Abstract
A fundamental but rarely contested assumption in economics and neuroeconomics is that decision-makers compute subjective values of risky options by multiplying functions of reward probability and magnitude. By contrast, an additive strategy for valuation allows flexible combination of reward information required in uncertain or changing environments. We hypothesized that the level of uncertainty in the reward environment should determine the strategy used for valuation and choice. To test this hypothesis, we examined choice between risky options in humans and rhesus macaques across three tasks with different levels of uncertainty. We found that whereas humans and monkeys adopted a multiplicative strategy under risk when probabilities are known, both species spontaneously adopted an additive strategy under uncertainty when probabilities must be learned. Additionally, the level of volatility influenced relative weighting of certain and uncertain reward information, and this was reflected in the encoding of reward magnitude by neurons in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
Access options
Subscribe to Journal
Get full journal access for 1 year
$99.00
only $8.25 per issue
All prices are NET prices.
VAT will be added later in the checkout.
Rent or Buy article
Get time limited or full article access on ReadCube.
from$8.99
All prices are NET prices.






Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.
Code availability
Custom computer codes that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.
References
- 1.
Bernoulli, D. Expositions of a new theory of the measurement of risk. Econometrica 22, 23–36 (1954).
- 2.
Edwards, W. The theory of decision making. Psychol. Bull. 51, 380 (1954).
- 3.
Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. On the psychology of prediction. Psych. Rev. 80, 237–251 (1973).
- 4.
Stewart, N. Information integration in risky choice: identification and stability. Front. Psychol. 2, 301 (2011).
- 5.
Ernst, M. O. & Banks, M. S. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 415, 429 (2002).
- 6.
Hunt, L. T., Dolan, R. J. & Behrens, T. E. Hierarchical competitions subserving multi-attribute choice. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 1613–1622 (2014).
- 7.
Farashahi, S., Rowe, K., Aslami, Z., Lee, D. & Soltani, A. Feature-based learning improves adaptability without compromising precision. Nat. Commun. 8, 1768 (2017).
- 8.
Farashahi, S. et al. Metaplasticity as a neural substrate for adaptive learning and choice under uncertainty. Neuron 94, 401–414 (2017).
- 9.
Spitmaan, M., Chu, E. & Soltani, A. Salience-driven value construction for adaptive choice under risk. J. Neurosci. 39, 5195–5209 (2019).
- 10.
Strait, C. E., Blanchard, T. C. & Hayden, B. Y. Reward value comparison via mutual inhibition in ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Neuron 82, 1357–1366 (2014).
- 11.
Farashahi, S., Azab, H., Hayden, B. & Soltani, A. On the flexibility of basic risk attitudes in monkeys. J. Neurosci. 38, 4383–4398 (2018).
- 12.
Hayden, B., Heilbronner, S. & Platt, M. Ambiguity aversion in rhesus macaques. Front. Neurosci. 4, 166 (2010).
- 13.
Donahue, C. H. & Lee, D. Dynamic routing of task-relevant signals for decision making in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 295–301 (2015).
- 14.
Massi, B., Donahue, C. H. & Lee, D. Volatility facilitates value updating in the prefrontal cortex. Neuron 99, 598–608 (2018).
- 15.
Stephan, K. E., Penny, W. D., Daunizeau, J., Moran, R. J. & Friston, K. J. Bayesian model selection for group studies. NeuroImage 46, 1005–1017 (2009); erratum 48, 311–311 (2009).
- 16.
Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W., Walton, M. E. & Rushworth, M. F. S. Learning the value of information in an uncertain world. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1214–1221 (2007).
- 17.
Tversky, A. Intransitivity of preferences. Psychol. Rev. 76, 31 (1969).
- 18.
Lichtenstein, S. & Slovic, P. The Construction of Preference (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2006).
- 19.
Ariely, D., Loewenstein, G. & Prelec, D. “Coherent arbitrariness”: stable demand curves without stable preferences. Q. J. Econ. 118, 73–106 (2003).
- 20.
Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G. & O’Donoghue, T. Time discounting and time preference: a critical review. J. Econ. Lit. 40, 351–401 (2002).
- 21.
Kolling, N., Wittmann, M. & Rushworth, M. F. Multiple neural mechanisms of decision making and their competition under changing risk pressure. Neuron 81, 1190–1202 (2014).
- 22.
Ferrari-Toniolo, S., Bujold, P. M. & Schultz, W. Probability distortion depends on choice sequence in rhesus monkeys. J. Neurosci. 39, 2915–2929 (2019).
- 23.
Hayden, B. Y. Time discounting and time preference in animals: a critical review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23, 39–53 (2016).
- 24.
Kennerley, S. W., Walton, M. E., Behrens, T. E. J., Buckley, M. J. & Rushworth, M. F. S. Optimal decision making and the anterior cingulate cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 940–947 (2006).
- 25.
Soltani, A. & Izquierdo, A. Adaptive learning under expected and unexpected uncertainty. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.1038/341583-019-0180-y (2019).
- 26.
Brainard, D. H. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat. Vis. 10, 433–436 (1997).
- 27.
Cornelissen, F. W., Peters, E. M. & Palmer, J. The Eyelink Toolbox: eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 34, 613–617 (2002).
Acknowledgements
We thank E. Chu, S. Nichols-Worley and L. Tran for collecting human data, and C. Strait and M. Mancarella for collecting monkey data in the gambling task. This work is supported by the National Science Foundation (CAREER Award no. BCS1253576 to B.Y.H. and EPSCoR Award no. 1632738 to A.S.), and the National Institutes of Health (grant no. R01 DA038615 to B.Y.H., grant nos. R01 DA029330 and R01 MH108629 to D.L., and grant no. R01 DA047870 to A.S.). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.
Author information
Affiliations
Contributions
A.S. conceived the project. C.H.D., B.Y.H. and D.L. designed the experiments in monkeys. S.F. and A.S. designed the human experiments. S.F. and A.S. performed model simulations and analysed the data. C.H.D. and S.F. conducted the experiments. C.H.D., S.F., D.L., B.Y.H. and A.S. analysed and interpreted the experimental data. D.L., B.Y.H. and A.S wrote the manuscript and all authors contributed to revising the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Peer review information: Primary Handling Editor: Marike Schiffer.
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Supplementary Information
Supplementary Notes 1 and 2 and Figs. 1–7.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Farashahi, S., Donahue, C.H., Hayden, B.Y. et al. Flexible combination of reward information across primates. Nat Hum Behav 3, 1215–1224 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0714-3
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
Further reading
-
Cognitiva Speciebus: Towards a Linnaean Approach to Cognition
Trends in Cognitive Sciences (2021)
-
The description–experience gap: a challenge for the neuroeconomics of decision-making under uncertainty
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences (2021)
-
Non-human primates use combined rules when deciding under ambiguity
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences (2021)
-
Inducing Affective Learning Biases with Cognitive Training and Prefrontal tDCS: A Proof-of-Concept Study
Cognitive Therapy and Research (2020)
-
Activity in orbitofrontal neuronal ensembles reflects inhibitory control
European Journal of Neuroscience (2020)