Abstract
Intense sociality has been a catalyst for human culture and civilization, and our social relationships at a personal level play a pivotal role in our health and well-being. These relationships are, however, sensitive to the time we invest in them. To understand how and why this should be, we first outline the evolutionary background in primate sociality from which our human social world has emerged. We then review defining features of that human sociality, putting forward a framework within which one can understand the consequences of mass social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic, including mental health deterioration, stress, sleep disturbance and substance misuse. We outline recent research on the neural basis of prolonged social isolation, highlighting especially higher-order neural circuits such as the default mode network. Our survey of studies covers the negative effects of prolonged social deprivation and the multifaceted drivers of day-to-day pandemic experiences.
Similar content being viewed by others
Main
Humans, like all anthropoid primates, are intensely social. There is now considerable evidence that interindividual differences in social embeddedness affect a variety of health and fitness indicators. In humans, the single best predictor of physical health and well-being, as well as future longevity, is the number and quality of close friendships, with the more conventional suspects (such as diet, obesity, alcohol consumption and air quality) ranking a distant second1,2. Indeed, the frequency of social engagement predicts psychological health and well-being3, self-rated feelings of happiness, satisfaction with life, and trust in one’s local community4.
The COVID-19 lockdowns of the past two years were a global stress test—large-scale social deprivation in a more dramatic extent and form than ever before in recorded history. At the peak of public health restrictions, >3.6 billion people worldwide were subject to government-imposed stay-at-home orders. On the individual scale, we know that we respond poorly to isolation. However, existing psychological and neuroscience research had little to say about the possible consequences of mass isolation. By contrast, there have been many large-scale epidemiological studies of the effects of social deprivation in the elderly5. Almost all of these investigations yielded evidence for detrimental effects on cognitive capacity, psychological and physical well-being, and even longevity. It is clear that the chronic experience of social isolation escalates the risk of depression and dementias, as well as cardiovascular disease and certain types of cancer6,7,8.
The present review provides a frame of reference that can help to situate current and future findings on mass social isolation by incorporating established knowledge on human sociality and its underlying neurobiological mechanisms. To this end, we first place human sociality within the broader context of anthropoid primate sociality, with its behavioural and neurobiological determinants. Our aim is to provide a more grounded explanation as to why the neurobiology of human sociality takes the form it does. We then survey some of the unfolding evidence with direct relevance to the neurobiological and psychological consequences of the large-scale lockdown during COVID-19 and subsequent social rehabilitation.
The neurobiology of primate sociality
Anthropoid primates exhibit a form of bonded sociality that is rarely encountered among other mammals or birds9,10. Primate sociality typically involves stable social groups based on dyadic relationships that can last a lifetime. These modes of interaction are reinforced by specific behaviours such as social grooming11 and involve constant visual monitoring of social partners12 that are the context for the regular exchange of social support13. In both humans and the most intensely social non-human primates (including pair-bonded species such as titi and owl monkeys, and the baboons, macaques and great apes), these relationships are characterized by a level of emotional intensity that is reflected in an apparent desire to be in close spatial proximity to the social partner12,14. Just as in humans, close relationships with other individuals are known to provide substantial health benefits: individuals who have more grooming partners recover faster from injuries, live longer, are more fertile and have offspring that are more likely to survive to reproductive age.
In anthropoid primates, social relationships of this kind are formed and maintained by a dual-process mechanism that exploits two separate neural systems in the brain15. One is an emotional (or ‘raw feels’16) component built on the endorphin system, mediated by social grooming (as a form of social ‘soft touch’), which may be closely related to brain correlates of social support in humans (see below). The other component is a more explicitly self-aware form of higher-level cognition17 that involves developing a conceptualized understanding of a relationship, which may be closely related to brain correlates of loneliness in humans (see below). The first neural system creates a psychopharmacological environment that incentivizes two bonded individuals to stay together over time. This in turn allows them to build relationships of trust, obligation and reciprocity via the second neural system. Other neurotransmitters (for example, serotonin and dopamine) and neurohormones (for example, oxytocin and testosterone) play important roles in the management of behaviour18. However, it is possible that none contribute to the formation of bonded relationships in the specific way that endorphins do19,20,21,22. If true, this may largely be a consequence of the fact that endorphins have a much longer half-life than the other chemicals22.
In primates, but not other avian and mammal orders, social group size is tied to brain volume (the social brain hypothesis)10,23,24. This evolutionary perspective implies that the cognitive demands of maintaining stable social groups become a function of expanding group size. An increasing cognitive cost was incurred as long-term bonded relationships emerged25—as evidenced in birds, for example, where species characterized by lifelong pair-bonds have larger brains than species that form new breeding pairs each year and those that mate promiscuously10. More importantly, neuroimaging studies of both humans26,27,28,29,30,31 and cercopithecine primates32,33,34 have shown that interindividual differences in the number of friends (or living group size) is associated with the volume of the higher associative brain circuits. Such evidence implies that the social brain relationship applies within species as well as between species.
Bonded sociality is enabled by a set of cognitive skills including mentalizing, empathy, self-control, causal reasoning and one-trial rule learning. These mental capacities, especially well developed in humans, allow us to manage several relationships simultaneously24. Probably unique to anthropoid primates35, these cognitive skills involve specialized neural circuits in the brain, in particular the perspective-taking-related default network24,36,37,38. A proto-form of that macroscopic brain system has been identified in other primates, where it also underpins sociality39,40,41,42,43,44.
In addition to these new requirements on the brain’s ‘hardware’ infrastructure, learning through experience plays a crucial role. Relationships change over time as individuals fall in and out of favour with each other45. This makes it impossible to legislate for all possible social contexts with hardwired behavioural responses of the kind that we find in the simpler social systems of many non-primate animals46. Equally, conventional associative learning would be too slow and inflexible. Instead, anthropoid primates operate by a set of learned rules (many involving complex social trade-offs in which the values of relationships with different individuals are compared) that can be applied in contexts where the costs and benefits can vary widely over time as social and ecological circumstances change. To provide scope for social learning, primates and especially humans require a prolonged adolescence during which socialization continuously sharpens the skills instrumental for social interaction. In primates, species that have bigger neocortices tend to have longer maturation periods47. Both developmental and neuroimaging studies suggest that, in humans, this critical period of the life cycle can occupy as much as the first two and a half decades of life48,49, coinciding with the completion of frontal lobe expansion and neural maturation as indexed by growth patterns50 and axon myelination51,52.
In anthropoid primates (including humans), the process of social bonding is bolstered by the endorphin-mediating parts of the central nervous system19,21,22,53,54,55,56,57,58. The hand actions used in grooming trigger the upregulation of endorphins in the brain20,59,60, mediated via the highly specialized afferent C-tactile neural system61,62. Aside from the social benefits created by the endorphin system, there is evidence to suggest that endorphins upregulate the immune system for better host defence, in particular by mobilizing natural killer cells63,64,65. Endorphins are also upregulated by exercise, with consequent benefits for the immune system66,67. Periods of scarce sociality and/or reduced physical activity may well lead to compensatory behaviours, such as increased use of substitute stimulants (for example, alcohol and nicotine), hinting at mechanistic pathways accompanying health deterioration.
Humans exploit this endorphin mechanism through the many forms of casual ‘soft touch’, such as hugging, stroking and caressing, that are woven into our modes of social interaction68,69. These bonding mechanisms heavily tax our time budgets and involve the investment of a considerable proportion of the waking day. One study of human friendship formation estimated that it takes around 200 hours of face-to-face contact to turn a stranger into a close friend70. More importantly, friendships require high frequencies of contact to maintain their emotional quality (for example, a minimum of half an hour per day for intimate friends71,72); otherwise, relationship quality decays steeply over time45,73. These patterns of investment appear to describe a social fingerprint that is both unique to the individual and stable over time73,74. In the absence of opportunity for social exchange, we might expect to see both turnover in social networks and changes in the way individuals think about the social world (as opposed to the non-social environment).
In primates, social grooming is so time-intensive that group size is limited to a maximum of ~50 individuals if stable cohesion is to be maintained11,75. Humans have been able to live in larger groups by supplementing the touch-based bonding mechanism of primates with other explicitly social behaviours that also turn out to trigger the endorphin system but are more time-efficient because they allow several individuals to be ‘groomed’ at the same time. These behaviours include laughter76,77, singing78,79,80, dancing81, religious rituals82, feasting83,84 and emotional storytelling85. Close relationships, however, depend on the more intimate processes of touch and face-to-face contact68,69,71. While the behavioural and health-related impacts of social isolation may result from the breakdown of social exchange in the close inner circles, failure to engage in the social activities that we use for the more peripheral relationships in the outer layers of our social networks may nonetheless have measurable consequences.
In sum, social interaction patterns in humans are underpinned by long-standing primate mechanisms, supplemented by novel forms of social interaction that make up a specialized human social toolkit. These are expensive to maintain, both neurophysiologically and in terms of time investment. Failure to invest results in the rapid decline of relationship quality and loss of the associated benefits, leading to increased psychological and physical ill health.
The structure of our social world
Our friendships and family relationships do not consist of isolated dyads. Rather, these ties form an interwoven network of relationships that spread out around us in a social space that is only partly mapped onto the physical space around us. Understanding the structure and dynamics of this social world provides an important framework that helps explain some of the consequences of social isolation.
We do not treat all the members of our social world equally. Rather, our social networks and communities have a distinctive fractal structure, forming a series of ever-widening layers around us86 (Fig. 1). These layers are defined both by the frequency with which we contact individual members and by the emotional closeness we feel to them71. The social layers in Fig. 1 have very specific sizes, with a scaling ratio close to 3 (refs. 86,87,88), such that each layer is three times the size of the layer immediately inside it. Note that each of these layers includes both extended family members and conventional friends, although the outermost layers are largely populated by acquaintances. For convenience, however, we follow the common practice of referring to them collectively as ‘friends’68 without distinction, unless we specify otherwise. A similar layered structure, with exactly the same numerical values of network layer sizes, is found in both the social networks and the group sizes of other primates24,89,90.
This pattern reflects a combination of the choices that we make about investing in individual relationships as a function of the benefits that they provide for us91,92 when the time available for focused social interaction is inevitably limited93,94,95. In part, this is a direct consequence of the fact that, in both anthropoid primates71 and humans72,94, the strength of a friendship, and hence the expectation that it will provide various essential services, directly reflects the time invested in it96. This patterning in how we distribute our effort is responsible for the layered effect, with each layer associated with a characteristic frequency of contact for each member71. If we drop below this frequency of contact to an individual, they will, over a period of just a few months, slide out through the circles until they settle into the layer with the new lower frequency of contact55. The innermost five layers in Fig. 1 appear to be the ones that have the greatest effect on our health and well-being: they consist of our most reliable family and friend relationships in terms of willingness to provide emotional, social, financial and other forms of help97.
The average size of human personal social networks is 150 individuals86. However, there are notable interindividual differences that reflect gender and personality71. Women typically have larger inner layers than men do, even though the total network size remains similar. This is commensurate with women’s often better performance on key social cognitive abilities such as putting oneself into others’ shoes71. In part, this typical advantage reflects the fact that women’s relationships are more emotionally intense, more focused and thus also more fragile (that is, prone to fracture) than those of men, whose relationships have a more casual, club-like quality71,98,99,100. Similarly, extraverts typically have larger social networks than introverts. Because extraverts distribute their available social time more widely, their average emotional closeness to network members is lower93,101. Age also has a dramatic effect on the structure of our social networks. Network size increases with age up to around 250 people in the mid-20s, retrenches to a stable 150 from the later 20s to the late 60s and then declines from the 70s onwards102. The increase during adolescence reflects the gradual refinement of perspective-taking and other social cognitive skills that enable us to handle more relationships103; the decline into old age largely reflects the failure to replace friendships that have been lost.
Some of these differences between people can be traced to brain substrates. Attachment style, for example, is closely linked to endorphin receptor density, notably in the frontal part of the higher association cortex57. This is reflected in the fact that those individuals with a cooler attachment style tend to have fewer friends than those with a warmer style104. Similarly, analysis of the topology of immigrants’ social networks provides clear evidence that the limited access to a pool of potential friends limits the size of personal networks and thus alters network topology94.
On the basis of these earlier findings, we anticipate that prolonged social isolation will lead to an elevated sense of social disconnection as the felt distance to one’s contacts widens with time. Those whom we normally see the most often (the members of the innermost circles in Fig. 1) will incur the greatest sense of loss. In addition, the known sources of interindividual variation in social style predict that sex/gender as well as other identity and diversity factors will influence the impact that social isolation has on different strata of society. For example, given the findings we summarize above, we expect more acute consequences of the recent COVID-19 lockdown in women (with their typically more intense relationships) than in men (with their often more casual social ties). Similarly, and perhaps more obviously, we would expect extraverts to find social isolation more frustrating than individuals with more introverted personalities. With regard to age, we might identify two periods of vulnerability: adolescence (when social skills are being honed and relationships developed) and old age (when social networks are naturally prone to contraction).
The emerging neurobiology of social isolation
At the brain level, the experience of social isolation has been found to exhibit manifestations (Fig. 2) especially in the prefrontal cortex and other parts of the higher association cortex in the wider human population37,105. Among all macroscopic brain circuits, the default network—known to represent information content about oneself as well as other people’s thoughts and likely intentions106,107—exhibits by far the strongest grey matter volume associations with perceived social isolation (that is, loneliness)37. With regard to intrinsic connectivity measures, functional coupling inside this major brain network was upregulated in lonely individuals. By contrast, the default network was functionally decoupled from the visual system in perceived social isolation. These findings were interpreted as reflecting the known tendency for internally generated thoughts to increase in individuals who feel socially isolated, including anthropomorphizing their pets, developing intense relationships with TV characters and being overly reminiscent about past social exchanges with others.
In support of shifts in brain correlates commonly related to thinking about other individuals, an analysis of 48 white matter tracts revealed a unique association between loneliness and the fornix37. This core fibre tract transports neural signals via axonal connections from the hippocampus towards the medial default network. The microstructure of this unidirectional hippocampus output pathway is known to vary between individuals as a function of episodic memory traits, such as vividness of recall, which would be needed to recall past social events or imagine future ones. In a targeted co-analysis of 38 microanatomically defined hippocampal subfields of these allocortical and neocortical regions108, it was especially the hippocampal CA1 and the stress-susceptible molecular layer that showed differences in structural covariation with specific medial prefrontal and posteromedial partner regions of the default network midline in the subjective perception of social isolation (that is, loneliness; Fig. 3). In the monkey brain, hippocampal CA1 neurons have direct neuron-to-neuron axon connections to the medial prefrontal cortex through the fornix white matter pathway—pyramidal CA1 neurons being the only hippocampal sites sending axons directly to cortical partners109,110. Notably, these same neural circuits have been implicated in the primary biology of Alzheimer’s disease as well as the retrieval and richness of episodic memory—linked to clinical hallmarks of that major neurodegenerative condition. The neurobiology of loneliness therefore appears to bear a close relation to the alteration of internally generated dimensions of cognition, including mental simulation of others’ thoughts and reactions of the kind that would be needed when we communicate with others, such as when texting people (for example, on Facebook or WhatsApp-type messengers) or when calling them on the phone. This offers a likely explanation for the associated differences in covariation between the hippocampus and the default network.
Loneliness is increasingly distinguished from more objective measures of social isolation, especially social support, which we here defined as the frequency of social contact (as directly measurable as days per week). Individuals may not subjectively feel isolated (that is, loneliness) but may still report a lack of regular social contact with other people (that is, weak social support), and vice versa. People typically invest as much as ~40% of their social effort in their ‘support network’—the innermost circle of about five family members and close friends from whom they receive the most social, emotional and economic help71. In contrast to the neural signature of loneliness, interindividual differences in (objective) social support are related to characteristic volume signatures in the salience and limbic networks26,27,28.
Regions of other higher neural circuits, especially the salience network, have repeatedly been shown to be closely involved in processing others’ emotions and monitoring one’s own interoceptive states107. Implemented shared representations thus underlie empathy—our ability to ‘click in’ emotionally with other people and mimic their emotional states, especially when engaged in similar or identical processes during direct face-to-face contact of an affective state and when witnessing someone else in that state88. According to demographic profiling and genome-wide analyses, these neural circuits, related to the social interaction frequency with close others, were implicated in factors associated with poor health and substance misuse, including smoking and alcohol consumption, as well as overall sadness and vulnerability to stress108,111. Reinforcing a difference in scarce social isolation with close versus more casual acquaintances, previous brain-imaging experiments have reported neural activity responses in these regions when humans think about familiar members of their social circle, such as friends as opposed to strangers112.
Of potential relevance for public health decision-making and intervention, these same neural circuits were highlighted in a seminal longitudinal study on the social brain that administered daily exercises of emotional sharing with others to several hundred participants113. This regular empathic engagement mediated adaptive increases in grey matter structure, which included the insula and the mid and posterior cingulate cortex. Neural plasticity changes induced by day-to-day social engagements coincided with improvements in behavioural assessments of social and emotional skills113. Thus, across-species (see above) and within-species (this paragraph) evidence speaks to the flexible changes of brain architecture as a function of regular face-to-face social exchange.
Consequences of mass social isolation in COVID-19
Various emerging studies carried out under COVID-19 lockdown suggest that social distancing has had considerable psychological and behavioural consequences. These include elevated levels of anxiety and depression, deterioration of mental health, changes in diet and increased suicidal ideation, in addition to less physical activity and higher levels of experienced loneliness. Importantly, there have been considerable interindividual differences, with some people experiencing limited effects, but others experiencing more serious consequences. The prevalence of reported loneliness was higher, for example, among those who were self-isolating alone and unemployed, and lower in people who were married or living with a partner114. Rates of loneliness were twice as high among people who met clinical criteria for depression, anxiety and post-traumatic stress disorder than they were in the rest of the population. Greater difficulties with emotion regulation and poorer sleep quality were also commonly associated with loneliness. On average, older people were found to show greater satisfaction with life, higher levels of social cohesion, less concern about family members contracting COVID-19 and lower levels of self-reported panic, depression, emotional disturbances and risk perception115.
Best and colleagues115 also showed evidence that, compared with men, women experienced more overall distress and higher levels of panic, depression, emotional disturbances and concerns about contracting COVID-19 and about the safety of their family—a finding in line with the generally greater intimacy of women’s friendships (see above). Individuals who relied on credible sources of information, such as mainstream news, employer-based information and public health announcements, reported less overall COVID-19 distress, panic, depression and emotional disturbance, as well as greater satisfaction with life and social cohesion115. Various physical factors were also identified. These effects might be exacerbated by confinement-induced decreases in physical activity levels and increases in sedentary behaviour, for example116. Such a lifestyle has been shown to precipitate rapid deterioration of cardiovascular health and premature death, especially through transitory ischemic attacks and strokes117.
One seminal study118 used an innovative experience-sampling approach to investigate the naturally occurring changes in everyday cognition before and during the first COVID-19 lockdown in the UK (Fig. 4). This naturalistic experiment is based on the notion of ‘mindwandering’, a form of daydreamed, spontaneous thought that is typically unrelated to the ambient environment and known to vary along several dimensions such as vividness, content and time direction to past/present/future. These sometimes random, sometimes purposeful internally paced episodic mental scenes tend to occur especially commonly when people are not actively focused on a mental task—which makes experience sampling at random time points during the day a particularly attractive research tool.
The participants’ ongoing thoughts and the contexts in which they occurred (for example, social environment, activity and location) were randomly sampled in real time over five days. Future-directed problem-solving was lower during lockdown, especially in older individuals (55+ years old) and in those who engaged in regular real-world interaction with others. Yet, this was less prominent when participants were interacting physically as opposed to virtually. Before the first lockdown was imposed, younger participants (18–35 years old) reported engaging in less vivid imagery during introspection when they were alone than when they were interacting with others. But during lockdown, the younger participants reported more vivid imagery when they were alone than when they were interacting with others. However, the kinds of episodic social cognition normally observed in interactive environments were greater during lockdown, this being more apparent across all forms of interaction when compared with not interacting at all. Detailed task focus was more apparent when interacting virtually than in other contexts.
Taken together, this behavioural comparison of moment-to-moment spontaneous thoughts before versus during COVID-19 suggests that the lockdown led to notable changes in ongoing thought patterns in daily life and that these changes were associated with changes to daily routines before versus after the public health restrictions were in place, to which individuals may have conformed to varying degrees. In particular, the naturalistic thought probes have characterized how social encounters are an important influence on our day-to-day thinking. Shifting opportunities for regular social contact following the stay-at-home orders are likely to have changed the expression of people’s ongoing in-the-moment thought tendencies.
It is clear that COVID-19 has also taken a heavy toll in terms of mental health, especially for some strata in the wider population. Robb and colleagues119 investigated the psychological effects of COVID-19 lockdown on 7,127 cognitively healthy older adults (mean age, 70.7; s.d., 7.4) from the UK. The authors found an association between subjective loneliness and increased indicators of depression and anxiety following lockdown. More than half of the respondents reported feeling lonely, and a quarter lived alone. These proportions were higher in women than in men. Around 12.3% and 12.8% of the respondents reported increased symptoms of anxiety and depression, respectively, during lockdown. Again, these proportions were higher for women than for men. Compared with those who never felt lonely, individuals reporting that they often felt lonely had 11- and 17-times-higher odds of feeling more anxiety and depression, respectively, during lockdown. Compared with men, lonely women were twice as likely to report worse symptoms of depression, whereas lonely men were around 5% more likely to report greater anxiety than females. Those who lived alone were more likely to report feeling worse on components of anxiety and depression than those living with others. Compared with individuals who reported daily contact, those reporting two to six occasions of online social contact per week had a 19% lower risk of feeling greater anxiety. These outcomes are broadly in line with what our review of the dynamics of human social networks (see above) would lead us to expect.
Interestingly, remote contact with friends or family via technology did not significantly alter the risk of reporting feeling worse on components of depression. Robb and colleagues119 reasoned that “Wider access to technology may help buffer loneliness and isolation that lead to worsened mental health. Older people, however, are more likely to have limited ability to access technology, most likely representing the more vulnerable of this demographic … As in-person intervention strategies during pandemics may be limited or impossible, the use of technologies, such as apps, may remain an important tool, albeit limited by the digital divide, thus potentially excluding significant numbers of particularly vulnerable older people.”
Adding further detail to the reported effects of social isolation from social distancing during COVID-19 in a pre-existing, longitudinal UK cohort119, in males, smokers were more likely to report greater depression than non-smokers, with no association observed among females. In women, a three-unit increase in alcohol consumption per week (approximately one large glass of wine) was associated with a 22% lower probability of reporting an improvement in components of depression. Additionally, as many as 40% of the cohort reported sleep disturbances, possibly because worry and ruminating thoughts provoke cognitive arousal and may disturb stress-related cortisol homeostasis, resulting in poorer sleep. However, as an important caveat, Robb and colleagues did not have pre-pandemic anxiety or depression measures to compare against the ones acquired during physical distancing mandates, which weakens causal attribution. Those reporting poor sleep at least three times per week had eight- and seven-times-higher odds of reporting worse symptoms of anxiety and depression, respectively. Robb and colleagues119 also found that those who were single/widowed/divorced and/or who lived alone were also at increased risk of reporting worse symptoms of depression and anxiety following COVID-19 lockdown, especially men. Indeed, being widowed or divorced as a risk factor for worse mental health has been reported in COVID-19 cohort studies in Spain (n = 3,055)120 and China (n = 1,060)121, although these were younger cohorts. Overall, women were more likely to report worse components of anxiety and depression than men, with this observation replicated in different age groups and in different countries, including Demark, Spain, Italy, Turkey and Iran119.
In a comparison of outcomes before and during the pandemic at the psychological and behavioural levels, Niedzwiedz and colleagues122 analysed data from almost 10,000 adults in the longitudinal arm of a nationally representative sample (the UK Household Longitudinal Study). In addition to increased regular and binge alcohol consumption, psychological distress rose, especially in women and young adults generally, as well as in the Asian community and among individuals with a degree education. Robinson and colleagues123 conducted a systematic meta-analysis of 65 longitudinal studies and documented an increase in mental health burden, particularly in the months after lockdown, especially in depressive symptoms, anxiety and mood disorder symptoms in American, European and other samples. Several of these changes during COVID-19 were especially pronounced in individuals with pre-existing physical conditions, which the authors interpreted as likely to reflect an elevated risk of infection. Moreover, Zaninotto and colleagues124, who examined the profiles of >5,000 adults from the English Longitudinal Study of Aging, reported that depression, anxiety, loneliness and poor quality of life increased in the months after the pandemic started. The manifestations were stronger in certain population strata: women, older individuals, and those who live alone or are less affluent. The collective findings from these longitudinal population investigations converged on a major breakdown in mental health and health-related behaviours throughout the COVID-19 pandemic in 2020. Targeted monitoring and psychological interventions are suggested by these authors, especially for more vulnerable parts of the community, including more socially isolated individuals (such as single, widowed and divorced people), who suffered disproportionately.
Only very few longitudinal brain-imaging studies relevant to COVID-19 have been published so far. Douaud and colleagues sampled 782 UK Biobank participants who had been brain-scanned before the pandemic and have been re-scanned during the pandemic125. Of these, 394 had been infected by COVID-19, and 388 acted as matched controls. Brain changes due to the infection itself were linked to a loss of grey matter (with cortical thickness and mean diffusivity measures as proxies to capture tissue changes) in the left parahippocampal gyrus, the left lateral orbitofrontal cortex and the left insula. When looking over the entire cortical surface, these results extended to a set of brain circuits including several nodes of the prefrontal, parietal and temporal default network, in addition to the anterior cingulate cortex and olfactory circuits. These differences in brain correlates of infection were associated with increased cognitive decline, as evidenced by significantly more time required to complete the numeric (A) and alphanumeric (B) version of the Trail Making Test. In addition, a decrease in whole brain volume suggests a diffuse loss of grey matter superimposed onto the more regional effects. In addition to the study’s unique pre- and post-COVID-19 data and careful matching of the patients and controls for age, sex, scan interval and ethnicity, rigorous diagnosis of infection made it possible to test for brain correlates of the disease in a way that was statistically more precise than would have been the case for cross-sectional post-infection differences alone. Although the social consequences of COVID-19 infection were not followed up, the reduced grey matter volume, especially in units of the default network, suggests that a loss of social skills and social network size is a likely outcome, although inferring causal directionality is challenging.
The rare evidence on brain changes in medial-temporal limbic, insula and orbitofrontal cortex seems to bear similarities to longitudinal studies in non-human primates. One brain-imaging study of dopaminergic function compared macaque monkeys housed in social isolation for 1.5 years and during later social housing with other monkeys126. The repercussions of diminished frequency and intensity of social interaction may be traced to dopaminergic neurotransmitter pathways implicated in reward processing, such as in the orbitofrontal cortex. Indeed, after social rehabilitation, less socially interactive males with fewer grooming opportunities displayed hyperactive dopamine responses not present before the isolation condition. The authors concluded that experimentally altering the social richness in the environment led to reward-related neural plasticity effects. The medial-temporal limbic system in turn has been related to plasticity changes in social network sizes in non-human primates, based on regularity of social contact in longitudinal research33. The authors also flagged changes in the prefrontal cortex structure, which may relate to social hierarchy organization that breaks down in times of scarce social interaction or changing social order with lower predictability. Finally, the insula was emphasized in longitudinal research in free-ranging monkeys, and its changes were interpreted to relate to affiliative behaviour and forms of empathy towards others34, instrumental for maintaining the regular grooming social network.
As reflections for future research on social isolation, among the elderly whose social circles are already contracting, we might expect isolation to hasten the rate at which friendships decay and are ultimately lost, resulting in increased susceptibility to mental (for example, cognitive decline, depression and anxiety) and physical (for example, stress, diminished cardiovascular health and impaired immune defence) deterioration over the following decade. Given that most of the social skills needed for the adult world are acquired through peer interaction rather than interactions with adults, we might anticipate some slowdown in the rate of social development in adolescents, possibly resulting in long-term loss in social skills. However, an alternative interpretation might be that the elderly tend to have smaller, more tightly knit, more family-oriented social networks that buffer them against the worst effects of isolation, whereas young adults usually have wider social networks in which peripheral friendships play an important casual role. Viewed from this angle, younger adults would be expected to experience more loss of social capital during the pandemic, with higher costs for mental and physical health. Follow-up studies will be needed to determine which causal sequence is correct. Either way, the population stratum that may be the least likely to be affected by lockdown is young adults: this age group’s strong natural desire to be social whenever possible is likely to result in a rapid re-establishment of normal social interaction patterns.
Social isolation effects linked to social inequality
A final possibility to consider is that social inequality significantly modulates the effects of lockdown. In a data-driven machine-learning study of >17,000 variables describing ~10,000 families in >20 cities across the USA127, social determinants of inequity were found to explain most differences in how children and parents experienced the COVID-19 pandemic, above and beyond other candidate predictors such as pre-existing medical or psychiatric conditions. Sociological factors such as household income, socio-economic status and the experience of racism emerged as the primary correlates of negative pandemic experiences. These effects included increased difficulties with schoolwork among children and concerns over racism among parents. Non-White and Hispanic families faced reduced income and resources and higher likelihoods of financial worry and food insecurity. Yet, these families were the most likely to abide by safe practices such as social distancing and hand washing. By contrast, White families, who typically enjoyed higher pre-pandemic income and presence of a parent with a postgraduate degree, experienced less harsh impacts from COVID-19. These families’ children reported longer nighttime sleep, less difficulty with remote learning for school and less worry about the impact of the pandemic on their family’s stability.
These findings may be analogous to observations from non-human primates, which report that social-isolation-induced plasticity effects in dopaminergic brain function depend on a monkey’s position in the social hierarchy126, with pronounced differences in social rehabilitation in higher- versus lower-ranking individuals. Additionally, lower dominance rank typically reduces the availability of social outlets, which are especially crucial in times of stress and disruption128. Sapolsky argued that lower resilience to encountered stressors escalates the glucocorticoid stress hormone levels and certain disease vulnerabilities128, with declining effectiveness of the immune response and other physiological impacts.
In sum, societal determinants of inequity have emerged as an important driver of negative pandemic experiences, regardless of pre-pandemic functionality. Yip and colleagues127 concluded that community-level, transgenerational intervention strategies may be needed to combat the disproportionate burden of pandemics on racial/ethnic minoritized and marginalized populations. Taken together, these findings have significant implications for child education and society, as well as the relevant public health decisions, in the future.
Conclusion
The human social world is deeply rooted in our primate ancestry. This social world is, however, extremely sensitive to the time we invest in it. Enforced social isolation can easily destabilize its delicate equilibrium. Many of the psychological sequalae of COVID-19 lockdowns are readily understood as resulting from the dislocation of these deeply rooted social processes. Indeed, many of these findings could have been anticipated long before the COVID-19 pandemic. For example, almost one in ten Europeans admitted never meeting friends or family outside of their own household in the course of an entire year, with direct consequences for their psychological and physical health129. Solitary living made up >50% of households in a growing number of metropolitan cities worldwide and has long been thought to be the cause of increasing levels of depression and psychological dystopia130,131. Indeed, aversive feelings of social isolation probably serve as a biological warning signal that alerts individuals to improve their social relationships132.
Three key points emerge from our present assessment. One is that COVID-19 and associated public health restrictions to curb the spread of the virus are likely to have demonstrable mental health and psychosocial ramifications for years to come. This will inevitably place a significant burden on our health systems and societies. The impact may, however, be largely restricted to specific population strata. Older people, for example, are likely to face disproportionately adverse consequences. Worryingly, prolonged social isolation seems to invoke changes in the capacity to visualize internally centred thoughts, especially in younger sub-population. This may presage a switch from an outward to an inward focus that may exacerbate the experience of social isolation in susceptible individuals. The longer-term implications of this are, however, yet to be determined. Second, the experience of undergoing social isolation is known to have significant effects on the structure and function of the hippocampus and default network, long recognized as a primary neural pathway implicated in the pathophysiology of dementia and other major neurodegenerative diseases as well as in effective social function. The fact that these same brain regions turn up in the neuroanatomical consequences of COVID-19 infection is concerning. Our third key point is that social determinants that condition inequality in our societies have strong impacts on lived day-to-day pandemic experiences. This is highlighted by the negative outcomes from COVID-19 for families of lower socio-economic status, single-parent households, and those with racial and ethnic minority backgrounds.
As a note of caution, in our judgement, few datasets or methodological tools exist today to definitively establish causal directionality in many of the population effects we have surveyed in this review. For example, many of the correlative links do not allow us to infer whether loneliness directly causes depression and anxiety, as opposed to already depressed, anxious individuals being more prone to developing loneliness in times of adversity. Similarly, none of the reviewed findings can be used to tease apart whether changes in psychopathology during periods of mass social isolation are the chicken or the egg of the many biological manifestations. To fill knowledge gaps on mediating mechanisms for theoretical models, future research requires carefully designed and controlled longitudinal before-versus-after COVID-19 population investigations.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
References
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T. & Stephenson, D. Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 227–237 (2015).
Yang, Y. C. et al. Social relationships and physiological determinants of longevity across the human life span. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 578–583 (2016).
Santini, Z. I. et al. The moderating role of social network size in the temporal association between formal social participation and mental health: a longitudinal analysis using two consecutive waves of the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 56, 417–428 (2021).
Dunbar, R. I. M. et al. Functional benefits of (modest) alcohol consumption. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 3, 118–133 (2017).
Bzdok, D. & Dunbar, R. I. M. The neurobiology of social distance. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 717–733 (2020).
van Harmelen, A. L. et al. Friendships and family support reduce subsequent depressive symptoms in at-risk adolescents. PLoS ONE 11, e0153715 (2016).
Martín-María, N. et al. Differential impact of transient and chronic loneliness on health status: a longitudinal study. Psychol. Health 35, 177–195 (2020).
Tomova, L. et al. Acute social isolation evokes midbrain craving responses similar to hunger. Nat. Neurosci. 23, 1597–1605 (2020).
Shultz, S. & Dunbar, R. Encephalization is not a universal macroevolutionary phenomenon in mammals but is associated with sociality. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 21582–21586 (2010).
Shultz, S. & Dunbar, R. I. The evolution of the social brain: anthropoid primates contrast with other vertebrates. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 2429–2436 (2007).
Lehmann, J., Korstjens, A. H. & Dunbar, R. I. Group size, grooming and social cohesion in primates. Anim. Behav. 74, 1617–1629 (2007).
Dunbar, R. I. & Shultz, S. Bondedness and sociality. Behaviour 147, 775–803 (2010).
Dunbar, R. Social structure as a strategy to mitigate the costs of group living: a comparison of gelada and guereza monkeys. Anim. Behav. 136, 53–64 (2018).
Aron, A., Aron, E. N. & Smollan, D. Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63, 596 (1992).
Alcala-Lopez, D. et al. Computing the social brain connectome across systems and states. Cereb. Cortex 28, 2207–2232 (2018).
Kirk, R. Raw Feeling: A Philosophical Account of the Essence of Consciousness (Oxford Univ. Press, 1996).
Alcalá-López, D., Vogeley, K., Binkofski, F. & Bzdok, D. Building blocks of social cognition: mirror, mentalize, share? Cortex 118, 4–18 (2018).
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. & Abu-Akel, A. The social salience hypothesis of oxytocin. Biol. Psychiatry 79, 194–202 (2016).
Loseth, G., Ellingsen, D.-M. & Leknes, S. State-dependent µ-opioid modulation of social motivation—a model. Front. Behav. Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00430 (2014).
Nummenmaa, L. et al. Social touch modulates endogenous μ-opioid system activity in humans. NeuroImage 138, 242–247 (2016).
Pearce, E., Wlodarski, R., Machin, A. & Dunbar, R. I. M. Variation in the β-endorphin, oxytocin, and dopamine receptor genes is associated with different dimensions of human sociality. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 5300–5305 (2017).
Dunbar, R. Virtual touch and the human social world. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 43, 14–19 (2022).
Dunbar, R. & Shultz, S. Why are there so many explanations for primate brain evolution? Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 372, 20160244 (2017).
Dunbar, R. I. & Shultz, S. Social complexity and the fractal structure of group size in primate social evolution. Biol. Rev. 96, 1889–1906 (2021).
Shultz, S. & Dunbar, R. I. Social bonds in birds are associated with brain size and contingent on the correlated evolution of life-history and increased parental investment. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 100, 111–123 (2010).
Bickart, K. C., Hollenbeck, M. C., Barrett, L. F. & Dickerson, B. C. Intrinsic amygdala–cortical functional connectivity predicts social network size in humans. J. Neurosci. 32, 14729–14741 (2012).
Kanai, R., Bahrami, B., Roylance, R. & Rees, G. Online social network size is reflected in human brain structure. Proc. R. Soc. B 279, 1327–1334 (2012).
Kiesow, H. et al. 10,000 social brains: sex differentiation in human brain anatomy. Sci. Adv. 6, eaaz1170 (2020).
Kwak, S., Joo, W.-T., Youm, Y. & Chey, J. Social brain volume is associated with in-degree social network size among older adults. Proc. R. Soc. B 285, 20172708 (2018).
Lewis, P. A., Rezaie, R., Brown, R., Roberts, N. & Dunbar, R. I. Ventromedial prefrontal volume predicts understanding of others and social network size. NeuroImage 57, 1624–1629 (2011).
Noonan, M., Mars, R., Sallet, J., Dunbar, R. & Fellows, L. The structural and functional brain networks that support human social networks. Behav. Brain Res. 355, 12–23 (2018).
Meguerditchian, A. et al. Baboons (Papio anubis) living in larger social groups have bigger brains. Evol. Hum. Behav. 42, 30–34 (2021).
Sallet, J. et al. Social network size affects neural circuits in macaques. Science 334, 697–700 (2011).
Testard, C. et al. Social connections predict brain structure in a multidimensional free-ranging primate society. Sci. Adv. 8, eabl5794 (2022).
Passingham, R. E. & Wise, S. P. The Neurobiology of the Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Evolution, and the Origin of Insight (Oxford Univ. Press, 2012).
Sallet, J. et al. The organization of dorsal frontal cortex in humans and macaques. J. Neurosci. 33, 12255–12274 (2013).
Spreng, R. N. et al. The default network of the human brain is associated with perceived social isolation. Nat. Commun. 11, 6393 (2020).
Mars, R. B. et al. The extreme capsule fiber complex in humans and macaque monkeys: a comparative diffusion MRI tractography study. Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 4059–4071 (2016).
Folloni, D. et al. Dichotomous organization of amygdala/temporal–prefrontal bundles in both humans and monkeys. eLife https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.47175 (2019).
Mantini, D. et al. Default mode of brain function in monkeys. J. Neurosci. 31, 12954–12962 (2011).
Mars, R. B. et al. Primate comparative neuroscience using magnetic resonance imaging: promises and challenges. Front. Neurosci. 8, 298 (2014).
Mars, R. B., Sallet, J., Neubert, F.-X. & Rushworth, M. F. Connectivity profiles reveal the relationship between brain areas for social cognition in human and monkey temporoparietal cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 10806–10811 (2013).
Roumazeilles, L. et al. Cortical morphology and white matter tractography of three phylogenetically distant primates: evidence for a simian elaboration. Cereb. Cortex 32, 1608–1624 (2021).
Sliwa, J. & Freiwald, W. A. A dedicated network for social interaction processing in the primate brain. Science 356, 745–749 (2017).
Roberts, S. B. & Dunbar, R. I. Managing relationship decay: network, gender, and contextual effects. Hum. Nat. 26, 426–450 (2015).
Mesulam, M. M. From sensation to cognition. Brain 121, 1013–1052 (1998).
Joffe, T. H. Social pressures have selected for an extended juvenile period in primates. J. Hum. Evol. 32, 593–605 (1997).
Atzil, S., Gao, W., Fradkin, I. & Barrett, L. F. Growing a social brain. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2, 624–636 (2018).
Blakemore, S. J. Imaging brain development: the adolescent brain. NeuroImage 61, 397–406 (2012).
Tamnes, C. K. et al. Development of the cerebral cortex across adolescence: a multisample study of inter-related longitudinal changes in cortical volume, surface area, and thickness. J. Neurosci. 37, 3402–3412 (2017).
Gogtay, N. et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101, 8174–8179 (2004).
Sowell, E. R. et al. Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 309–315 (2003).
Depue, R. A. & Morrone-Strupinsky, J. V. A neurobehavioral model of affiliative bonding: implications for conceptualizing a human trait of affiliation. Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 313–350 (2005). Discussion 350–395.
Dunbar, R. I. The social role of touch in humans and primates: behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 260–268 (2010).
Inagaki, T. K., Ray, L. A., Irwin, M. R., Way, B. M. & Eisenberger, N. I. Opioids and social bonding: naltrexone reduces feelings of social connection. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 728–735 (2016).
Machin, A. J. & Dunbar, R. I. The brain opioid theory of social attachment: a review of the evidence. Behaviour 148, 985–1025 (2011).
Nummenmaa, L. et al. Adult attachment style is associated with cerebral μ‐opioid receptor availability in humans. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 3621–3628 (2015).
Pearce, E., Wlodarski, R., Machin, A. & Dunbar, R. I. The influence of genetic variation on social disposition, romantic relationships and social networks: a replication study. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 4, 400–422 (2018).
Fabre-Nys, C., Meller, R. E. & Keverne, E. B. Opiate antagonists stimulate affiliative behaviour in monkeys. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 16, 653–659 (1982).
Keverne, E. B., Martensz, N. D. & Tuite, B. Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid of monkeys are influenced by grooming relationships. Psychoneuroendocrinology 14, 155–161 (1989).
Gursul, D. et al. Stroking modulates noxious-evoked brain activity in human infants. Curr. Biol. 28, R1380–R1381 (2018).
Olausson, H., Wessberg, J., Morrison, I., McGlone, F. & Vallbo, A. The neurophysiology of unmyelinated tactile afferents. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 185–191 (2010).
Mandler, R. N., Biddison, W. E., Mandler, R. & Serrate, S. A. Beta-endorphin augments the cytolytic activity and interferon production of natural killer cells. J. Immunol. 136, 934–939 (1986).
Puente, J. et al. Enhancement of human natural killer cell activity by opioid peptides: similar response to methionine-enkephalin and beta-endorphin. Brain Behav. Immun. 6, 32–39 (1992).
Sarkar, D. K., Sengupta, A., Zhang, C., Boyadjieva, N. & Murugan, S. Opiate antagonist prevents mu- and delta-opiate receptor dimerization to facilitate ability of agonist to control ethanol-altered natural killer cell functions and mammary tumor growth. J. Biol. Chem. 287, 16734–16747 (2012).
Fiatarone, M. A. et al. Endogenous opioids and the exercise-induced augmentation of natural killer cell activity. J. Lab. Clin. Med. 112, 544–552 (1988).
Gannon, G. A. et al. Beta-endorphin and natural killer cell cytolytic activity during prolonged exercise: is there a connection? Am. J. Physiol. 275, R1725–R1734 (1998).
Suvilehto, J. T., Glerean, E., Dunbar, R. I., Hari, R. & Nummenmaa, L. Topography of social touching depends on emotional bonds between humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 13811–13816 (2015).
Suvilehto, J. T. et al. Cross-cultural similarity in relationship-specific social touching. Proc. R. Soc. B 286, 20190467 (2019).
Hall, J. A. How many hours does it take to make a friend? J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 36, 1278–1296 (2019).
Dunbar, R. I. M. The anatomy of friendship. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 32–51 (2018).
Sutcliffe, A., Dunbar, R., Binder, J. & Arrow, H. Relationships and the social brain: integrating psychological and evolutionary perspectives. Br. J. Psychol. 103, 149–168 (2012).
Saramaki, J. et al. Persistence of social signatures in human communication. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, 942–947 (2014).
Heydari, S., Roberts, S. G., Dunbar, R. I. M. & Saramaki, J. Multichannel social signatures and persistent features of ego networks. Appl. Netw. Sci. 3, 8 (2018).
Dunbar, R. I. Functional significance of social grooming in primates. Folia Primatol. 57, 121–131 (1991).
Dunbar, R. I. et al. Social laughter is correlated with an elevated pain threshold. Proc. R. Soc. B 279, 1161–1167 (2012).
Manninen, S. et al. Social laughter triggers endogenous opioid release in humans. J. Neurosci. 37, 6125–6131 (2017).
Pearce, E., Launay, J. & Dunbar, R. I. The ice-breaker effect: singing mediates fast social bonding. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2, 150221 (2015).
Pearce, E., Launay, J., MacCarron, P. & Dunbar, R. I. Tuning in to others: exploring relational and collective bonding in singing and non-singing groups over time. Psychol. Music 45, 496–512 (2017).
Weinstein, D., Launay, J., Pearce, E., Dunbar, R. I. & Stewart, L. Singing and social bonding: changes in connectivity and pain threshold as a function of group size. Evol. Hum. Behav. 37, 152–158 (2016).
Tarr, B., Launay, J., Benson, C. & Dunbar, R. I. Naltrexone blocks endorphins released when dancing in synchrony. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 3, 241–254 (2017).
Charles, S. J. et al. Blocking mu-opioid receptors inhibits social bonding in rituals. Biol. Lett. 16, 20200485 (2020).
Dunbar, R. I. M. Breaking bread: the functions of social eating. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 3, 198–211 (2017).
Tuulari, J. J. et al. Feeding releases endogenous opioids in humans. J. Neurosci. 37, 8284–8291 (2017).
Dunbar, R. I. et al. Emotional arousal when watching drama increases pain threshold and social bonding. R. Soc. Open Sci. 3, 160288 (2016).
Dunbar, R. I. M. Structure and function in human and primate social networks: implications for diffusion, network stability and health. Proc. Math. Phys. Eng. Sci. 476, 20200446 (2020).
Hamilton, M. J., Milne, B. T., Walker, R. S., Burger, O. & Brown, J. H. The complex structure of hunter–gatherer social networks. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 2195–2203 (2007).
Zhou, W. X., Sornette, D., Hill, R. A. & Dunbar, R. I. Discrete hierarchical organization of social group sizes. Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 439–444 (2005).
Dunbar, R. I. M., Mac Carron, P. & Shultz, S. Primate social group sizes exhibit a regular scaling pattern with natural attractors. Biol. Lett. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2017.0490 (2018).
Hill, R. A., Bentley, R. A. & Dunbar, R. I. Network scaling reveals consistent fractal pattern in hierarchical mammalian societies. Biol. Lett. 4, 748–751 (2008).
Binder, J. F., Roberts, S. G. & Sutcliffe, A. G. Closeness, loneliness, support: core ties and significant ties in personal communities. Soc. Netw. 34, 206–214 (2012).
Dyble, M., van Leeuwen, A. J. & Dunbar, R. Gender differences in Christmas gift-giving. Evol. Behav. Sci. 9, 140 (2015).
Miritello, G. et al. Time as a limited resource: communication strategy in mobile phone networks. Soc. Netw. 35, 89–95 (2013).
Tamarit, I., Cuesta, J. A., Dunbar, R. I. & Sánchez, A. Cognitive resource allocation determines the organization of personal networks. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115, 8316–8321 (2018).
Tamarit, I., Sánchez, A. & Cuesta, J. A. Beyond Dunbar circles: a continuous description of social relationships and resource allocation. Sci. Rep. 12, 2287 (2022).
Burton-Chellew, M. N. & Dunbar, R. I. Hamilton’s rule predicts anticipated social support in humans. Behav. Ecol. 26, 130–137 (2015).
Curry, O., Roberts, S. G. & Dunbar, R. I. Altruism in social networks: evidence for a ‘kinship premium’. Br. J. Psychol. 104, 283–295 (2013).
Benenson, J. F. et al. Males’ greater tolerance of same-sex peers. Psychol. Sci. 20, 184–190 (2009).
David-Barrett, T. et al. Women favour dyadic relationships, but men prefer clubs: cross-cultural evidence from social networking. PLoS ONE 10, e0118329 (2015).
Ghosh, A., Monsivais, D., Bhattacharya, K., Dunbar, R. I. & Kaski, K. Quantifying gender preferences in human social interactions using a large cellphone dataset. EPJ Data Sci. 8, 9 (2019).
Pollet, T. V., Roberts, S. G. & Dunbar, R. I. Extraverts have larger social network layers. J. Individ. Differ. 32, 161–169 (2011).
Roberts, S. G., Dunbar, R. I., Pollet, T. V. & Kuppens, T. Exploring variation in active network size: constraints and ego characteristics. Soc. Netw. 31, 138–146 (2009).
Henzi, S. P. et al. Look who’s talking: developmental trends in the size of conversational cliques. Evol. Hum. Behav. 28, 66–74 (2007).
Pearce, E., Wlodarski, R., Machin, A. & Dunbar, R. I. Exploring the links between dispositions, romantic relationships, support networks and community inclusion in men and women. PLoS ONE 14, e0216210 (2019).
Zajner, C., Spreng, N. & Bzdok, D. Loneliness is linked to specific subregional alterations in hippocampus-default network co-variation. J. Neurophysiol. https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.08.19.456905 (2021).
Smallwood, J. et al. The default mode network in cognition: a topographical perspective. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 22, 503–513 (2021).
Bzdok, D. et al. Segregation of the human medial prefrontal cortex in social cognition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 232 (2013).
Zajner, C., Spreng, R. N. & Bzdok, D. Loneliness is linked to specific subregional alterations in hippocampus-default network covariation. J. Neurophysiol. 126, 2138–2157 (2021).
Aggleton, J. P., Wright, N. F., Rosene, D. L. & Saunders, R. C. Complementary patterns of direct amygdala and hippocampal projections to the macaque prefrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 25, 4351–4373 (2015).
György Buzsáki, M. The Brain from Inside Out (Oxford Univ. Press, 2019).
Schurz, M. et al. Variability in brain structure and function reflects lack of peer support. Cereb. Cortex 31, 4612–4627 (2021).
Krienen, F. M., Tu, P.-C. & Buckner, R. L. Clan mentality: evidence that the medial prefrontal cortex responds to close others. J. Neurosci. 30, 13906–13915 (2010).
Valk, S. L. et al. Structural plasticity of the social brain: differential change after socio-affective and cognitive mental training. Sci. Adv. 3, e1700489 (2017).
Groarke, J. M. et al. Loneliness in the UK during the COVID-19 pandemic: cross-sectional results from the COVID-19 Psychological Wellbeing Study. PLoS ONE 15, e0239698 (2020).
Best, L. A., Law, M. A., Roach, S. & Wilbiks, J. M. The psychological impact of COVID-19 in Canada: effects of social isolation during the initial response. Can. Psychol. 62, 143 (2021).
Peçanha, T., Goessler, K. F., Roschel, H. & Gualano, B. Social isolation during the COVID-19 pandemic can increase physical inactivity and the global burden of cardiovascular disease. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 318, H1441–H1446 (2020).
Benussi, A. et al. Clinical characteristics and outcomes of inpatients with neurologic disease and COVID-19 in Brescia, Lombardy, Italy. Neurology 95, e910–e920 (2020).
McKeown, B. et al. The impact of social isolation and changes in work patterns on ongoing thought during the first COVID-19 lockdown in the United Kingdom. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 118, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2102565118 (2021).
Robb, C. E. et al. Associations of social isolation with anxiety and depression during the early COVID-19 pandemic: a survey of older adults in London, UK. Front. Psychiatry 11, 591120 (2020).
Rodriguez-Rey, R., Garrido-Hernansaiz, H. & Collado, S. Psychological impact and associated factors during the initial stage of the coronavirus (COVID-19) pandemic among the general population in Spain. Front. Psychol. 11, 1540 (2020).
Tian, F. et al. Psychological symptoms of ordinary Chinese citizens based on SCL-90 during the level I emergency response to COVID-19. Psychiatry Res. 288, 112992 (2020).
Niedzwiedz, C. L. et al. Mental health and health behaviours before and during the initial phase of the COVID-19 lockdown: longitudinal analyses of the UK Household Longitudinal Study. J. Epidemiol. Community health 75, 224–231 (2021).
Robinson, E., Sutin, A. R., Daly, M. & Jones, A. A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies comparing mental health before versus during the COVID-19 pandemic in 2020. J. Affect. Disord. 296, 567–576 (2022).
Zaninotto, P., Iob, E., Demakakos, P. & Steptoe, A. Immediate and longer-term changes in the mental health and well-being of older adults in England during the COVID-19 pandemic. JAMA Psychiatry 79, 151–159 (2022).
Douaud, G. et al. SARS-CoV-2 is associated with changes in brain structure in UK Biobank. Nature 604, 697–707 (2022).
Morgan, D. et al. Social dominance in monkeys: dopamine D2 receptors and cocaine self-administration. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 169–174 (2002).
Yip, S., Jordan, A., Kohler, R., Holmes, A. & Bzdok, D. Multivariate, transgenerational associations of the COVID-19 pandemic across minoritized and marginalized communities. JAMA Psychiatry 79, 350–358 (2022).
Sapolsky, R. M. The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science 308, 648–652 (2005).
Lelkes, O. Social participation and social isolation. Income Living Cond. Eur. 217, 240 (2010).
Lederbogen, F., Haddad, L. & Meyer-Lindenberg, A. Urban social stress—risk factor for mental disorders: the case of schizophrenia. Environ. Pollut. 183, 2–6 (2013).
van den Bosch, M. & Meyer-Lindenberg, A. Environmental exposures and depression: biological mechanisms and epidemiological evidence. Annu. Rev. Public Health 40, 239–259 (2019).
Saporta, N. et al. Opposing association of situational and chronic loneliness with interpersonal distance. Brain Sci. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11091135 (2021).
Zajner, C., Spreng, N. & Bzdok, D. Lacking social support is associated with structural divergences in hippocampus-default network co-variation patterns. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.08.19.456949 (2022).
Acknowledgements
D.B. was supported by the Brain Canada Foundation, through the Canada Brain Research Fund, with the financial support of Health Canada, National Institutes of Health (grant nos NIH R01 AG068563A and NIH R01 R01DA053301-01A1), the Canadian Institute of Health Research (grant nos CIHR 438531 and CIHR 470425), the Healthy Brains Healthy Lives initiative (Canada First Research Excellence fund), Google (Research Award and Teaching Award) and the CIFAR Artificial Intelligence Chairs programme (Canada Institute for Advanced Research). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
R.I.M.D. and D.B. conceived and wrote the work.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Human Behaviour thanks the anonymous reviewers for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
About this article
Cite this article
Bzdok, D., Dunbar, R.I.M. Social isolation and the brain in the pandemic era. Nat Hum Behav 6, 1333–1343 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01453-0
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01453-0
This article is cited by
-
(R)-ketamine restores anterior insular cortex activity and cognitive deficits in social isolation-reared mice
Molecular Psychiatry (2024)
-
Social isolation and the risk of Parkinson disease in the UK biobank study
npj Parkinson's Disease (2024)
-
Psychometric Evaluation of the Autism Spectrum Rating Scales (6–18 Years Parent Report) in a Clinical Sample
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders (2024)
-
Central regulation of stress-evoked peripheral immune responses
Nature Reviews Neuroscience (2023)
-
An investigation of simulated and real touch on feelings of loneliness
Scientific Reports (2023)