Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Neural dynamics of racial categorization predicts racial bias in face recognition and altruism

Abstract

The classification of individuals into different racial groups provides a precondition for racial bias in cognition and behaviour, but how the brain enables spontaneous racial categorization is not fully understood. Here using multimodal brain imaging measures, including electroencephalography, functional magnetic resonance imaging and magnetoencephalography, we probe the neural dynamics of racial categorization by quantifying the repetition suppression of neural responses to faces of different individuals of each racial group (that is, Asian, black or white). We show that categorization of other-race faces engages early two-stage dynamic activities in neural networks consisting of multiple interactive brain regions. Categorization of same-race faces, however, recruits a different and simple network in a later time window. Dynamic neural activities involved in racial categorization predict racial biases in face recognition and altruistic intention. These results suggest that there are distinct neural dynamics by which the brain sorts people into different racial groups as a social ground for cognition and action.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Stimuli and behavioural results.
Fig. 2: ERP results of Chinese participants in experiment 1a.
Fig. 3: Results of Chinese participants in experiment 2.
Fig. 4: ERP results of white participants in experiment 3.
Fig. 5: fMRI results of Chinese participants in experiments 5a and 5b.
Fig. 6: MEG results of Chinese participants in experiment 6.
Fig. 7: Dynamic neural models of racial categorization revealed in experiment 6.
Fig. 8: Coupling between neural categorization of OR faces and racial biases in cognition and altruistic intension in experiment 6.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Code availability

The code used to analyse the data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request

References

  1. Pager, D. & Shepherd, H. The sociology of discrimination: racial discrimination in employment, housing, credit, and consumer markets. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 34, 181–209 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Goyal, M. K., Kuppermann, N., Cleary, S. D., Teach, S. J. & Chamberlain, J. M. Racial disparities in pain management of children with appendicitis in emergency departments. JAMA Pediatr. 169, 996–1002 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Anwar, S., Bayer, P. & Hjalmarsson, R. The impact of jury race in criminal trials. Q. J. Econ. 127, 1017–1055 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Wetts, R. & Willer, R. Privilege on the precipice: perceived racial status threats lead White Americans to oppose welfare programs. Soc. Forces 97, 793–822 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Meissner, C. A. & Brigham, J. C. Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Pub. Policy Law 7, 3–35 (2001).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Baron, A. S. & Banaji, M. R. The development of implicit attitudes: evidence of race evaluations from ages 6 and 10 and adulthood. Psychol. Sci. 17, 53–58 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. McConnell, A. R. & Leibold, J. M. Relations among the implicit association test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit measures of racial attitudes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 37, 435–442 (2001).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Xu, X., Zuo, X., Wang, X. & Han, S. Do you feel my pain? Racial group membership modulates empathic neural responses. J. Neurosci. 29, 8525–8529 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Avenanti, A., Sirigu, A. & Aglioti, S. M. Racial bias reduces empathic sensorimotor resonance with other-race pain. Curr. Biol. 20, 1018–1022 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Han, S. Neurocognitive basis of racial ingroup bias in empathy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 400–421 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Ge, L. et al. Two faces of the other-race effect: recognition and categorisation of Caucasian and Chinese faces. Perception 38, 1199–1210 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Qian, M. K. et al. Implicit racial biases in preschool children and adults from Asia and Africa. Child Dev. 87, 285–296 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Sheng, F. & Han, S. Manipulations of cognitive strategies and intergroup relationships reduce the racial bias in empathic neural responses. NeuroImage 61, 786–797 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Sheng, F., Han, X. & Han, S. Dissociated neural representations of pain expressions of different races. Cereb. Cortex 26, 1221–1233 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Cosmides, L., Tooby, J. & Kurzban, R. Perceptions of race. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 173–179 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Kawakami, K., Amodio, D. M. & Hugenberg, K. Intergroup perception and cognition: an integrative framework for understanding the causes and consequences of social categorization. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 55, 1–80 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  17. Gallagher, C. In Theories of Race and Ethnicity (eds Murji, K. & Solomos, J.) 40–56 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2014).

  18. Levin, D. T. Race as a visual feature: using visual search and perceptual discrimination tasks to understand face categories and the cross-race recognition deficit. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 129, 559–574 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Hugenberg, K., Young, S. G., Bernstein, M. J. & Sacco, D. F. The categorization-individuation model: an integrative account of the other-race recognition deficit. Psychol. Rev. 117, 1168–1187 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Ito, T. A. & Urland, G. R. Race and gender on the brain: electrocortical measures of attention to the race and gender of multiply categorizable individuals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 616–626 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Ito, T. A. & Urland, G. R. The influence of processing objectives on the perception of faces: an ERP study of race and gender perception. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 5, 21–36 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Kubota, J. T. & Ito, T. A. Multiple cues in social perception: the time course of processing race and facial expression. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 43, 738–752 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Ito, T. A. & Bartholow, B. D. The neural correlates of race. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 524–531 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Wiese, H. The role of age and ethnic group in face recognition memory: ERP evidence from a combined own-age and own-race bias study. Biol. Psychol. 89, 137–147 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Senholzi, K. B. & Ito, T. A. Structural face encoding: how task affects the N170’s sensitivity to race. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 937–942 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Lieberman, M. D., Hariri, A., Jarcho, J. M., Eisenberger, N. I. & Bookheimer, S. Y. An fMRI investigation of race-related amygdala activity in African–American and Caucasian–American individuals. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 720–722 (2005).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Phelps, E. A. et al. Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 729–738 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Golby, A. J., Gabrieli, J. D., Chiao, J. Y. & Eberhardt, J. L. Differential responses in the fusiform region to same-race and other-race faces. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 845–450 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Richeson, J. A. et al. An fMRI investigation of the impact of interracial contact on executive function. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 1323–1328 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Iidaka, T., Nogawa, J., Kansaku, K. & Sadato, N. Neural correlates involved in processing happy affect on same race faces. J. Psychophysiol. 22, 91–99 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Cunningham, W. A. et al. Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces. Psychol. Sci. 15, 806–813 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Song, M. Introduction: who’s at the bottom? Examining claims about racial hierarchy. Ethnic Racial Stud. 27, 859–877 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Sadler, M. S., Correll, J., Park, B. & Judd, C. M. The world is not black and white: racial bias in the decision to shoot in a multiethnic context. J. Soc. Issues 68, 286–313 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Gross, T. F. Own-ethnicity bias in the recognition of Black, East Asian, Hispanic, and White faces. Basic Appl. Soc. Psych. 31, 128–135 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Young, A. W. Faces, people and the brain: the 45th Sir Frederic Bartlett Lecture. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 71, 569–594 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Kubota, J. T., Banaji, M. R. & Phelps, E. A. The neuroscience of race. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 940–948 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Grill-Spector, K., Henson, R. & Martin, A. Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 14–23 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Ringo, J. L. Stimulus specific adaptation in inferior temporal and medial temporal cortex of the monkey. Behav. Brain Res. 76, 191–197 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Vizioli, L., Rousselet, G. A. & Caldara, R. Neural repetition suppression to identity is abolished by other-race faces. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 20081–20086 (2010).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Heisz, J. J., Watter, S. & Shedden, J. M. Automatic face identity encoding at the N170. Vis. Res. 46, 4604–4614 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Baillet, S. Magnetoencephalography for brain electrophysiology and imaging. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 327–339 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Fiske, S. T. & Neuberg, S. L. A continuum of impression formation, from category-based to individuating processes: influences of information and motivation on attention and interpretation. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 23, 1–73 (1990).

    Google Scholar 

  43. Eberhardt, J. L., Goff, P. A., Purdie, V. J. & Davies, P. G. Seeing black: race, crime, and visual processing. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 876–893 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Plant, E. A., Goplen, J. & Kunstman, J. W. Selective responses to threat: the roles of race and gender in decisions to shoot. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bullet. 37, 1274–1281 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Wilson, J. P., Hugenberg, K. & Rule, N. O. Racial bias in judgments of physical size and formidability: from size to threat. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 113, 59–80 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Ha, S. E. & Jang, S. J. Immigration, threat perception, and national identity: evidence from South Korea. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 44, 53–62 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Maddox, K. B. & Gray, S. A. Cognitive representations of Black Americans: reexploring the role of skin tone. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bullet. 28, 250–259 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Hayward, W. G., Rhodes, G. & Schwaninger, A. An own-race advantage for components as well as configurations in face recognition. Cognition 106, 1017–1027 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Rossion, B. & Gauthier, I. How does the brain process upright and inverted faces? Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 1, 63–75 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G. & Buchner, A. G* Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences 2nd edn (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1988).

  52. Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E. & Schwartz, J. L. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 1464–1480 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A. & Banaji, M. R. Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 197–216 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Han, S. et al. Empathic neural responses to others’ pain are modulated by emotional contexts. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 3227–3237 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Barth, F. Ethnic Groups and Boundaries: The Social Organization of Culture Difference (Waveland Press, 1998).

  56. Quinn, P. C., Lee, K. & Pascalis, O. Perception of face race by infants: five developmental changes. Child Dev. Perspect. 12, 204–209 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Byatt, G. & Rhodes, G. Identification of own-race and other-race faces: implications for the representation of race in face space. Psychol. Bull. Rev. 11, 735–741 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Stolier, R. M. & Freeman, J. B. Functional and temporal considerations for top-down influences in social perception. Psychol. Inq. 27, 352–357 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Freeman, J. B. & Johnson, K. L. More than meets the eye: split-second social perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 362–374 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Kanwisher, N. & Yovel, G. The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 361, 2109–2128 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Rice, G. E., Lambon Ralph, M. A. & Hoffman, P. The roles of left versus right anterior temporal lobes in conceptual knowledge: an ALE meta-analysis of 97 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb. Cortex 25, 4374–4391 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Mitchell, J. P., Heatherton, T. F. & Macrae, C. N. Distinct neural systems subserve person and object knowledge. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, 15238–15243 (2002).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Amodio, D. M. The neuroscience of prejudice and stereotyping. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 670–682 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Binder, J. R. & Desai, R. H. The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 527–536 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Gilbert, S. J., Swencionis, J. K. & Amodio, D. M. Evaluative vs. trait representation in intergroup social judgments: distinct roles of anterior temporal lobe and prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia 50, 3600–3611 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Van Overwalle, F. Social cognition and the brain: a meta‐analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 829–858 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Van der Cruyssen, L., Heleven, E., Ma, N., Vandekerckhove, M. & Van Overwalle, F. Distinct neural correlates of social categories and personality traits. NeuroImage 104, 336–346 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C. M. & Wittenbrink, B. The police officer’s dilemma: using ethnicity to disambiguate potentially threatening individuals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 83, 1314–1329 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Wiese, H., Kaufmann, J. M. & Schweinberger, S. R. The neural signature of the own-race bias: evidence from event-related potentials. Cereb. Cortex 24, 826–835 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Michel, C., Caldara, R. & Rossion, B. Same-race faces are perceived more holistically than other-race faces. Vis. Cog. 14, 55–73 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Hughes, B. L. et al. Neural adaptation to faces reveals racial outgroup homogeneity effects in early perception. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 116, 14532–14537 (2019).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Nestor, A., Plaut, D. C. & Behrmann, M. Unraveling the distributed neural code of facial identity through spatiotemporal pattern analysis. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 9998–10003 (2011).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Freeman, J. B., Schiller, D., Rule, N. O. & Ambady, N. The neural origins of superficial and individuated judgments about ingroup and outgroup members. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 150–159 (2010).

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Stolier, R. M. & Freeman, J. B. Neural pattern similarity reveals the inherent intersection of social categories. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 795–797 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Strohminger, N. et al. The MR2: a multi-racial, mega-resolution database of facial stimuli. Behav. Res. Methods 48, 1197–1204 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Ma, D. S., Correll, J. & Wittenbrink, B. The Chicago face database: a free stimulus set of faces and norming data. Behav. Res Methods 47, 1122–1135 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Valentine, T. & Endo, M. Towards an exemplar model of face processing: the effects of race and distinctiveness. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 44, 671–703 (1992).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  78. Johnston, R. A., Kanazawa, M., Kato, T. & Oda, M. Exploring the structure of multidimensional face-space: the effects of age and gender. Vis. Cogn. 4, 39–57 (1997).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Semlitsch, H. V., Anderer, P., Schuster, P. & Presslich, O. A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. Psychophysiology 23, 695–703 (1986).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Luck, S. J. & Gaspelin, N. How to get statistically significant effects in any ERP experiment (and why you shouldn’t). Psychophysiology 54, 146–157 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Eklund, A., Nichols, T. E. & Knutsson, H. Cluster failure: why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 7900–7905 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Xia, M., Wang, J. & He, Y. BrainNet Viewer: a network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS One 8, e68910 (2013).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Tadel, F., Baillet, S., Mosher, J. C., Pantazis, D. & Leahy, R. M. Brainstorm: a user-friendly application for MEG/EEG analysis. Comput. Intel. Neurosci. 8, 879716 (2011).

    Google Scholar 

  84. Maris, E. & Oostenveld, R. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. J. Neurosci. Methods 164, 177–190 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Blanco-Elorrieta, E., Emmorey, K. & Pylkkänen, L. Language switching decomposed through MEG and evidence from bimodal bilinguals. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115, 9708–9713 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  86. Blanco-Elorrieta, E. & Pylkkänen, L. Bilingual language switching in the laboratory versus in the wild: the spatiotemporal dynamics of adaptive language control. J. Neurosci. 37, 9022–9036 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  87. Fischl, B. et al. Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex 14, 11–22 (2004).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Desikan, R. S. et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage 31, 968–980 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Barnett, L. & Seth, A. K. The MVGC multivariate Granger causality toolbox: a new approach to Granger-causal inference. J. Neurosci. Methods 223, 50–68 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Seth, A. K. A MATLAB toolbox for Granger causal connectivity analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 186, 262–273 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Iwasaki, M., Noguchi, Y. & Kakigi, R. Neural correlates of time distortion in a preaction period. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 804–817 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Morey, R. D., & Rouder, J. N. BayesFactor: Computation of Bayes Factors for common designs. R package version 0.9.11-11 (2015).

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (projects 31661143039, 31421003 and 31871134). The authors thank J. Sheng, S. Shu, Z. Liu, L. Liu, X. Tian, H. Luo, N. Ding and J. Gao for technical assistance and D. Pfabigan for proofreading the manuscript. The funder had no role in the conceptualization, design, data collection, analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

S.H. and Y.Z. conceived the research programme and designed the experiments. Y.Z., T.G., T.Z., W.L., T.W., X.H. and S.H. carried out the experiments. Y.Z., X.H. and S.H. analysed the data. S.H. and Y.Z. wrote the paper. S.H. supervised the entire work.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shihui Han.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Peer review information Primary handling editor: Mary Elizabeth Sutherland.

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Extended data

Extended Data Fig. 1 Statistical results of P2 and N2 amplitudes in experiment 1A.

aNote: Effect size is indexed as the partial eta-squared value. The 90% CIs are reported for partial eta-squared values.

Extended Data Fig. 2 Statistical results of P2 and N2 amplitudes in experiment 1B.

aNote: Effect size is indexed as the partial eta-squared value. The 90% CIs are reported for partial eta-squared values.

Extended Data Fig. 3 Statistical results of P2 and N2 amplitudes in experiment 2.

aNote: Effect size is indexed as the partial eta-squared value. FG: Face gender. The reported for partial eta-squared values.

Extended Data Fig. 4 Statistical results of P2 and N2 amplitudes in experiment 3.

aNote: Effect size is indexed as the partial eta-squared value. FG: Face gender. The reported for partial eta-squared values.

Extended Data Fig. 5 Statistical results of P2 and N2 amplitudes in experiment 4.

aNote: Effect size is indexed as the partial eta-squared value. FG: Face gender. The reported for partial eta-squared values.

Extended Data Fig. 6 Model structures and results of the DCM analysis in experiment 5a.

(a) Illustration of individual models in the DCM model space. The models were different in the region that driving vision input were assigned to and the intrinsic connectivity between regions. Only the intrinsic connectivity between brain regions and the driving visual input were plotted. The modulation effect of on each intrinsic connectivity and the intrinsic connectivity within each regions of the single models were omitted. (b) The exceedance probabilities of single models of Black and White faces. Model 2 marked inside the red square had the highest exceedance probability for both Black and White faces. (c) Strength of the intrinsic and modulatory connectivity estimated based on Model 2 for Black and White faces, respectively. Repetition of Black faces significantly modulated within-region connectivity in both mPFC and PCC (N=53; mPFC to mPFC: t(52) = −3.06, p = 0.003, Cohen’s D = 0.42, 95% CI = −0.27, −0.06, PCC to PCC : t(52) = −3.34, p = 0.002, Cohen’s D = 0.46, 95% CI = −0.12, −0.03) and between-region connectivity from mPFC to PCC (t(52) = 6.12, p < 0.001, Cohen’s D = 0.84, 95% CI = 0.30, 0.60). By contrast, repetition of White faces only significantly modulated within- region connectivity for PCC (t(52) = −2.10, p = 0.041, Cohen’s D = 0.29, 95% CI = −0.06, −0.001). *p<0.05,** p<0.01,*** p<0.001.

Extended Data Fig. 7

RTs and accuracies (mean ± SD) in experiment 6.

Extended Data Fig. 8

Results of MEG sensor-space signals in experiment 6. The left panel shows the magnetometer sensors that showed the strongest neural responses at 140–200 ms across all conditions (p <0.05, FDR corrected). The middle panel shows mean MEG responses over these sensors to OR-faces and SR-faces. The right bar charts illustrate the mean RS effects in sensor-space MEG signals (Alt-Cond > Rep-Cond) by showing quartiles (boxes), means (square inside boxes), medians (horizontal lines inside boxes), maximum and minimum excluding outliers(whiskers), and outliers (diamonds). ANOVAs of the mean sensor-space magnetic responses at 140–200 ms confirmed a larger RS effect for OR-(collapsing Black and White faces) than SR- (Asian) faces (N=26; F(1,25) = 6.663, p = 0.016, ηp = 0.210, 90% CI = 0.02, 0.41).

Extended Data Fig. 9 MEG results of ROI analyses in experiment 6.

Based on our fMRI results, we predicted stronger RS effects on source-space signals in PCC to OR- than SR-faces. To test this, we extracted time courses of MEG signals in the PCC (3/-67/25) identified in our fMRI results (shown in the left panel) and compared the RS effects (that is. Alt-Cond vs. Rep-Cond) between OR-faces and SR-faces at each time points. Cluster based permutation tests (one-tailed) were conducted across time points using a priori cluster threshold p < 0.025, 10,000 iterations. This test yielded a significant cluster showing greater RS effect for OR-faces than SR-faces at 266–305 ms in PCC (N=26; cluster p = 0.028), as illustrated in the middle and right panels. a.u.= arbitrary unit.

Extended Data Fig. 10 Coupling between neural categorization of OR-faces and racial biases in cognition and altruistic intension in experiment 6.

(A)The time course of Spearman rank correlations between RS of LFG (yellow line) and LATL (blue line) activity and false alarm rates during face recognition. Correlations were calculated point-by-point at 140–200 ms. Significant correlations were observed at 170- 185 ms for LFG activity and at 187–200 ms for LATL activity (p < 0.05, FDR corrected). Averaged RS effects on LFG (170–185 ms) and LATL (187–200 ms) activities predicted false alarms during recognition of Black (vs. Asian) faces (N=22; LFG: r = 0.570, p = 0.010, 95% CI = 0.18, 0.83; LATL: r = 0.554, p = 0.010, 95% CI = 0.08, 0.87, FDR corrected). (C) Averaged RS effects on LFG (170–185 ms) and LATL (187- 200 ms) activities predicted false alarms during recognition of Black faces (LFG: r = 0.435, p = 0.043, 95% CI = 0, 0.76; LATL: r = 0.428, p = 0.047, 95% CI = −0.06, 0.75, uncorrected).

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Methods 1, Supplementary Results 1–3, Supplementary Figs. 1–9 and Supplementary Tables 1–9.

Reporting Summary

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhou, Y., Gao, T., Zhang, T. et al. Neural dynamics of racial categorization predicts racial bias in face recognition and altruism. Nat Hum Behav 4, 69–87 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0743-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0743-y

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing