Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Exposure to peers’ pro-diversity attitudes increases inclusion and reduces the achievement gap

Subjects

Abstract

There is a dearth of empirically validated pro-diversity methods that effectively create a more inclusive social climate. We developed two scalable interventions that target people’s perceptions of social norms by communicating to them that their peers hold pro-diversity attitudes and engage in inclusive behaviours. We tested the interventions in six randomized controlled trials at a large public university in the United States (total n = 2,490). Non-marginalized students exposed to our interventions reported more positive attitudes toward outgroups and greater appreciation of diversity, whereas marginalized students had an increased sense of belonging, reported being treated more inclusively by their peers and earned better grades. While many current pro-diversity initiatives focus on raising awareness about the fact that implicit bias and subtle discrimination are widespread, our findings spotlight the importance of drawing people’s attention to their peers’ pro-diversity values and attitudes to create positive and lasting effects on the social climate.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Perceptions of inclusive climate as a function of experimental condition in experiments 1–5.
Fig. 2: Results from the mediation analysis in Experiment 4.
Fig. 3: Perceptions of inclusive climate as a function of experimental condition among marginalized students in Experiment 5.
Fig. 4: Course grades as a function of experimental condition and student privilege in experiment 6.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

Due to Institutional Review Board restrictions, the data are not publicly available. However, the data are stored on a secure university server and are available upon request (contact markus.brauer@wisc.edu).

Code availability

As for data availability, all R code files can also be provided upon request. All other study materials are included in the Supplementary Information.

References

  1. Black, R. et al. The effect of environmental change on human migration. Glob. Environ. Change 21, S3–S11 (2011).

    Google Scholar 

  2. Colby, S. L. & Ortman, J. M. Projections of the Size and Composition of the US Population: 2014 to 2060 (US Census Bureau, 2015).

  3. Jaekyung, J. L. Racial and ethnic achievement gap trends: reversing the progress toward equity? Educ. Res. 31, 3–12 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  4. Strayhorn, T. L. College Students’ Sense of Belonging: A Key to Educational Success for All Students (Routledge, 2012).

  5. Walton, G. M. & Cohen, G. L. A question of belonging: race, social fit, and achievement. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92, 82–96 (2007).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Zumbrunn, S., McKim, C., Buhs, E. & Hawley, L. R. Support, belonging, motivation, and engagement in the college classroom: a mixed method study. Instr. Sci. 42, 661–684 (2014).

    Google Scholar 

  7. Hausmann, L. R. M., Schofield, J. W. & Woods, R. L. Sense of belonging as a predictor of intentions to persist among African American and White first-year college students. Res. High. Educ. 48, 803–839 (2007).

    Google Scholar 

  8. Kalev, A., Dobbin, F. & Kelly, E. Best practices or best guesses? Assessing the efficacy of corporate affirmative action and diversity policies. Am. Sociol. Rev. 71, 589–617 (2006).

    Google Scholar 

  9. Kulik, C. T., Pepper, M. B., Roberson, L. & Parker, S. K. The rich get richer: predicting participation in voluntary diversity training. J. Organ. Behav. 28, 753–769 (2007).

    Google Scholar 

  10. Lai, C. K. et al. Reducing implicit racial preferences: II. Intervention effectiveness across time. J. Exp. Psychol. 145, 1001–1016 (2016).

    Google Scholar 

  11. Forscher, P. S. et al. A meta-analysis of procedures to change implicit measures. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 117, 522–559 (2019).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Paluck, E. L. & Green, D. P. Prejudice reduction: what works? A review and assessment of research and practice. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 339–367 (2009).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Sechrist, G. B. & Stangor, C. Perceived consensus influences intergroup behavior and stereotype accessibility. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 80, 645–654 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. McDonald, R. I. & Crandall, C. S. Social norms and social influence. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 3, 147–151 (2015).

    Google Scholar 

  15. Perkins, H. W. & Berkowitz, A. D. Perceiving the community norms of alcohol use among students: some research implications for campus alcohol education programming. Int. J. Addict. 21, 961–976 (1986).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Gavac, S., Murrar, S. & Brauer, M. in Social Psychology: How Other People Influence our Thoughts and Actions (ed. Summers, R.) 333–360 (ABC-CLIO, 2015).

  17. Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R. & Kallgren, C. A. A focus theory of normative conduct: recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58, 1015–1026 (1990).

    Google Scholar 

  18. Tankard, M. E. & Paluck, E. L. Norm perception as a vehicle for social change. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 10, 181–211 (2016).

    Google Scholar 

  19. Miller, D. T. & Prentice, D. A. Changing norms to change behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67, 339–361 (2016).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Jones, S. C., Andrews, K. & Francis, K. (2017). Combining social norms and social marketing to address underage drinking: development and process evaluation of a whole-of-community intervention. PLoS ONE 12, 1–14 (2017).

  21. Perkins, H. W. & Craig, D. W. A successful social norms campaign to reduce alcohol misuse among college student-athletes. J. Stud. Alcohol 67, 880–889 (2006).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Haines, M. P., Barker, G. P. & Rice, R. in The Social Norms Approach to Preventing School and College Age Substance Abuse: A Handbook for Educators, Counselors, and Clinicians (ed. Perkins, W.) 235–244 (John Wiley, 2003).

  23. Goldstein, N. J., Cialdini, R. B. & Griskevicius, V. A room with a viewpoint: using social norms to motivate environmental conservation in hotels. J. Consum. Res. 35, 472–482 (2008).

    Google Scholar 

  24. Allcott, H. & Rogers, T. The short-run and long-run effects of behavioral interventions: experimental evidence from energy conservation. Am. Econ. Rev. 104, 3003–3037 (2014).

    Google Scholar 

  25. Bernedo, M., Ferraro, P. J. & Price, M. The persistent impacts of norm-based messaging and their implications for water conservation. J. Consum. Policy 37, 437–452 (2014).

    Google Scholar 

  26. Stangor, C., Sechrist, G. B. & Jost, J. T. Changing racial beliefs by providing consensus information. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 27, 486–496 (2001).

    Google Scholar 

  27. Blanchard, F. A., Crandall, C. S., Brigham, J. C. & Vaughn, L. A. Condemning and condoning racism: a social context approach to interracial settings. J. Appl. Psychol. 79, 993–997 (1994).

    Google Scholar 

  28. Crandall, C. S., Eshleman, A. & O’Brien, L. Social norms and the expression and suppression of prejudice: the struggle for internalization. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 359–378 (2002).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Legault, L., Gutsell, J. N. & Inzlicht, M. Ironic effects of antiprejudice messages: how motivational interventions can reduce (but also increase) prejudice. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1472–1477 (2011).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Paluck, E. L., Shepherd, H. & Aronow, P. M. Changing climates of conflict: a social network experiment in 56 schools. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 566–571 (2016).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Kaiser, C. R. et al. Presumed fair: ironic effects of organizational diversity structures. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 104, 504–519 (2013).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Campbell, M. R. & Brauer, M. Incorporating social marketing insights into prejudice research: advancing theory and demonstrating real-world applications. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 15, 608–629 (2020).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Cohen, J. A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 112, 155–159 (1992).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Kirk, R. E. Experimental Design: Procedures for the Behavioral Sciences (SAGE, 2013).

  35. Cumming, G. The new statistics: why and how. Psychol. Sci. 25, 7–29 (2014).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Campbell, M. R. & Brauer, M. Is discrimination widespread? Testing assumptions about bias on a university campus. Preprint at PsyArXiv https://psyarxiv.com/evp8b/ (2020).

  37. Cialdini, R. B. Crafting normative messages to protect the environment. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 12, 105–109 (2003).

    Google Scholar 

  38. Blodorn, A., Major, B. & Kaiser, C. Perceived discrimination and poor health: accounting for self-blame complicates a well-established relationship. Soc. Sci. Med. 153, 27–34 (2016).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Bates, D., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S. & Baayen, R. H. Parsimonious mixed models. Preprint at arXiv https://arxiv.org/abs/1506.04967v2 (2015).

  40. Matuschek, H., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., Baayen, R. H. & Bates, D. Balancing type I error and power in linear mixed models. J. Mem. Lang. 94, 305–315 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  41. Wasserstein, R. L., Schirm, A. L. & Lazar, N. A. Moving to a world beyond “p < .05.”. Am. Stat. 73, 1–19 (2019).

    Google Scholar 

  42. Sparkman, G. & Walton, G. M. Dynamic norms promote sustainable behavior, even if it is counternormative. Psychol. Sci. 28, 1663–1674 (2017).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Gerber, A. & Rogers, T. Descriptive social norms and motivation to vote: everybody’s voting and so should you. J. Politics 71, 1–14 (2009).

    Google Scholar 

  44. McConahay, J. B. in Prejudice, Discrimination, and Racism (eds Dovidio, J. F. & Gaertner, S. L.) 91–125. (Academic Press, 1986).

  45. Plant, E. A. & Devine, P. G. Internal and external motivation to respond without prejudice. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 75, 811–832 (1998).

    Google Scholar 

  46. Blanchard, F. A., Lilly, T. & Vaughn, L. A. Reducing the expression of racial prejudice. Psychol. Sci. 2, 101–105 (1991).

    Google Scholar 

  47. Waghorn, G., Chant, D. & King, R. Classifying socially-valued role functioning among community residents with psychiatric disorders. Am. J. Psychiatr. Rehab. 10, 185–221 (2007).

    Google Scholar 

  48. Er-rafiy, A. & Brauer, M. Modifying perceived variability: four laboratory and filed experiments show the effectiveness of a ready-to-be-used prejudice intervention. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 43, 840–853 (2013).

    Google Scholar 

  49. S. Alfieri, S. & Marta, E. Positive attitudes toward the outgroup: adaptation and validation of the Allophilia scale. Test. Psychom. Methodol. Appl. Psychol. 18, 99–116 (2011).

    Google Scholar 

  50. Williams, D. R., Neighbors, H. W. & Jackson, J. S. Racial/ethnic discrimination and health: findings from community studies. Am. J. Public Health 93, 200–208 (2003).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to the university instructors who allowed us to test our intervention in their classrooms. We thank J. Schwakopf, who played an important role coordinating this research and recruiting instructors to participate. We also thank G. Walton for his feedback on earlier versions of this manuscript. This research was partially supported by the Office of the Vice Provost and Chief Diversity Officer and grant no. 150 PRJ73DX by the College of Letters & Sciences (both University of Wisconsin-Madison). The video was created with the help of M. Mederson. The funders had no role in study conceptualization and design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

M.B. and S.M. jointly conceived the research and decided on the stimulus material and outcome measures. S.M. created the first drafts of the poster and video and collected data for experiments 1–4. M.C. collected data for experiments 5 and 6. All authors contributed to the analyses of the data. All authors participated in the writing and revision of the paper.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Markus Brauer.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Peer review information Primary Handling Editors: Mary Elizabeth Sutherland; Aisha Bradshaw.

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data

Extended Data Fig. 1 Effect size estimates and the results of the internal meta-analysis for Experiments 1–5.

Points represent standardized mean difference effect size estimates. The lines around the points represent the 95% confidence intervals. The overall effect of the social norms manipulations was .33, indicating that the inclusive climate score of students in the social norms conditions was on average .33 standard deviation units higher than that of students in the control conditions.

Extended Data Fig. 2 Means and standard deviations broken down by condition, as well as inferential statistics for the outcome variables assessed in Experiment 1.

(a) IMS = Internal Motivation to Respond without Prejudice, EMS = External Motivation to Respond without Prejudice Note: All variables have been recoded so that higher values express greater Inclusive Climate. “(R)” denotes scales that have been inverted from their original scoring to fit this standard. “95% CI” refers to the 95% confidence interval for the effect.

Extended Data Fig. 3 Means and standard deviations broken down by condition, as well as inferential statistics for the outcome variables assessed in Experiment 2.

Note: “95% CI” refers to the 95% confidence interval for the effect.

Extended Data Fig. 4 Means and standard deviations broken down by condition, as well as inferential statistics for the outcome variables assessed in Experiment 3.

Note: “95% CI” refers to the 95% confidence interval for the effect.

Extended Data Fig. 5 Means and standard deviations broken down by condition, as well as inferential statistics for the outcome variables assessed in Experiment 4.

Note: “95% CI” refers to the 95% confidence interval for the effect.

Extended Data Fig. 6 Mediation analyses conducted in Experiments 4 and 5. The effect of the social norms video is mediated by participants’ perceptions of peer norms.

Note: The confidence intervals were computed with 1,000 bootstrapped samples. “95% CI” refers to the 95% confidence interval for the effect.

Extended Data Fig. 7 Means and standard deviations broken down by condition, as well as inferential statistics for the entire sample (both privileged and marginalized students) in Experiment 5.

Note: “Omnibus test” is the two-degree of freedom F test for the condition variable. The “Bias versus Control” and “Social Norms versus Control” columns show the inferential statistics obtained with dummy codes representing these two comparisons. All scales are scored such that higher values indicate more positivity/inclusiveness. “(R)” denotes scales that have been inverted from their original scoring to fit this standard.

Extended Data Fig. 8 Means and standard deviations broken down by condition, as well as inferential statistics for students from marginalized groups only in Experiment 5.

Note: “Omnibus test” is the two degree of freedom F test for the condition variable. The “Bias versus Control” and “Social Norms versus Control” columns show the inferential statistics obtained with dummy codes representing these two comparisons. All scales are scored such that higher values indicate more positivity/inclusiveness. “(R)” denotes scales that have been inverted from their original scoring to fit this standard.

Extended Data Fig. 9 Parameter estimates and inferential statistics for the entire sample (privileged and marginalized students) in Experiment 5 when accounting for the non-independence due to classroom.

Note: We estimated a linear mixed-effects model in which we regressed the outcome variable on the two dummy codes (see Experiment 5 Results) and included a by-classroom random intercept. These results should be interpreted with caution, because 9 (out of 51, that is, 18%) classrooms had 2 or fewer respondents and 19 (37%) had 5 or fewer respondents, leading to substantial volatility in the classroom means (and thus relatively large standard errors of the parameter estimates). With error degrees of freedom around 40, the inferential tests are underpowered. All scales are scored such that higher values indicate more positivity/inclusiveness. “(R)” denotes scales that have been inverted from their original scoring to fit this standard.

Extended Data Fig. 10 Parameter estimates and inferential statistics for students from marginalized groups only in Experiment 5 when accounting for the non-independence due to classroom.

Note: We estimated a linear mixed-effects model in which we regressed the outcome variable on the two dummy codes (see Experiment 5 Results) and included a by-classroom random intercept. These results should be interpreted with caution, because 17 (out of 41, that is, 41%) classrooms with data for students from marginalized groups had 2 or fewer respondents and 28 (68%) had 5 or fewer respondents, leading to substantial volatility in the classroom means (and thus relatively large standard errors of the parameter estimates). All scales are scored such that higher values indicate more positivity/inclusiveness. “(R)” denotes scales that have been inverted from their original scoring to fit this standard.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

Correlation Tables 1–5, experimental materials and outcome measures.

Reporting Summary

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Murrar, S., Campbell, M.R. & Brauer, M. Exposure to peers’ pro-diversity attitudes increases inclusion and reduces the achievement gap. Nat Hum Behav 4, 889–897 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0899-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0899-5

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing