the organizational psychology of effort for, against, and with others. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 73, 301–326 (2022).
Rathje, S., Roozenbeek, J., Van Bavel, J. J. & van der Linden, S. Accuracy and social motivations shape judgements of (mis)information. Nat. Hum. Behav. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-023-01540-w (2023).
Pretus, C. et al. The role of political devotion in sharing partisan misinformation. Preprint at PsyArXiv https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/7k9gx (2021).
Van Bavel, J. J. & Pereira, A. The partisan brain: an identity-based model of political belief. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 213–224 (2018).
Jost, J. T. & Krochik, M. Ideological differences in epistemic motivation: implications for attitude structure, depth of information processing, susceptibility to persuasion, and stereotyping. Adv. Motiv. Sci. 1, 181–231 (2014).
Rutjens, B. T., Sutton, R. M. & van der Lee, R. Not all skepticism is equal: exploring the ideological antecedents of science acceptance and rejection. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 44, 384–405 (2018).
Campbell, T. H. & Kay, A. C. Solution aversion: on the relation between ideology and motivated disbelief. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 107, 809–824 (2014).
Pennycook, G., Cannon, T. D. & Rand, D. G. Prior exposure increases perceived accuracy of fake news. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 147, 1865–1880 (2018).
Smelter, T. J. & Calvillo, D. P. Pictures and repeated exposure increase perceived accuracy of news headlines. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 34, 1061–1071 (2020).
Calvillo, D. P. & Smelter, T. J. An initial accuracy focus reduces the effect of prior exposure on perceived accuracy of news headlines. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 5, 55 (2020).
Effron, D. A. & Raj, M. Misinformation and morality: encountering fake-news headlines makes them seem less unethical to publish and share. Psychol. Sci. 31, 75–87 (2020).
Pennycook, G., McPhetres, J., Zhang, Y., Lu, J. G. & Rand, D. G. Fighting COVID-19 misinformation on social media: experimental evidence for a scalable accuracy nudge intervention. Psychol. Sci. 31, 770–780 (2020).
Pennycook, G. et al. Shifting attention to accuracy can reduce misinformation online. Nature 592, 590–595 (2021).
Freiling, I., Krause, N. M., Scheufele, D. A. & Brossard, D. Believing and sharing misinformation, fact-checks, and accurate information on social media: the role of anxiety during COVID-19. N. Media Soc. 25, 141–162 (2023).
Li, M., Chen, Z. & Rao, L.-L. Emotion, analytic thinking and susceptibility to misinformation during the COVID-19 outbreak. Comput. Hum. Behav. 133, 107295 (2022).
Saling, L. L., Mallal, D., Scholer, F., Skelton, R. & Spina, D. No one is immune to misinformation: an investigation of misinformation sharing by subscribers to a fact-checking newsletter. PLoS ONE 16, e0255702 (2021).
Petersen, M., Osmundsen, M. & Arceneaux, K. The “need for chaos” and motivations to share hostile political rumors. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055422001447 (2023).
Mosleh, M., Martel, C., Eckles, D. & Rand, D. G. Perverse downstream consequences of debunking: being corrected by another user for posting false political news increases subsequent sharing of low qality, partisan, and toxic content in a twiter field experiment. In Proc. 2021 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (eds Kitamura, Y. et al.) 1–13 (Association for Computing Machinery, 2021).
Epstein, Z. et al. Developing an accuracy-prompt toolkit to reduce COVID-19 misinformation online. Harv. Kennedy Sch. Misinformation Rev. https://doi.org/10.37016/mr-2020-71 (2021).
Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. Accuracy prompts are a replicable and generalizable approach for reducing the spread of misinformation. Nat. Commun. 13, 2333 (2022).
Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. Nudging social media sharing towards accuracy. Ann. Am. Acad. Pol. Soc. Sci. 700, 152–164 (2022).
Guess, A. M. et al. A digital media literacy intervention increases discernment between mainstream and false news in the United States and India. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 117, 15536–15545(2020).
Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. The right way to fight fake news. The New York Times (24 March 2020).
Godel, W. et al. Moderating with the mob: evaluating the efficacy of real-time crowdsourced fact-checking. J. Online Trust Saf. https://doi.org/10.54501/jots.v1i1.15 (2021).
Resnick, P., Alfayez, A., Im, J. & Gilbert, E. Searching for or reviewing evidence improves crowdworkers’ misinformation judgments and reduces partisan bias. Collective Inteligence https://doi.org/10.1177/26339137231173407 (2023).
Martel, C., Allen, J. N. L., Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. Crowds can effectively identify misinformation at scale. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. (in the press).
Porter, E. & Wood, T. J. The global effectiveness of fact-checking: evidence from simultaneous experiments in Argentina, Nigeria, South Africa, and the United Kingdom. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 118, e2104235118 (2021).
Bak-Coleman, J. et al. Combining interventions to reduce the spread of viral misinformation. Nat. Hum. Behav 6, 1372–1380 (2022).
Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. Who falls for fake news? The roles of bullshit receptivity, overclaiming, familiarity, and analytic thinking. J. Pers. 88, 185–200 (2020).
Faris, R. M. et al. Partisanship, propaganda, and disinformation: online media and the 2016 U.S. Presidential Election. Berkman Klein Cent. Internet Soc. Res. Pap. https://ssrn.com/abstract=3019414 (2017).
Berinsky, A. Rumors and health care reform: experiments in political misinformation. Br. J. Polit. Sci. 47, 241–262 (2017).
Mosleh, M., Pennycook, G. & Rand, D. G. Self-reported willingness to share political news articles in online surveys correlates with actual sharing on Twitter. PLoS ONE 15, e0228882 (2020).
Acknowledgements
The authors gratefully acknowledge funding from: The MIT Sloan Latin America Office; The Sloan Foundation; The National Science Foundation (2047152); The Ethics and Governance of Artificial Intelligence Initiative of the Miami Foundation; The William and Flora Hewlett Foundation; The Reset Initiative of Luminate (part of the Omidyar Network); The John Templeton Foundation; The TDF Foundation; The Canadian Institutes of Health Research; The Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada; The Australian Research Council (DP180102384); Google. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
A.A.A., A.J.B., G.P. and D.G.R. conceived the research; A.A.A., J.A., A.J.B., R.C., Z.E., K.G., A.G., J.G.L., R.M.R., M.N.S., Y.Z., G.P. and D.G.R. designed the study; A.A.A. conducted the study; A.A.A., J.A. and D.G.R. analysed the data; A.A.A., G.P. and D.G.R. wrote the paper with input from J.A., A.J.B., R.C., Z.E., K.G., A.G., J.G.L., R.M.R., M.N.S. and Y.Z.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
A.J.B., G.P. and D.G.R. received research support through gifts from Google. G.P. and D.G.R. received research support through gifts from Facebook. R.C. and A.G. were employees of Google when the work was conducted. A.J.B. and G.P. were Faculty Research Fellows at Google for several months in 2002. The other authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Human Behaviour thanks Nathan Walter, Michael Hameleers and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Supplementary Information
Supplementary Text, Figs. 1–15 and Tables 1–7.
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
About this article
Cite this article
Arechar, A.A., Allen, J., Berinsky, A.J. et al. Understanding and combatting misinformation across 16 countries on six continents. Nat Hum Behav 7, 1502–1513 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-023-01641-6
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-023-01641-6
This article is cited by
-
Human detection of political speech deepfakes across transcripts, audio, and video
Nature Communications (2024)
-
Softly empowering a prosocial expert in the family: lasting effects of a counter-misinformation intervention in an informational autocracy
Scientific Reports (2024)
-
Misunderstanding the harms of online misinformation
Nature (2024)
-
Companies inadvertently fund online misinformation despite consumer backlash
Nature (2024)
-
Battling the coronavirus ‘infodemic’ among social media users in Kenya and Nigeria
Nature Human Behaviour (2024)