Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

The automatic influence of advocacy on lawyers and novices

This article has been updated

Abstract

It has long been known that advocating for a cause can alter the advocate’s beliefs. Yet a guiding assumption of many advocates is that the biasing effect of advocacy is controllable. Lawyers, for instance, are taught that they can retain unbiased beliefs while advocating for their clients and that they must do so to secure just outcomes. Across ten experiments (six preregistered; N = 3,104) we show that the biasing effect of advocacy is not controllable but automatic. Merely incentivizing people to advocate altered a range of beliefs about character, guilt and punishment. This bias appeared even in beliefs that are highly stable, when people were financially incentivized to form true beliefs and among professional lawyers, who are trained to prevent advocacy from biasing their judgements.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Regression coefficients and 95% CIs corresponding to the influence of advocacy incentives on all dependent measures across all experiments.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available at https://osf.io/ewv2a/.

Code availability

The custom code that supports the findings of this study is available at https://osf.io/ewv2a/.

Change history

  • 18 September 2020

    In the version of this article originally published, there was a typographic error in the last sentence of the second paragraph. This has been corrected in the HTML and PDF versions.

References

  1. Kahneman, D. Thinking, Fast and Slow (Macmillan, 2011).

  2. Loewenstein, G., Issacharoff, S., Camerer, C. & Babcock, L. Self-serving assessments of fairness and pretrial bargaining. J. Leg. Stud. 22, 135–159 (1993).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Plato. (transl. Waterford, R.) Gorgias (Oxford Univ. Press, 1998).

  4. Deflem, M. Sociology of Law: Visions of a Scholarly Tradition (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2008).

  5. Perlman, A. M. A behavioral theory of legal ethics. Indiana Law J. 90, 1639–1669 (2015).

    Google Scholar 

  6. Kahan, D. M. Laws of cognition and the cognition of law. Cognition 135, 56–60 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Krieger, L. S. Institutional denial about the dark side of law school, and fresh empirical guidance for constructively breaking the silence. J. Leg. Educ. 52 (2002).

  8. Bargh, J. A. in Handbook of Social Cognition Vol. 1 (eds Wyer, R. S. & Srull, T. K.) 1–40 (Erlbaum, 1994).

  9. Bargh, J. A. in Unintended Thought (eds Uleman, J. S. & Bargh, J. A.) 3–51 (Guilford, 1989).

  10. Melnikoff, D. E. & Bargh, J. A. The mythical number two. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 280–293 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Moors, A. & De Houwer, J. Automaticity: a theoretical and conceptual analysis. Psychol. Bull. 132, 297–326 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Bem, D. J. Self-perception: an alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychol. Rev. 74, 183–200 (1967).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Festinger, L. & Carlsmith, J. M. Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 58, 203–210 (1959).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Higgins, E. T. & Rholes, W. S. Saying is believing: effects of message modification on memory and liking for the person described. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 14, 363–378 (1978).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Thompson, L. & Loewenstein, G. Egocentric interpretations of fairness and interpersonal conflict. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 51, 176–197 (1992).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Thompson, L. “They saw a negotiation”: partisanship and involvement. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 68, 839–853 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T. & Schwarzbach, J. Different time courses for visual perception and action priming. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 6275–6280 (2003).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Maniscalco, B., Bang, J. W., Iravani, L., Camps-Febrer, F. & Lau, H. Does response interference depend on the subjective visibility of flanker distractors? Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 74, 841–851 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Janis, I. L. & Gilmore, J. B. The influence of incentive conditions on the success of role playing in modifying attitudes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1, 17–27 (1965).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Fodor, J. A. The Modularity of Mind (MIT Press, 1983).

  21. Melnikoff, D. E. & Bailey, A. H. Preferences for moral vs. immoral traits in others are conditional. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115, E592–E600 (2018).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Ratner, K. G., Kaul, C. & Van Bavel, J. J. Is race erased? Decoding race from patterns of neural activity when skin color is not diagnostic of group boundaries. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 750–755 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. De Frietas, J., Cikara, M., Grossman, I. & Schlegel, R. Origins of the belief in good true selves. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 634–636 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Strohminger, N., Knobe, J. & Newman, G. The true self: a psychological concept distinct from the self. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 551–560 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. De Frietas, J. & Cikara, M. Deep down my enemy is good: thinking about the true self reduces intergroup bias. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 74, 307–316 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. De Frietas, J. et al. Consistent belief in a good true self in misanthropes and three interdependent cultures. Cogn. Sci. 42, 134–160 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Newman, G. E., De Frietas, J. & Knobe, J. Beliefs about the true self explain asymmetries based on moral judgment. Cogn. Sci. 39, 96–125 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Hogarth, R. Judgment and Choice 2nd edn (Wiley, 1987).

  29. Kruglanski, A. W. Lay Epistemics and Human Knowledge: Cognitive and Motivational Bases (Plenum, 1989).

  30. Shiffrin, R. M. & Schneider, W. Controlled and automatic human information processing II: perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychol. Rev. 84, 127–190 (1977).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Newman, G., Bloom, P. & Knobe, J. Value judgments and the true self. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 40, 203–216 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Haidt, J. The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychol. Rev. 108, 814–834 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Tetlock, P. E. et al. People as intuitive prosecutors: the impact of social-control goals on attributions of responsibility. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 43, 195–209 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Baumeister, R. F. & Newman, L. S. Self-regulation of cognitive inference and decision processes. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 20, 3–19 (1994).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Bates, D., Machler, M., Bolker, B. M. & Walker, S. C. Fitting linear mixed effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Moss, A. J. & Litman, L. After the bot scare: understanding what’s been happening with data collection on MTurk and how to stop it. Cloudprime https://blog.turkprime.com/after-the-bot-scare-understanding-whats-been-happening-with-data-collection-on-mturk-and-how-to-stop-it (2018).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank P. Ellsworth, A. Shanor, J. Sun, A. Irgens, C. Cusimano and L. Drayton for contributions that aided in the development of this manuscript. We declare no specific funding for this work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

D.E.M. and N.S. conceptualized, designed and performed the research and wrote the paper. D.E.M. analysed the data.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David E. Melnikoff.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Peer review information Primary Handling Editor: Charlotte Payne.

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Tables 1 and 2, Supplementary Methods and Supplementary Results.

Reporting Summary

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Melnikoff, D.E., Strohminger, N. The automatic influence of advocacy on lawyers and novices. Nat Hum Behav 4, 1258–1264 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-00943-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-00943-3

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing