Abstract
We present the results of a large, US$8.9 million campaign-wide field experiment, conducted among 2 million moderate- and low-information persuadable voters in five battleground states during the 2020 US presidential election. Treatment group participants were exposed to an 8-month-long advertising programme delivered via social media, designed to persuade people to vote against Donald Trump and for Joe Biden. We found no evidence that the programme increased or decreased turnout on average. We found evidence of differential turnout effects by modelled level of Trump support: the campaign increased voting among Biden leaners by 0.4 percentage points (s.e. = 0.2 pp) and decreased voting among Trump leaners by 0.3 percentage points (s.e. = 0.3 pp) for a difference in conditional average treatment effects of 0.7 points (t1,035,571 = −2.09; P = 0.036; \(\widehat{{\rm{DIC}}}=0.7\) points; 95% confidence interval = −0.014 to 0). An important but exploratory finding is that the strongest differential effects appear in early voting data, which may inform future work on early campaigning in a post-COVID electoral environment. Our results indicate that differential mobilization effects of even large digital advertising campaigns in presidential elections are likely to be modest.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Access options
Access Nature and 54 other Nature Portfolio journals
Get Nature+, our best-value online-access subscription
$29.99 per month
cancel any time
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 12 digital issues and online access to articles
$119.00 per year
only $9.92 per issue
Rent or buy this article
Get just this article for as long as you need it
$39.95
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout






Data availability
An anonymized replication dataset is available via Dataverse at https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataset.xhtml?persistentId=doi:10.7910/DVN/YMKVA1. TargetSmart generously agreed to make replication data available for this paper. By downloading replication data, researchers agree to use the data only for academic research, agree not to share the data with outside parties and agree not to attempt to re-identify individuals in the dataset in order to download the data.
Code availability
Replication scripts are available via Dataverse at https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataset.xhtml?persistentId=doi:10.7910/DVN/YMKVA1.
References
Romer, D., Kenski, K., Winneg, K., Adasiewicz, C. & Jamieson, K. H. Capturing Campaign Dynamics, 2000 & 2004: The National Annenberg Election Survey (Univ. Pennsylvania Press, 2006).
Sides, J., Vavreck, L. & Warshaw, C. The effect of television advertising in United States elections. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 116, 702–718 (2022).
Druckman, J. N. The implications of framing effects for citizen competence. Polit. Behav. 23, 225–256 (2001).
Druckman, J. N. & Holmes, J. W. Does presidential rhetoric matter? Priming and presidential approval. Pres. Stud. Q. 34, 755–778 (2004).
Coppock, A., Hill, S. J. & Vavreck, L. The small effects of political advertising are small regardless of context, message, sender, or receiver: evidence from 59 real-time randomized experiments. Sci. Adv. 6, 36 (2020).
Chong, D. & Druckman, J. N. A theory of framing and opinion formation in competitive elite environments. J. Commun. 57, 99–118 (2007).
Zaller, J. R. The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1992).
Vavreck, L. The Message Matters: The Economy and Presidential Campaigns (Princeton Univ. Press, 2009).
Gerber, A. S., Gimpel, J. G., Green, D. P. & Shaw, D. R. How large and long-lasting are the persuasive effects of televised campaign ads? Results from a randomized field experiment. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 105, 135–150 (2011).
Kalla, J. L. & Broockman, D. E. The minimal persuasive effects of campaign contact in general elections: evidence from 49 field experiments. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 112, 148–166 (2018).
Mellman, M. Are we doing it all wrong? The Hill https://thehill.com/opinion/campaign/353720-mark-mellman-are-we-doing-it-all-wrong (2017).
Spenkuch, J. L. & Toniatti, D. Political advertising and election results. Q. J. Econ. 133, 1981–2036 (2018).
Ansolabehere, S., Iyengar, S., Simon, A. & Valentino, N. Does attack advertising demobilize the electorate? Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 88, 829–838 (1994).
Wattenberg, M. P. & Brians, C. L. Negative campaign advertising: demobilizer or mobilizer?. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 93, 891–899 (1999).
Ansolabehere, S. D., Iyengar, S. & Simon, A. Replicating experiments using aggregate and survey data: the case of negative advertising and turnout. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 93, 901–909 (1999).
Kahn, K. F. & Kenney, P. J. Do negative campaigns mobilize or suppress turnout? Clarifying the relationship between negativity and participation. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 93, 877–889 (1999).
Freedman, P. & Goldstein, K. Measuring media exposure and the effects of negative campaign ads. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 43, 1189–1208 (1999).
Goldstein, K. & Freedman, P. Campaign advertising and voter turnout: new evidence for a stimulation effect. J. Polit. 64, 721–740 (2002).
Finkel, S. E. & Geer, J. G. A spot check: casting doubt on the demobilizing effect of attack advertising. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 42, 573–595 (1998).
Krupnikov, Y. When does negativity demobilize? Tracing the conditional effect of negative campaigning on voter turnout. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 55, 797–813 (2011).
Lau, R. R. & Rovner, I. B. Negative campaigning. Annu. Rev. Polit. Sci. 12, 285–306 (2009).
Schuirmann, D. J. A comparison of the two one-sided tests procedure and the power approach for assessing the equivalence of average bioavailability. J. Pharmacokinet. Biopharm. 15, 657–680 (1987).
Lakens, D. Equivalence tests: a practical primer for t tests, correlations, and meta-analyses. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 8, 355–362 (2017).
Samuelsohn, D. Facebook: Russian-linked accounts bought $150,000 in ads during 2016 race. Politico https://www.politico.com/story/2017/09/06/facebook-ads-russia-linked-accounts-242401 (2017).
Redlawsk, D. Feeling Politics: Emotion in Political Information Processing (Springer, 2006).
Albertson, B., Dun, L. & Kushner Gadarian, S. in The Oxford Handbook of Electoral Persuasion (eds Suhay, E. et al.) 169–183 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2020).
Broockman, D. E. & Kalla, J. L. When and why are campaigns’ persuasive effects small? Evidence from the 2020 U.S. presidential election. Am. J. Pol. Sci. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12724 (2022).
Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. B Methodol. 57, 289–300 (1995).
Fowler, E.F., Franz, M. & Ridout, T.N. Political advertising in the United States (Routledge, 2021).
Broockman, D. & Kalla, J. The impacts of selective partisan media exposure: a field experiment with Fox News viewers. Preprint at OSF https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/jrw26 (2022).
House GOP campaign arm created misleading fake news sites. Talking Points Memo https://talkingpointsmemo.com/livewire/nrcc-fake-news-sites (2014).
Shaw, D. Blue dog-affiliated website blurs line between newsroom and political advertiser. Sludge https://readsludge.com/2021/09/17/blue-dog-affiliated-website-blurs-line-between-newsroom-and-political-advertiser/ (2021).
Fischer, S. Facebook says new pre-election political ad rules apply to boosted posts. Axios https://www.axios.com/facebook-says-new-pre-election-political-ad-rules-apply-to-boosted-posts-ecbab5e5-48c8-42b4-878f-aa7581ed6186.html (2020).
2020: Working America. Fight for a Better America (2020). https://www.fightforbetter.org/workingamerica
Levy, A., Rodriguez, S. & Graham, M. Why political campaigns are flooding facebook with ad dollars. CNBC https://www.cnbc.com/2020/10/08/trump-biden-pacs-spend-big-on-facebook-as-election-nears.html (2020).
R Core Development Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2022).
Wickham, H. et al. Welcome to the tidyverse. J. Open Source Softw. 4, 1686 (2019).
Blair, G., Cooper, J., Coppock, A., Humphreys, M. & Sonnet, L. estimatr: Fast estimators for design-based inference. R package version 0.30.6 https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=estimatr (2022).
Fox, J. & Weisberg, S. An R Companion to Applied Regression 3rd edn (Sage, 2019).
Acknowledgements
We thank D. Green, F. Sävje, M. Michelson, B. Sinclair, E. Porter, L. Vavreck, Y. Velez, J. Kalla and D. Broockman for generous early feedback. We also thank E. Franklin Fowler and the Wesleyan Media Project for generously sharing data. We received no specific external funding for this work.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
S.M. and J.B. conceived of the initial research. S.M. designed and implemented the original experiment. A.B. and H.H. administered the experiment. D.F., M.A., K.Z., S.Z., J.A. and S.M. contributed to data collection and curation. M.A., J.A. and A.C. analysed the results of the experiment. A.C. and S.M. drafted the initial manuscript and figures. All authors contributed to the revision and editing of the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
All researchers were employed by Acronym or were contractors thereof during the 2020 election cycle. J.A., S.Z. and H.H. have a substantial financial interest in Facebook. Acronym played a role in conceptualizing and designing the study presented here: all of the authors except A.C. contributed to the design and/or implementation of the experiment while employed by Acronym from January 2020 to January 2021. After the termination of their employment in January 2021, but before working on this manuscript, the other primary authors signed explicit third-party data-sharing agreements on 8 February 2021 to allow data access while they conducted the scientific analysis and reporting presented here, with other institutions. Acronym and the authors agreed in writing to the publication of this manuscript in advance of manuscript analysis and preparation. The authors agreed to provide Acronym with a draft of the manuscript before publication.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Human Behaviour thanks the anonymous reviewers for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Supplementary Information
Matching of treatment voters to Facebook users, a description of the advertisement content, a description of the advertisement testing programme, analysis of the overall Facebook advertisement environment, an overview of the PAP, PAP deviations, randomization code and Supplementary Tables 1–3 and Figs. 1–4.
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
About this article
Cite this article
Aggarwal, M., Allen, J., Coppock, A. et al. A 2 million-person, campaign-wide field experiment shows how digital advertising affects voter turnout. Nat Hum Behav 7, 332–341 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01487-4
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01487-4