Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

A brain-based general measure of attention

Abstract

Attention is central to many aspects of cognition, but there is no singular neural measure of a person’s overall attentional functioning across tasks. Here, using original data from 92 participants performing three different attention-demanding tasks during functional magnetic resonance imaging, we constructed a suite of whole-brain models that can predict a profile of multiple attentional components (sustained attention, divided attention and tracking, and working memory capacity) for novel individuals. Multiple brain regions across the salience, subcortical and frontoparietal networks drove accurate predictions, supporting a common (general) attention factor across tasks, distinguished from task-specific ones. Furthermore, connectome-to-connectome transformation modelling generated an individual’s task-related connectomes from rest functional magnetic resonance imaging, substantially improving predictive power. Finally, combining the connectome transformation and general attention factor, we built a standardized measure that shows superior generalization across four independent datasets (total N = 495) of various attentional measures, suggesting broad utility for research and clinical applications.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Prediction accuracy of nine CPMs.
Fig. 2: Cross-prediction results of nine original CPMs across all cognitive states and attention tasks.
Fig. 3: Cross-prediction results of five CPMs trained to predict a common attention factor using different fMRI data.
Fig. 4: Cross-prediction results of CPMs trained to predict task-specific variance.
Fig. 5: Network contribution to CPMs’ prediction performance.
Fig. 6: Prediction of individual behaviours by applying the original CPMs trained using task fMRI to rest fMRI with a rest-to-task connectome transformation using C2C modelling.
Fig. 7: The general attention model in internal validation.
Fig. 8: The general attention model generalizes to predict different attentional measures in four independent datasets.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

Raw task and rest fMRI data used in the primary analyses (n = 92) are available at https://doi.org/10.15154/1520622.

Code availability

Scripts for the predictive model (the general attention model, C2C model and CPM) construction are available for download at https://github.com/rayksyoo/General_Attention. Scripts for the other (statistical) analyses are available from the corresponding author upon request.

References

  1. Chun, M. M., Golomb, J. D. & Turk-Browne, N. B. A taxonomy of external and internal attention. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 73–101 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Weissman, D. H., Roberts, K. C., Visscher, K. M. & Woldorff, M. G. The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 971–978 (2006).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Heinrichs, R. W. & Zakzanis, K. K. Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology 12, 426–445 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Biederman, J., Newcorn, J. & Sprich, S. Comorbidity of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with conduct, depressive, anxiety, and other disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 148, 564–577 (1991).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Levin, H. S. et al. Neurobehavioral outcome following minor head injury: a three-center study. J. Neurosurg. 66, 234–243 (1987).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Rosenberg, M. D. et al. Functional connectivity predicts changes in attention observed across minutes, days, and months. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 117, 3797–3807 (2020).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Kucyi, A. et al. Prediction of stimulus-independent and task-unrelated thought from functional brain networks. Nat. Commun. 12, 1793 (2021).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Deary, I. J., Penke, L. & Johnson, W. The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 201–211 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Miyake, A. et al. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Huang, L., Mo, L. & Li, Y. Measuring the interrelations among multiple paradigms of visual attention: an individual differences approach. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 38, 414–428 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Corbetta, M. & Shulman, G. L. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 215–229 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Kanwisher, N. & Wojciulik, E. Visual attention: insights from brain imaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1, 91–100 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Rosenberg, M. D., Finn, E. S., Scheinost, D., Constable, R. T. & Chun, M. M. Characterizing attention with predictive network models. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 290–302 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Rosenberg, M. D. et al. A neuromarker of sustained attention from whole-brain functional connectivity. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 165–171 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Wu, E. X. W. et al. Overlapping attentional networks yield divergent behavioral predictions across tasks: neuromarkers for diffuse and focused attention? Neuroimage 209, 116535 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Kucyi, A., Hove, M. J., Esterman, M., Hutchison, R. M. & Valera, E. M. Dynamic brain network correlates of spontaneous fluctuations in attention. Cereb. Cortex 27, 1831–1840 (2017).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Shen, X. et al. Using connectome-based predictive modeling to predict individual behavior from brain connectivity. Nat. Protoc. 12, 506–518 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Finn, E. S. et al. Functional connectome fingerprinting: identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1664–1671 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Woo, C. W., Chang, L. J., Lindquist, M. A. & Wager, T. D. Building better biomarkers: brain models in translational neuroimaging. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 365–377 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Gratton, C. et al. Defining individual-specific functional neuroanatomy for precision psychiatry. Biol. Psychiatry 88, 28–39 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Cohen, J. R. & D’Esposito, M. The segregation and integration of distinct brain networks and their relationship to cognition. J. Neurosci. 36, 12083–12094 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Yoo, K. et al. Multivariate approaches improve the reliability and validity of functional connectivity and prediction of individual behaviors. Neuroimage 197, 212–223 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Rosenberg, M. D. et al. Methylphenidate modulates functional network connectivity to enhance attention. J. Neurosci. 36, 9547–9557 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Rosenberg, M. D., Hsu, W.-T., Scheinost, D., Todd Constable, R. & Chun, M. M. Connectome-based models predict separable components of attention in novel individuals. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 30, 160–173 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Yoo, K. et al. Connectome-based predictive modeling of attention: comparing different functional connectivity features and prediction methods across datasets. Neuroimage 167, 11–22 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Lin, Q. et al. Resting-state functional connectivity predicts cognitive impairment related to Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10, 94 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Avery, E. W. et al. Distributed patterns of functional connectivity predict working memory performance in novel healthy and memory-impaired individuals. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 241–255 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Zhang, H. et al. Do intrinsic brain functional networks predict working memory from childhood to adulthood? Hum. Brain Mapp. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25143 (2020).

  29. Tomasi, D. & Volkow, N. D. Network connectivity predicts language processing in healthy adults. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41, 3696–3708 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Beaty, R. E. et al. Robust prediction of individual creative ability from brain functional connectivity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 115, 1087–1092 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Hsu, W.-T., Rosenberg, M. D., Scheinost, D., Constable, R. T. & Chun, M. M. Resting-state functional connectivity predicts neuroticism and extraversion in novel individuals. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 13, 224–232 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Jiang, R. et al. Connectome-based individualized prediction of temperament trait scores. Neuroimage 183, 366–374 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Cai, H., Chen, J., Liu, S., Zhu, J. & Yu, Y. Brain functional connectome-based prediction of individual decision impulsivity. Cortex 125, 288–298 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Esterman, M., Noonan, S. K., Rosenberg, M. & Degutis, J. In the zone or zoning out? Tracking behavioral and neural fluctuations during sustained attention. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2712–2723 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Fossella, J., Flombaum, J. I. & Posner, M. I. The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage 26, 471–479 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Kardan, O. et al. Adult neuromarkers of sustained attention and working memory predict inter- and intra-individual differences in these processes in youth. Preprint at bioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.08.01.454530 (2021).

  37. Engle, R. W. Working memory capacity as executive attention. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 11, 19–23 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Yoo, K. et al. A cognitive state transformation model for task-general and task-specific subsystems of the brain connectome. Preprint at bioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.12.23.424176 (2020).

  39. Noble, S. et al. Influences on the test-retest reliability of functional connectivity MRI and its relationship with behavioral utility. Cereb. Cortex 27, 5415–5429 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Varoquaux, G. Cross-validation failure: small sample sizes lead to large error bars. NeuroImage 180, 68–77 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Jangraw, D. C. et al. A functional connectivity-based neuromarker of sustained attention generalizes to predict recall in a reading task. Neuroimage 166, 99–109 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Fountain-Zaragoza, S., Samimy, S., Rosenberg, M. D. & Prakash, R. S. Connectome-based models predict attentional control in aging adults. Neuroimage 186, 1–13 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Van Essen, D. C. et al. The WU-Minn Human Connectome Project: an overview. Neuroimage 80, 62–79 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. DuPaul, G. J., Power, T. J., Anastopoulos, A. D. & Reid, R. ADHD Rating Scale—IV: Checklists, Norms, and Clinical Interpretation (Guilford, 1998).

  45. Consortium, T. A.-200. The ADHD-200 Consortium: a model to advance the translational potential of neuroimaging in clinical neuroscience. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 6, 62 (2012).

    Google Scholar 

  46. Satterthwaite, T. D. et al. Neuroimaging of the Philadelphia neurodevelopmental cohort. NeuroImage 86, 544–553 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Casey, B. J. et al. The adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study: imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 43–54 (2018).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Wojciulik, E. & Kanwisher, N. The generality of parietal involvement in visual attention. Neuron 23, 747–764 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Duncan, J. & Owen, A. M. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci. 23, 475–483 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Ramnani, N. & Owen, A. M. Anterior prefrontal cortex: insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 184–194 (2004).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Miller, E. K. & Cohen, J. D. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Pardo, J. V., Fox, P. T. & Raichle, M. E. Localization of a human system for sustained attention by positron emission tomography. Nature 349, 61–64 (1991).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Corbetta, M., Shulman, G. L., Miezin, F. M. & Petersen, S. E. Superior parietal cortex activation during spatial attention shifts and visual feature conjunction. Sci. (80-.) 270, 802–805 (1995).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  54. Hopfinger, J. B., Buonocore, M. H. & Mangun, G. R. The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 284–291 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Sprague, T. C. & Serences, J. T. Attention modulates spatial priority maps in the human occipital, parietal and frontal cortices. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1879–1887 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Wimmer, R. D. et al. Thalamic control of sensory selection in divided attention. Nature 526, 705–709 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Heinze, H. J. et al. Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans. Nature 372, 543–546 (1994).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Coull, J. T., Vidal, F., Nazarian, B. & Macar, F. Functional anatomy of the attentional modulation of time estimation. Sci. (80-.) 303, 1506–1508 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  59. Gao, J. H. et al. Cerebellum implicated in sensory acquisition and discrimination rather than motor control. Sci. (80-.). 272, 545–547 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  60. Leiner, H. C., Leiner, A. L. & Dow, R. S. Does the cerebellum contribute to mental skills? Behav. Neurosci. 100, 443–454 (1986).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., Posner, M. I., Mintun, M. & Raichle, M. E. Positron emission tomographic studies of the processing of single words. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1, 153–170 (1989).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Stoodley, C. J. The cerebellum and cognition: evidence from functional imaging studies. Cerebellum 11, 352–365 (2012).

  63. Strick, P. L., Dum, R. P. & Fiez, J. A. Cerebellum and nonmotor function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 32, 413–434 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Buckner, R. L. The cerebellum and cognitive function: 25 years of insight from anatomy and neuroimaging. Neuron 80, 807–815 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Allen, G., Buxton, R. B., Wong, E. C. & Courchesne, E. Attentional activation of the cerebellum independent of motor involvement. Sci. (80-.) 275, 1940–1943 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  66. Rees, G., Frackowiak, R. & Frith, C. Two modulatory effects of attention that mediate object categorization in human cortex. Sci. (80-.) 275, 835–838 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  67. Gottwald, B., Mihajlovic, Z., Wilde, B. & Mehdorn, H. M. Does the cerebellum contribute to specific aspects of attention? Neuropsychologia 41, 1452–1460 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Greene, A. S., Gao, S., Scheinost, D. & Constable, R. T. Task-induced brain state manipulation improves prediction of individual traits. Nat. Commun. 9, 2807 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  69. Jiang, R. et al. Task-induced brain connectivity promotes the detection of individual differences in brain–behavior relationships. Neuroimage 207, 116370 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Sui, J., Jiang, R., Bustillo, J. & Calhoun, V. Neuroimaging-based individualized prediction of cognition and behavior for mental disorders and health: methods and promises. Biol. Psychiatry 88, 818–828 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Gao, S., Greene, A. S., Constable, R. T. & Scheinost, D. Combining multiple connectomes improves predictive modeling of phenotypic measures. Neuroimage 201, 116038 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Pujol, J. et al. Clinical application of functional magnetic resonance imaging in presurgical identification of the central sulcus. J. Neurosurg. 88, 863–869 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Bullmore, E. The future of functional MRI in clinical medicine. Neuroimage 62, 1267–1271 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Vanderwal, T., Kelly, C., Eilbott, J., Mayes, L. C. & Castellanos, F. X. Inscapes: a movie paradigm to improve compliance in functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 122, 222–232 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Rosenberg, M., Noonan, S., DeGutis, J. & Esterman, M. Sustaining visual attention in the face of distraction: a novel gradual-onset continuous performance task. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 75, 426–439 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Pylyshyn, Z. W. & Storm, R. W. Tracking multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. Spat. Vis. 3, 179–197 (1988).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Luck, S. J. & Vogel, E. K. The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions. Nature 390, 279–284 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Pashler, H. Familiarity and visual change detection. Percept. Psychophys. 44, 369–378 (1988).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., Morey, C. C. & Cowan, N. How to measure working memory capacity in the change detection paradigm. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 324–330 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. Cox, R. W. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29, 162–173 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Shen, X., Tokoglu, F., Papademetris, X. & Constable, R. T. Groupwise whole-brain parcellation from resting-state fMRI data for network node identification. Neuroimage 82, 403–415 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Scheinost, D. et al. Ten simple rules for predictive modeling of individual differences in neuroimaging. Neuroimage 193, 35–45 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Nichols, T. E. & Holmes, A. P. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15, 1–25 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Abbas, K. et al. GEFF: graph embedding for functional fingerprinting. Neuroimage 221, 117181 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Amico, E. & Goñi, J. The quest for identifiability in human functional connectomes. Sci. Rep. 8, 8254 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  86. Barch, D. M. et al. NeuroImage function in the human connectome: task-fMRI and individual differences in behavior. Neuroimage 80, 169–189 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Smith, S. M. et al. Resting-state fMRI in the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage 80, 144–168 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This project was supported by National Institutes of Health grant MH108591 to M.M.C. and by National Science Foundation grant BCS1558497 to M.M.C.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

K.Y., M.D.R. and M.M.C. designed the study. Y.H.K. and E.W.A. performed fMRI experiments. K.Y. and M.D.R. analysed behavioural data. K.Y. and Y.H.K. analysed fMRI data. K.Y. conducted modelling and visualization. K.Y., M.M.C., M.D.R., Q.L., D.S. and R.T.C. discussed the results and implications. M.M.C. and R.T.C. supervised the project. K.Y., Y.H.K. and M.M.C. wrote the original draft; K.Y., M.M.C., M.D.R., Q.L., E.W.A., D.S. and R.T.C. reviewed the original draft and contributed to the final version of the paper.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Kwangsun Yoo or Marvin M. Chun.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature Human Behaviour thanks Jing Sui, Francisco Castellanos and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data

Extended Data Fig. 1 Predictive anatomy of three task-based CPMs.

a. The scale bar in gradCPT, MOT and VSTM represents the relative ratio of predictive functional connections to all possible number of functional connections between networks with a sign representing whether the connection is in a positive or negative network. The scale bar in overlap represents the actual number of predictive functional connections with a sign representing whether the connection is in a positive or negative network. GradCPT: gradual-onset continuous performance task, MOT: multiple object tracking, and VSTM: visual short-term memory. MF: medial-frontal network, FP: frontoparietal network, DM: default mode network, VI: visual I, VII: visual II, VAs: visual association, SA: salience network, Subc: subcortex, Cbl: cerebellum. b. The number of predictive connections of three task-based CPMs in positive and negative networks.

Extended Data Fig. 2 Cross-prediction results of five common attention factor CPMs.

a. Cross-prediction results when models were applied to predict the common attention factor from different fMRI data. Models’ prediction accuracies were assessed by prediction q2 and correlation r between observed and predicted common factor measures. P values of significance were obtained using 1,000 permutations and corrected for all 5×5 tests (***: p < 0.001, **: p < 0.01, *: p < 0.05, and ~: p < 0.1). Rows represent different fMRI data used to predict a common attention factor used in model construction, and columns represent the same but in model validation. b. Cross-prediction results, taking into account shared variance (the common factor) between task behaviors. Models’ prediction accuracies were assessed by partial correlation between observed and predicted behavior scores while controlling for the shared variance. P values of significance were obtained using 1,000 permutations and corrected for all 5×9 tests (***: p < 0.001, **: p < 0.01, *: p < 0.05, and ~: p < 0.1). Rows represent different fMRI data used to predict a common attention factor used in model construction, and columns represent combinations of fMRI data and behavior scores used in model validation. GradCPT: gradual-onset continuous performance task, MOT: multiple object tracking, and VSTM: visual short-term memory.

Extended Data Fig. 3 A similarity of individual behaviours between different tasks.

The similarity was assessed by Pearson’s correlation of individual performances between attention tasks. Individual behaviors were significantly correlated between every pair of tasks. GradCPT: gradual-onset continuous performance task, MOT: multiple object tracking, and VSTM: visual short-term memory.

Extended Data Fig. 4 Cross-prediction results of task-specific CPMs.

a. Cross-prediction results, taking into account shared variance between task behaviors. Models’ prediction accuracies were assessed by partial correlation between observed and predicted behavior scores while controlling for the shared variance. P value was obtained using 1,000 permutations and corrected for multiple tests (***: p < 0.001, **: p < 0.01, *: p < 0.05, and ~: p < 0.1). Rows represent combinations of fMRI data and behavior scores used in model construction, and columns represent combinations of fMRI data and behavior scores used in model validation. GradCPT: gradual-onset continuous performance task, MOT: multiple object tracking, and VSTM: visual short-term memory. b. Cross-prediction results when models were applied to predict the common attention factor from different fMRI data. Models’ prediction accuracies were assessed by correlation between observed and predicted common factor. P value was obtained using 1,000 permutations and corrected for all 9×5 tests (***: p < 0.001, **: p < 0.01, *: p < 0.05, and ~: p < 0.1). Rows represent combinations of fMRI data and behavior scores used in model construction, and columns represent different fMRI data used to predict a common attention factor used in model validation.

Extended Data Fig. 5 Cross-prediction using connectivity between the frontoparietal (FP, 2), visual II (VII, 6), salience (SA, 8), subcortical (Subc, 9), cerebellar (Cbl, 10) networks.

Prediction of a model using connectivity between the medial-frontal (1), default mode (3), motor (4), visual I (5), visual association (7) networks was also obtained as a control. A. Rows represent combinations of networks (indicated by numbers) used in each model. Models’ prediction accuracies were assessed by correlating model-predicted and observed behavioral scores. B. Prediction performance of each network obtained by averaging all models that used the network in A. C. The same result as A, but model accuracies were assessed by q2. D. Prediction performance of each network obtained by averaging all models that used the network in C. GradCPT: gradual-onset continuous performance task, MOT: multiple object tracking, and VSTM: visual short-term memory.

Extended Data Fig. 6 Similarity between C2C model-generated task connectomes and empirical task connectomes.

Error bar represents standard deviation from 1,000 iterations. A and C represent a spatial similarity between two connectomes assessed by Pearson’s correlation. Darker bars represent the similarity between empirical task and generated task connectomes, and lighter bars represent the similarity between empirical task and empirical rest connectomes. The higher similarity of the generated connectome indicates that the C2C model accurately generates the target task connectome from the rest connectome. B and D represent root mean square (RMS) difference between two connectomes. The smaller difference of the generated connectome indicates that the C2C model accurately generates the target task connectome from the rest connectome. In a box-whisker plot, a box covers the first to third quartile (q1 and q3, respectively) of the data, and a center line represents the median. A red dot represents the mean. Whisker covers approximately 99.3% of data (±2.7*standrad deviation), extended to the most extreme point that is not an outlier. A data point is considered an outlier if it is greater than q3+1.5*(q3-q1) or less than q1-1.5*(q3-q1). GradCPT: gradual-onset continuous performance task, MOT: multiple object tracking, and VSTM: visual short-term memory. *: p < 0.001 from 1,000 permutations.

Extended Data Fig. 7 The general attention connectome lookup table.

Out of a total 30,135 edges, 10,885 (36.1%) edges were pulled from gradCPT, 12,542 (41.6%) edges were from MOT, and 6,708 (22.3%) were from VSTM. The Ratio map was obtained based on All map. In each within- or between-network element in Ratio, the number of edges in the element for each task was counted and normalized by the total number of edges of each task. A task with the highest normalized value was assigned.

Extended Data Fig. 8 Scatter plots of predicted and observed attention scores in four external datasets.

Three models, the general attention model and two single task models (model 1 and 4 in Table 1) were trained within the internal dataset and then applied to rest connectomes in the four datasets. If a fitted line closely passes the origin (0,0) with a positive slope (staying within white quadrants), the model could be considered successfully predicting actual attentional abilities. There was no constraint on intercepts in fitting a line. The general model best generalized to predict various attentional measures in four independent external datasets.

Extended Data Fig. 9 Prediction error, assessed by mean square error (MSE), of the general attention model in four independent datasets.

The general model significantly reduced prediction error (assessed by MSE) compared to null models in four datasets. In all datasets, the general attention model produced the lowest prediction error among all models tested. ***: p < 0.001, **: p < 0.01, *: p < 0.05, and ~: p < 0.1 from 1,000 permutations.

Supplementary information

Supplementary information

Supplementary methods, results, discussion, references, Tables 1–7 and Figs. 1–11.

Reporting summary

Peer review information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Yoo, K., Rosenberg, M.D., Kwon, Y.H. et al. A brain-based general measure of attention. Nat Hum Behav 6, 782–795 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01301-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-022-01301-1

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing