Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Age-related preference for geometric spatial cues during real-world navigation

Abstract

Ageing effects on spatial navigation are characterized mainly in terms of impaired allocentric strategies. However, an alternative hypothesis is that navigation difficulties in aged people are associated with deficits in processing and encoding spatial cues. We tested this hypothesis by studying how geometry and landmark cues control navigation in young and older adults in a real, ecological environment. Recordings of body and gaze dynamics revealed a preference for geometry-based navigation in older adults, and for landmark-based navigation in younger ones. While cue processing was associated with specific fixation patterns, advanced age manifested itself in a longer reorientation time, reflecting an unbalanced exploration–exploitation trade-off in scanning policies. Moreover, a battery of tests revealed a specific cognitive deficit in older adults with geometric preference. These results suggest that allocentric strategy deficits in ageing can result from difficulties related to landmark coding, and predict recovery of allocentric strategies in geometrically polarized environments.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: The real, ecological experimental set-up and the spatial task.
Fig. 2: Preference for geometric cues in older adults.
Fig. 3: Spatial navigation performance of young versus older adults during learning.
Fig. 4: Spatiotemporal signatures of visual fixational patterns predict landmark versus geometry cue preference.
Fig. 5: Spatial distribution of visual fixations as a function of cue preference.
Fig. 6: Age- and cue preference-related differences in visual fixation characteristics.
Fig. 7: Visual and cognitive correlates of geometric preference in ageing.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The datasets generated and analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

Code availability

All custom software can be found at http://www.aging-vision-action.fr/ava_files/silversight/custom.software.zip.

References

  1. Lester, A. W., Moffat, S. D., Wiener, J. M., Barnes, C. A. & Wolbers, T. The aging navigational system. Neuron 95, 1019–1035 (2017).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Moffat, S. D., Elkins, W. & Resnick, S. M. Age differences in the neural systems supporting human allocentric spatial navigation. Neurobiol. Aging 27, 965–972 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Bates, S. L. & Wolbers, T. How cognitive aging affects multisensory integration of navigational cues. Neurobiol. Aging 35, 2761–2769 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Kunz, L. et al. Reduced grid-cell-like representations in adults at genetic risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Science 350, 430–433 (2015).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Gazova, I. et al. Spatial navigation—a unique window into physiological and pathological aging. Front. Aging Neurosci. 4, 16 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Lord, S. R., Rogers, M. W., Howland, A. & Fitzpatrick, R. Lateral stability, sensorimotor function and falls in older people. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 47, 1077–1081 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Anderson, P. G., Nienhuis, B., Mulder, T. & Hulstijn, W. Are older adults more dependent on visual information in regulating self-motion than younger adults? J. Mot. Behav. 30, 104–113 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Lövdén, M., Schellenbach, M., Grossman-Hutter, B., Krüger, A. & Lindenberger, U. Environmental topography and postural control demands shape aging-associated decrements in spatial navigation performance. Psychol. Aging 20, 683–694 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Sekuler, R., Hutman, L. & Owsley, C. Human aging and spatial vision. Science 209, 1255–1256 (1980).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Warren, W. H., Blackwell, aW. & Morris, M. W. Age differences in perceiving the direction of self-motion from optical flow. J. Gerontol. 44, 147–153 (1989).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Mahmood, O., Adamo, D., Briceno, E. & Moffat, S. D. Age differences in visual path integration. Behav. Brain Res. 205, 88–95 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Klencklen, G., Després, O. & Dufour, A. What do we know about aging and spatial cognition? Reviews and perspectives. Ageing Res. Rev. 11, 123–135 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Eyler, L. T., Sherzai, A., Kaup, A. R. & Jeste, D. V. A review of functional brain imaging correlates of successful cognitive aging. Biol. Psychiatry 70, 115–122 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Konishi, K. & Bohbot, V. D. Spatial navigational strategies correlate with gray matter in the hippocampus of healthy older adults tested in a virtual maze. Front. Aging Neurosci. 5, 1–8 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Iaria, G., Palermo, L., Committeri, G. & Barton, J. J. S. Age differences in the formation and use of cognitive maps. Behav. Brain Res. 196, 187–191 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Harris, M. A., Wiener, J. M. & Wolbers, T. Aging specifically impairs switching to an allocentric navigational strategy. Front. Aging Neurosci. 4, 1–9 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Foo, P., Warren, W. H., Duchon, A. & Tarr, M. J. Do humans integrate routes into a cognitive map? Map- versus landmark-based navigation of novel shortcuts. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 31, 195–215 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Bosco, A., Picucci, L., Caffò, A. O., Lancioni, G. E. & Gyselinck, V. Assessing human reorientation ability inside virtual reality environments: the effects of retention interval and landmark characteristics. Cogn. Process. 9, 299–309 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Newcombe, N. S. & Ratliff, R. Reorienting when cues conflict: evidence for an adaptive-combination view. Psychol. Sci. 19, 1301–1307 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Wegman, J., Tyborowska, A. & Janzen, G. Encoding and retrieval of landmark-related spatial cues during navigation: an fMRI study. Hippocampus 24, 853–868 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Gallistel, C. R. The Organization of Learning (MIT Press, 1990).

  22. Cheng, K. A purely geometric module in the rat’s spatial representation. Cognition 23, 149–178 (1986).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Doeller, C. F. & Burgess, N. Distinct error-correcting and incidental learning of location relative to landmarks and boundaries. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 5909–5914 (2008).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Pearce, J. M. The 36th Sir Frederick Bartlett Lecture: an associative analysis of spatial learning. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 62, 1665–1684 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Presson, C. C. & Montello, D. R. Points of reference in spatial cognition. Stalking the elusive landmark. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 6, 378–381 (1988).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Hermer, L. & Spelke, E. A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children. Nature 370, 57–59 (1994).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Learmonth, A. E., Newcombe, N. S., Sheridan, N. & Jones, M. Why size counts: children’s spatial reorientation in large and small enclosures. Dev. Sci. 11, 414–426 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Hermer-Vazquez, L., Spelke, E. S. & Katsnelson, A. S. Sources of flexibility in human cognition: dual-task studies of space and language. Cogn. Psychol. 39, 3–36 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Cheng, K., Huttenlocher, J. & Newcombe, N. S. 25 years of research on the use of geometry in spatial reorientation: a current theoretical perspective. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 1033–1054 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Picucci, L., Caffò, A. O. & Bosco, A. Age and sex differences in a virtual version of the reorientation task. Cogn. Process. 10, 272–275 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Schuck, N. W., Doeller, C. F., Polk, T. A., Lindenberger, U. & Li, S.-C. Human aging alters the neural computation and representation of space. Neuroimage I, 141–150 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Adamo, D. E., Briceño, E. M., Sindone, Ja, Alexander, N. B. & Moffat, S. D. Age differences in virtual environment and real world path integration. Front. Aging Neurosci. 4, 26 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Harris, Ma & Wolbers, T. Ageing effects on path integration and landmark navigation. Hippocampus 22, 1770–1780 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Sturz, B. R., Kilday, Za & Bodily, K. D. Does constraining field of view prevent extraction of geometric cues for humans during virtual-environment reorientation? J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 39, 390–396 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Rolls, E. T. Spatial view cells and the representation of place in the primate hippocampus. Hippocampus 9, 467–480 (1999).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Ekstrom, A. D. Why vision is important to how we navigate. Hippocampus 25, 731–735 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Killian, N. J., Jutras, M. J. & Buffalo, E. A. A map of visual space in the primate entorhinal cortex. Nature 5, 3–6 (2012).

    Google Scholar 

  38. Epstein, R. & Kanwisher, N. A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature 392, 598–601 (1998).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Epstein, R. & Vass, L. Neural systems for landmark-based wayfinding in humans. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 369, 20120533 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Ekstrom, A. D. et al. Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation. Nature 425, 184–188 (2003).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Meister, M. L. R. & Buffalo, E. A. Getting directions from the hippocampus: the neural connection between looking and memory. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 134, 135–144 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Hamilton, Da, Johnson, T. E., Redhead, E. S. & Verney, S. P. Control of rodent and human spatial navigation by room and apparatus cues. Behav. Process. 81, 154–169 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Sandstrom, N. J., Kaufman, J. & Huettel, A. S. Males and females use different distal cues in a virtual environment navigation task. Cogn. Brain Res. 6, 351–360 (1998).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Brandstatt, K. L. & Voss, J. L. Age-related impairments in active learning and strategic visual exploration. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6, 19 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Uiga, L., Cheng, K. K. C., Wilson, M., Masters, R. S. W. & Capio, C. M. Acquiring visual information for locomotion by older adults: a systematic review. Ageing Res. Rev. 20, 24–34 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Wirth, S., Baraduc, P., Planté, A., Pinède, S. & Duhamel, J. R. Gaze-informed, task-situated representation of space in primate hippocampus during virtual navigation. PLoS Biol. 15, e2001045 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Gilroy, K. E. & Pearce, J. M. The role of local, distal, and global information in latent spatial learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Learn. Cogn. 40, 212–224 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Ball, K. K., Beard, B. L., Roenker, D. L., Miller, R. L. & Griggs, D. S. Age and visual search: expanding the useful field of view. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 5, 2210–2219 (1988).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Sekuler, A. B., Bennett, P. J. & Mamelak, M. Effects of aging on the useful field of view. Exp. Aging Res. 26, 103–120 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Scialfa, C. T., Thomas, D. M. & Joffe, K. M. Age differences in the useful field of view: an eye movement analysis. Optom. Vis. Sci. 71, 736–742 (1994).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Moffat, S. D., Kennedy, K. M., Rodrigue, K. M. & Raz, N. Extrahippocampal contributions to age differences in human spatial navigation. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1274–1282 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Hegarty, M. & Waller, D. A dissociation between mental rotation and perspective-taking spatial abilities. Intelligence 32, 175–191 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Bian, Z., Braunstein, M. L. & Andersen, G. J. The ground dominance effect in the perception of relative distance in 3-D scenes is mainly due to characteristics of the ground surface. Percept. Psychophys. 68, 1297–1309 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Kelly, D. M. & Spetch, M. L. Reorientation in a two-dimensional environment: I. Do adults encode the featural and geometric properties of a two-dimensional schematic of a room? J. Comp. Psychol. 118, 384–395 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Ferrara, K. & Park, S. Neural representation of scene boundaries. Neuropsychologia 89, 180–190 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Hermer, L. & Spelke, E. Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation. Cognition 61, 195–232 (1996).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Sheynikhovich, D., Chavarriaga, R., Strösslin, T., Arleo, A. & Gerstner, W. Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? Insights from a rodent navigation model. Psychol. Rev. 116, 540–566 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Cheng, K. & Newcombe, N. S. Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? Squaring theory and evidence. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12, 1–23 (2005).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Hermer-Vazquez, L., Moffet, A. & Munkholm, P. Language, space, and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans: the case of two spatial memory tasks. Cognition 79, 263–299 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Lindenberger, U. Human cognitive aging: corriger la fortune? Science 346, 572–578 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Lambrey, S., Doeller, C., Berthoz, A. & Burgess, N. Imagining being somewhere else: neural basis of changing perspective in space. Cereb. Cortex 22, 166–174 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Fields, A. W. & Shelton, A. L. Individual skill differences and large-scale environmental learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 32, 506–515 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Kozhevnikov, M., Motes, M. A., Rasch, B. & Blajenkova, O. Perspective-taking vs. mental rotation transformations and how they predict spatial navigation performance. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 20, 397–417 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Zhong, J. Y. & Moffat, S. D. Age-related differences in associative learning of landmarks and heading directions in a virtual navigation task. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8, 122 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Head, D. & Isom, M. Age effects on wayfinding and route learning skills. Behav. Brain Res. 209, 49–58 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Wilkniss, S. M., Jones, M. G., Korol, D. L., Gold, P. E. & Manning, C. A. Age-related differences in an ecologically based study of route learning. Psychol. Aging 12, 372–375 (1997).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Fon, G. J., Pitt, M. J. & Thies, A. C. Thoracic kyphosis: range in normal subjects. Am. J. Roentgenol. 134, 979–983 (1980).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  68. Itoh, N. & Fukuda, T. Comparative study of eye movements in extent of central and peripheral vision and use by young and elderly walkers. Percept. Mot. Skills 94, 1283–1291 (2002).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Rodgers, M. K., Sindone, J. A. & Moffat, S. D. Effects of age on navigation strategy. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 997–1003 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Bohbot, V. D. et al. Virtual navigation strategies from childhood to senescence: evidence for changes across the life span. Front. Aging Neurosci. 4, 28 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Colombo, D. et al. Egocentric and allocentric spatial reference frames in aging: a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 80, 605–621 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Klatzky, R. L. in Spatial Cognition. Lecture Notes in Computer Science Vol. 1404 (eds. Freksa, C. et al.) 1–17 (Springer, 1998).

  73. Arleo, A. & Rondi-Reig, L. Multimodal sensory integration and concurrent navigation strategies for spatial cognition in real and artificial organisms. J. Integr. Neurosci. 6, 327–366 (2007).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Burgess, N. Spatial memory: how egocentric and allocentric combine. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 551–557 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Ekstrom, A. D., Huffman, D. J. & Starrett, M. Interacting networks of brain regions underlie human spatial navigation: a review and novel synthesis of the literature. J. Neurophysiol. 118, 3328–3344 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Wang, R. F. Spatial updating and common misinterpretations of spatial reference frames. Spat. Cogn. Comput. 17, 222–249 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Klein, R. M. Inhibition of return. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 138–147 (2000).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Langley, L. K., Fuentes, L. J., Hochhalter, A. K., Brandt, J. & Overmier, J. B. Inhibition of return in aging and Alzheimers disease: performance as a function of task demands and stimulus timing. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 23, 431–446 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Castel, A. D., Chasteen, A. L., Scialfa, C. T. & Pratt, J. Adult age differences in the time course of inhibition of return. J. Gerontol. B 58, P256–P259 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Bullens, J., Iglói, K., Berthoz, A., Postma, A. & Rondi-Reig, L. Developmental time course of the acquisition of sequential egocentric and allocentric navigation strategies. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 107, 337–350 (2010).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Cowan, N., Naveh-Benjamin, M., Kilb, A. & Saults, J. S. Life-span development of visual working memory: when is feature binding difficult? Dev. Psychol. 42, 1089–1102 (2006).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Douaud, G. et al. A common brain network links development, aging, and vulnerability to disease. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, 17648–17653 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Pelz, J. B. & Canosa, R. Oculomotor behavior and perceptual strategies in complex tasks. Vis. Res. 41, 3587–3596 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Land, M. F. & Hayhoe, M. In what ways do eye movements contribute to everyday activities? Vis. Res. 41, 3559–3565 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Henderson, J. M., Hayes, T. R., Rehrig, G. & Ferreira, F. Meaning guides attention during real-world scene description. Sci. Rep. 8, 13504 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  86. Osborne, J. W. Improving your data transformations: applying the Box-Cox transformation. Pract. Assess. Res. Eval. 15, 1–9 (2010).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by ANR—Essilor SilverSight Chair No. ANR-14-CHIN-0001. The funders had no role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript. We thank S. Mohand-Said of the Clinical Investigation Centre of the Quinze-Vingts Hospital, Paris, for medical supervision during clinical screening of participants. We also thank K. Lagrené and A.-R. Bourefis, from the Aging in Vision and Action laboratory at Vision Institute, for helping in enrolling/profiling the participants and for assistance in performing the experiments, respectively. Finally, the authors wish to thank E. Gutman, J. Lebrun and C. Authié of the Streetlab team for technical support in setting up the experiments in the Streetlab platform.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

M.B., D.S., L.L.B., J.-A.S. and A.A. designed the experiment. M.B., G.T., C.P.A. and L.L.B. collected and analysed the data. M.B., D.S., C.P.A., J.-A.S. and A.A. wrote the article.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Marcia Bécu or Angelo Arleo.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Peer review information Primary handling editor: Stavroula Kousta.

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Results, Supplementary Discussion, Supplementary Figs. 1–12, Supplementary Tables 1 and 2, and Supplementary References.

Reporting Summary

Supplementary Video 1

Streetlab platform at the Vision Institute: an ecological street-like environment used to assess navigation under fully controlled conditions. The experimental environment consisted of a rectangular enclosure (aspect ratio, 1.99) with 19 realistic panels providing real-world wall textures that fully covered the walls of the enclosure. Both whole-body and eye motion were recorded (at 120 and 60 Hz, respectively) as subjects navigated around the obstacle-free environment.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bécu, M., Sheynikhovich, D., Tatur, G. et al. Age-related preference for geometric spatial cues during real-world navigation. Nat Hum Behav 4, 88–99 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0718-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0718-z

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing