Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Homophily and acrophily as drivers of political segregation

Abstract

Political segregation is an important social problem, increasing polarization and impeding effective governance. Previous work has viewed the central driver of segregation to be political homophily, the tendency to associate with others who have similar views. Here we propose that, in addition to homophily, people’s social tie decisions are driven by political acrophily, the tendency to associate with others who have more extreme political views (rather than more moderate). We examined this using a paradigm in which participants share emotions and attitudes on political policies, observe others’ responses and choose which others to affiliate with. In four studies (N = 1,235), both liberal and conservative participants’ social tie decisions reflected the presence of acrophily. We found that participants who viewed peers who expressed more extreme views as more prototypical of their political group also tended to engage in greater acrophily. These studies identify a previously overlooked tendency in tie formation.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Three hypothetical tie selection strategies.
Fig. 2: An outline of a trial in the choice condition.
Fig. 3: Results from study 2 (n = 213, experimental condition).
Fig. 4: Results from study 3.
Fig. 5: Task and results of study 4.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

Data for study 1 are available here: https://osf.io/nz4dk/. Data for studies 2–4 are available here: https://osf.io/649fq/. Data for the agent-based model are available here: https://osf.io/ad7vh/.

Code availability

Code for study 1 is available here: https://osf.io/nz4dk/. Code for studies 2–4 is available here: https://osf.io/649fq/. Code for the agent-based model is available here: https://osf.io/ad7vh/.

References

  1. Brown, J. R. & Enos, R. D. The measurement of partisan sorting for 180 million voters. Nat. Hum. Behav. 5, 998–1008 (2021).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Finkel, E. J. et al. Political sectarianism in America. Science 370, 533–536 (2020).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Bishop, B. The Big Sort: Why the Clustering of Like-Minded America Is Tearing Us Apart (Mariner Books, 2009).

  4. Iyengar, S., Lelkes, Y., Levendusky, M., Malhotra, N. & Westwood, S. J. The origins and consequences of affective polarization in the United States. Annu. Rev. Polit. Sci. 22, 1–35 (2018).

    Google Scholar 

  5. McPherson, M., Smith-Llovin, L. & Cook, J. M. Birds of a feather: homophily in social networks. Annu. Rev. Sociol 27, 415–444 (2001).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Dehghani, M. et al. Purity homophily in social networks. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 145, 366–375 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Kossinets, G. & Watts, D. J. Origins of homophily in an evolving social network. Am. J. Sociol. 115, 405–450 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Halberstam, Y. & Knight, B. Homophily, group size, and the diffusion of political information in social networks: evidence from Twitter. J. Public Econ. 143, 73–88 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Huber, G. A. & Malhotra, N. Political homophily in social relationships: evidence from online dating behavior. J. Polit. 79, 269–283 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Diprete, T. A., Gelman, A., Mccormick, T., Teitler, J. & Zheng, T. Segregation in social networks based on acquaintanceship and trust. Am. J. Sociol. 116, 1234–1283 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Brady, W. J., Wills, J. A., Jost, J. T., Tucker, J. A. & Van Bavel, J. J. Emotion shapes the diffusion of moralized content in social networks. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 7313–7318 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Boutyline, A. & Willer, R. The social structure of political echo chambers: variation in ideological homophily in online networks. Polit. Psychol. 38, 551–569 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Morrison, K. R. & Miller, D. T. Distinguishing between silent and vocal minorities: not all deviants feel marginal. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 94, 871–882 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Abrams, D., Bown, N., Marques, J. M. & Henson, M. Pro-norm and anti-norm deviance within and between groups. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 906–912 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Abrams, D., Marques, J., Bown, N. & Dougill, M. Anti-norm and pro-norm deviance in the bank and on the campus: two experiments on subjective group dynamics. Gr. Process. Intergr. Relations 5, 163–182 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Kulibert, D., Moss, A., Appleby, J. & O’Brien, L. Perceptions of political deviants: a lay theory of subjective group dynamics. Preprint at PsyArXiv https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/aq652 (2021).

  17. Zimmerman, F., Garbulsky, G., Ariely, D., Sigman, M. & Navajas, J. Political coherence and certainty as drivers of interpersonal liking over and above similarity. Sci. Adv. 8, eabk1909 (2022).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Goldenberg, A., Sweeny, T. D., Shpigel, E. & Gross, J. J. Is this my group or not? The role of ensemble coding of emotional expressions in group categorization. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 149, 445–460 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Hogg, M. A. & Adelman, J. Uncertainty-identity theory: extreme groups, radical behavior, and authoritarian leadership. J. Soc. Issues 69, 436–454 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Rabinowitz, G. & Macdonals, S. E. A directional theory of issue voting. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 83, 93–121 (1989).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Bischof, D. & Wagner, M. Do voters polarize when radical parties enter parliament? Am. J. Pol. Sci. 63, 888–904 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Tomz, M. & Van Houweling, R. P. Candidate positioning and voter choice. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 102, 303–318 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Lacy, D. & Paolino, P. Testing proximity versus directional voting using experiments. Elect. Stud. 29, 460–471 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Gallati, L. & Giger, N. Proximity and directional voting: testing for the region of acceptability. Elect. Stud. 64, 102024 (2020).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Dion, K. L., Baron, R. S. & Miller, N. Why do groups make riskier decisions than individuals? Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 5, 305–377 (1970).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Cartwright, D. Determinants of scientific progress: the case of research on the risky shift. Am. Psychol. 28, 222–231 (1973).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Moscovici, S. & Zavalloni, M. The group as a polarizer of attitudes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 12, 125–135 (1969).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Myers, D. G. & Bishop, G. Discussion effects on racial attitudes. ScienceScience 169, 778–779 (1970).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Lord, C. G., Ross, L. & Lepper, M. R. Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: the effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 37, 2098–2109 (1979).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Myers, D. G. & Lamm, H. The group polarization phenomenon. Psychol. Bull. 83, 602–627 (1976).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Westfall, J., Judd, C. M. & Kenny, D. A. Replicating studies in which samples of participants respond to samples of stimuli. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 390–399 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Skinner, M. & Stephenson, G. M. The effects of intergroup comparison on the polarization of opinions. Curr. Psychol. Res. 1, 49–59 (1981).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Packer, D. J. On being both with us and against us: a normative conflict model of dissent in social groups. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 12, 50–72 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Jetten, J. & Hornsey, M. J. Deviance and dissent in groups. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 65, 461–485 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Hornsey, M. J. & Jetten, J. The individual within the group: balancing the need to belong with the need to be different. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 8, 220–247 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Goldenberg, A. et al. Beyond emotional similarity: the role of situation specific motives. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 149, 138–159 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Robinson, R. J., Keltner, D., Ward, A. & Ross, L. Actual versus assumed differences in construal: ‘naive realism’ in intergroup perception and conflict. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 68, 404–417 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Ahler, D. J. & Sood, G. The parties in our heads: misperceptions about party composition and their consequences. J. Polit. 80, 964–981 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Levendusky, M. S. & Malhotra, N. (Mis)perceptions of partisan polarization in the American public. Public Opin. Q. 80, 378–391 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Van Boven, L., Ehret, P. J. & Sherman, D. K. Psychological barriers to bipartisan public support for climate policy. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 13, 492–507 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Lau, T., Morewedge, C. K. & Cikara, M. Overcorrection for social-categorization information moderates impact bias in affective forecasting. Psychol. Sci. 27, 1340–1351 (2016). 0956797616660292.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Koller, M. robustlmm: an R package for robust estimation of linear mixed-effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 75, 1–24 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Batson, C. D. et al. Anger at unfairness: is it moral outrage? Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 37, 1272–1285 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Brady, W. J., Crockett, M. & Bavel, J. J. V. The MAD model of moral contagion: the role of motivation, attention, and design in the spread of moralized content online. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 15, 978–1010 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Jordan, J. J. & Rand, D. G. Signaling when no one is watching: a reputation heuristics account of outrage and punishment in one-shot anonymous interactions. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 118, 57–88 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. van Kleef, G. A. How emotions regulate social life the emotions as social information (EASI) model. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 18, 184–188 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Panda, A., Siddarth, D. & Pal, J. COVID, BLM, and the polarization of US politicians on Twitter. Preprint at arXiv https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2008.03263 (2020).

  48. Costello, T. H. & Bowes, S. M. Absolute certainty and political ideology: a systematic test of curvilinearity. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. https://doi.org/10.1177/19485506211070410 (2022).

  49. van Prooijen, J. W. & Krouwel, A. P. M. Psychological features of extreme political ideologies. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 28, 159–163 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Van Der Does, T., Galesic, M., Dunivin, Z. O. & Smaldino, P. E. Strategic identity signaling in heterogeneous networks. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 119, 1–10 (2022).

    Google Scholar 

  51. Schweighofer, S., Schweitzer, F. & Garcia, D. A weighted balance model of opinion hyperpolarization. J. Artif. Soc. Soc. Simul. 23, 1 (2020).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Roccas, S., Klar, Y. & Liviatan, I. The paradox of group-based guilt: modes of national identification, conflict vehemence, and reactions to the in-group’s moral violations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 698–711 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Leary, M. R., Kelly, K. M., Cottrell, C. A. & Schreindorfer, L. S. Construct validity of the need to belong scale: mapping the nomological network. J. Pers. Assess. 95, 610–624 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J. & Swann, W. B. A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. J. Res. Pers. 37, 504–528 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Haddock, G., Zanna, M. P. & Esses, V. M. Assessing the structure of prejudicial attitudes: the case of attitudes toward homosexuals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 65, 1105–1118 (1993).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank M. Reyes, N. Ebeid and N. Hunt for their assistance in running the lab studies, V. Puri for her assistance in developing the simulations and processing the peer information, and A. Boutyline and J. Jordan for their comments on the research. J.S. is supported in part by the German Academic Scholarship Foundation (Promotionsförderung der Studienstiftung des deutschen Volkes). The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

A.G., J.J.G., E.H., R.W. and J.M.A. conceived and designed the experiments. A.G., Z.H., J.S. and D.B. ran the experiments. A.G. and J.M.A. analysed studies 1–4. A.G. wrote the paper, and E.H., R.W., J.M.A. and J.J.G. were involved in reviewing and editing the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Amit Goldenberg.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature Human Behaviour thanks Stephen Vaisey, Simon Schweighofer and Joshua Tucker for their contribution to the peer review of this work.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary information file.

Reporting Summary

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Goldenberg, A., Abruzzo, J.M., Huang, Z. et al. Homophily and acrophily as drivers of political segregation. Nat Hum Behav 7, 219–230 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01474-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01474-9

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing