Abstract
Spatial and mathematical abilities are strongly associated. Here, we analysed data from 17,648 children, aged 6–8 years, who performed 7 weeks of mathematical training together with randomly assigned spatial cognitive training with tasks demanding more spatial manipulation (mental rotation or tangram), maintenance of spatial information (a visuospatial working memory task) or spatial, non-verbal reasoning. We found that the type of cognitive training children performed had a significant impact on mathematical learning, with training of visuospatial working memory and reasoning being the most effective. This large, community-based study shows that spatial cognitive training can result in transfer to academic abilities, and that reasoning ability and maintenance of spatial information is relevant for mathematics learning in young children.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Relevant articles
Open Access articles citing this article.
-
Children with Mathematical Learning Difficulties—How Do Their Working Memory Skills Differ from Typically Developing First Graders?
Journal für Mathematik-Didaktik Open Access 26 June 2023
-
Overemphasizing individual differences and overlooking systemic factors reinforces educational inequality
npj Science of Learning Open Access 08 May 2023
-
A dual-process model for cognitive training
npj Science of Learning Open Access 06 May 2023
Access options
Access Nature and 54 other Nature Portfolio journals
Get Nature+, our best-value online-access subscription
$29.99 / 30 days
cancel any time
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 12 digital issues and online access to articles
$119.00 per year
only $9.92 per issue
Rent or buy this article
Prices vary by article type
from$1.95
to$39.95
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout




Data availability
The data to replicate the main analysis (that is, mixed-effects model) are available at https://github.com/njudd/spatialcognition. Data for the baseline characteristics and graphs in this study are available upon request from the corresponding author.
Code availability
The code to replicate the main analysis (that is, mixed-effects model) is available at https://github.com/njudd/spatialcognition. Code for the baseline characteristics and graphs in this study is available upon request from the corresponding author.
References
Wai, J., Lubinski, D. & Benbow, C. P. Spatial ability for STEM domains: aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. J. Educ. Psychol. 101, 817–835 (2009).
Hawes, Z. & Ansari, D. What explains the relationship between spatial and mathematical skills? A review of evidence from brain and behavior. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 27, 465–482 (2020).
Mix, K. S. et al. Separate but correlated: the latent structure of space and mathematics across development. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 145, 1206–1227 (2016).
Peng, P., Namkung, J., Barnes, M. & Sun, C. A meta-analysis of mathematics and working memory: moderating effects of working memory domain, type of mathematics skill, and sample characteristics. J. Educ. Psychol. 108, 455–473 (2016).
Gathercole, S. E. & Brown, L. Working memory assessments at school entry as longitudinal predictors of National Curriculum attainment levels. Educ. Child Psychol. 20, 109–122 (2003).
Geary, D. C. Cognitive predictors of achievement growth in mathematics: a 5-year longitudinal study. Dev. Psychol. 47, 1539–1552 (2011).
Dillon, M. R., Kannan, H., Dean, J. T., Spelke, E. S. & Duflo, E. Cognitive science in the field: a preschool intervention durably enhances intuitive but not formal mathematics. Science 357, 47–55 (2017).
Newcombe, N. Harnessing Spatial Thinking to Support STEM Learning Working paper 161 (OECD iLibrary, 2017); https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/education/harnessing-spatial-thinking-to-support-stem-learning_7d5dcae6-en
Stieff, M. & Uttal, D. How much can spatial training improve STEM achievement? Educ. Psychol. Rev. 27, 607–615 (2015).
Paying Attention to Spatial Reasoning, K-12: Support Document for Paying Attention to Mathematics Education (Ontario Ministry of Education, 2014); http://www.edu.gov.on.ca/eng/literacynumeracy/lnspayingattention.pdf
Lohman, D. F. in Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence Vol. 4 (ed. Sternberg, R. J.) 181–248 (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1988).
Carpenter, P. A. & Just, M. A. in Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence Vol. 3 (ed. Stenberg, R. J.) 221–252 (Erlbaum, 1986).
Cheng, Y.-L. & Mix, K. S. Spatial training improves children’s mathematics ability. J. Cogn. Dev. 15, 2–11 (2014).
Hawes, Z., Moss, J., Caswell, B., Naqvi, S. & MacKinnon, S. Enhancing children’s spatial and numerical skills through a dynamic spatial approach to early geometry instruction: effects of a 32-week intervention. Cogn. Instr. 35, 236–264 (2017).
Lowrie, T., Logan, T. & Hegarty, M. The influence of spatial visualization training on students’ spatial reasoning and mathematics performance. J. Cogn. Dev. 20, 729–751 (2019).
Hawes, Z., Moss, J., Caswell, B. & Poliszczuk, D. Effects of mental rotation training on children’s spatial and mathematics performance: a randomized controlled study. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 4, 60–68 (2015).
Cornu, V., Schiltz, C., Pazouki, T. & Martin, R. Training early visuo-spatial abilities: a controlled classroom-based intervention study. Appl. Dev. Sci. 23, 1–21 (2017).
Rodán, A., Gimeno, P., Elosúa, M. R., Montoro, P. R. & Contreras, M. J. Boys and girls gain in spatial, but not in mathematical ability after mental rotation training in primary education. Learn. Individ. Differ. 70, 1–11 (2019).
Wright, H. et al. Improving Working Memory (Education Endowment Foundation, 2019); https://westminsterresearch.westminster.ac.uk/download/1d07359f1fda308387fc3679b914dac0eb89947b618f7f68f999a6f87793bfba/1174522/Working%20Memory.pdf
Berger, E. M., Fehr, E., Hermes, H., Schunk, D. & Winkel, K. The Impact of Working Memory Training on Children’s Cognitive and Noncognitive Skills Discussion Paper No. 09/2020 (NHH Department of Economics, 2020); https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3622985
Bergman-Nutley, S. & Klingberg, T. Effect of working memory training on working memory, arithmetic and following instructions. Psychol. Res. 78, 869–877 (2014).
Roberts, G. et al. Academic outcomes 2 years after working memory training for children with low working memory: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr. 170, e154568 (2016).
Schwaighofer, M., Fischer, F. & Bühner, M. Does working memory training transfer? A meta-analysis including training conditions as moderators. Educ. Psychol. 50, 138–166 (2015).
Simons, D. J. et al. Do “brain-training” programs work? Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 17, 103–186 (2016).
Francis, G. Too good to be true: publication bias in two prominent studies from experimental psychology. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 19, 151–156 (2012).
Green, C. S. et al. Improving methodological standards in behavioral interventions for cognitive enhancement. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 3, 2–29 (2019).
Mackintosh, N. & Mackintosh, N. J. IQ and Human Intelligence (Oxford Univ. Press, 2011).
Bergman Nutley, S. et al. Gains in fluid intelligence after training non-verbal reasoning in 4-year-old children: a controlled, randomized study. Dev. Sci. 14, 591–601 (2011).
Klauer, K. J. & Phye, G. D. Inductive reasoning: a training approach. Rev. Educ. Res. 78, 85–123 (2008).
Mackey, A. P., Hill, S. S., Stone, S. I. & Bunge, S. A. Differential effects of reasoning and speed training in children. Dev. Sci. 14, 582–590 (2011).
Nemmi, F. et al. Behavior and neuroimaging at baseline predict individual response to combined mathematical and working memory training in children. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 43–51 (2016).
Fischer, U., Moeller, K., Bientzle, M., Cress, U. & Nuerk, H.-C. Sensori-motor spatial training of number magnitude representation. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 177–183 (2011).
Outhwaite, L. A., Faulder, M., Gulliford, A. & Pitchford, N. J. Raising early achievement in math with interactive apps: a randomized control trial. J. Educ. Psychol. 111, 284–298 (2019).
Klingberg, T. et al. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD—a randomized, controlled trial. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 44, 177–186 (2005).
Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J. & Perrig, W. J. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 6829–6833 (2008).
Schmiedek, F., Lövdén, M. & Lindenberger, U. Hundred days of cognitive training enhance broad cognitive abilities in adulthood: findings from the COGITO study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2, 27 (2010).
Roid, G. H. & Miller, L. J. Leiter International Performance Scale—Revised: Examiner’s Manual (Stoelting, 1997).
Mix, K. S. Why are spatial skill and mathematics related? Child Dev. Perspect. 13, 121–126 (2019).
Lortie-Forgues, H. & Inglis, M. Rigorous large-scale educational RCTs are often uninformative: should we be concerned? Educ. Res. 48, 158–166 (2019).
Bloom, H. S., Hill, C. J., Black, A. R. & Lipsey, M. W. Performance trajectories and performance gaps as achievement effect-size benchmarks for educational interventions. J. Res. Educ. Eff. 1, 289–328 (2008).
Abelson, R. P. A variance explanation paradox: when a little is a lot. Psychol. Bull. 97, 129–133 (1985).
Funder, D. C. & Ozer, D. J. Evaluating effect size in psychological research: sense and nonsense. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 2, 156–168 (2019).
Butterworth, B. & Kovas, Y. Understanding neurocognitive developmental disorders can improve education for all. Science 340, 300–305 (2013).
Uttal, D. H. et al. The malleability of spatial skills: a meta-analysis of training studies. Psychol. Bull. 139, 352–402 (2013).
Neuburger, S., Jansen, P., Heil, M. & Quaiser-Pohl, C. Gender differences in pre-adolescents’ mental-rotation performance: do they depend on grade and stimulus type? Pers. Individ. Differ. 50, 1238–1242 (2011).
Revelle, W. psych: procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research. https://www.scholars.northwestern.edu/en/publications/psych-procedures-for-personality-and-psychological-research (Northwestern University, 2019).
R Core Development Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2014).
Rosseel, Y. Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling and more. Version 0.5-12 (BETA). J. Stat. Softw. 48, 1–36 (2012).
Enders, C. K. & Bandalos, D. L. The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Struct. Equ. Model. 8, 430–457 (2001).
Hu, L. & Bentler, P. M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55 (1999).
Putnick, D. L. & Bornstein, M. H. Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: the state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Dev. Rev. 41, 71–90 (2016).
Chen, F. F. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct. Equ. Model. 14, 464–504 (2007).
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48 (2015).
Lenth, R. emmeans: estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/emmeans/index.html (Univ. Iowa, 2019).
Acknowledgements
We acknowledge R. Almeida, D. Sjölander, J. Beckeman, B. Sauce and D. Zhang for extensive help with various aspects of the study. This work was supported by contributions from M. Westman and S. Westman, along with funding from The Swedish Medical Research Foundation. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
N.J. and T.K. contributed equally in all aspects of the study.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
T.K. holds an unpaid position as Chief Scientific Officer for the non-profit organization Cognition Matters. N.J. declares no competing interests.
Additional information
Peer review information Nature Human Behaviour thanks Kelly S. Mix and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Supplementary Information
Supplementary Figs. 1–7 and Supplementary Tables 1–5.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Judd, N., Klingberg, T. Training spatial cognition enhances mathematical learning in a randomized study of 17,000 children. Nat Hum Behav 5, 1548–1554 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01118-4
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01118-4
This article is cited by
-
No evidence of a positive effect of learning Chinese language as an L2 on spatial ability
Scientific Reports (2023)
-
Overemphasizing individual differences and overlooking systemic factors reinforces educational inequality
npj Science of Learning (2023)
-
A dual-process model for cognitive training
npj Science of Learning (2023)
-
Connecting spatial thinking to STEM learning through visualizations
Nature Reviews Psychology (2023)
-
Children with Mathematical Learning Difficulties—How Do Their Working Memory Skills Differ from Typically Developing First Graders?
Journal für Mathematik-Didaktik (2023)