Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Homophily and minority-group size explain perception biases in social networks

Abstract

People’s perceptions about the size of minority groups in social networks can be biased, often showing systematic over- or underestimation. These social perception biases are often attributed to biased cognitive or motivational processes. Here we show that both over- and underestimation of the size of a minority group can emerge solely from structural properties of social networks. Using a generative network model, we show that these biases depend on the level of homophily, its asymmetric nature and on the size of the minority group. Our model predictions correspond well with empirical data from a cross-cultural survey and with numerical calculations from six real-world networks. We also identify circumstances under which individuals can reduce their biases by relying on perceptions of their neighbours. This work advances our understanding of the impact of network structure on social perception biases and offers a quantitative approach for addressing related issues in society.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Individual- and group-level social perception bias.
Fig. 2: Survey results: bias in perception of minority-group size for participants whose personal networks exhibit different levels of homophily (h) and for attributes held by a small, medium or large minority group in a given country.
Fig. 3: Network model results: bias in perception of minority-group size for the minority and majority groups, as a function of homophily (h) and the minority fraction (fm) in the overall network.
Fig. 4: Numerical simulations: group-level social perception biases that could occur in six empirical social networks.
Fig. 5: Social perception biases for individual nodes and for the weighted average of perceptions of individual nodes and their neighbours.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The three empirical data (DBLP, GitHub, APS) can be found online at https://github.com/frbkrm/NtwPerceptionBias. The network data for Brazil can be found in the data description of the study51 published in PLoS Computational Biology in 2011. POK can be found from the corresponding authors of the study52 published in Social Networks in 2004, and USF51 can be found from the corresponding author of the study54 in Physica A, 2011. The survey data can be obtained from the authors upon request.

Code availability

The Python scripts used for the generative model and empirical network analyses are available online at https://github.com/frbkrm/NtwPerceptionBias. Additional information about codes is available from the corresponding authors upon request.

References

  1. Cialdini, R. B. & Trost, M. R. Social influence: social norms, conformity and compliance. in The Handbook of Social Psychology (eds. Gilbert, D. T., Fiske, S. T., & Lindzey, G.) 151–192 (McGraw-Hill, 1998).

  2. Bond, R. M. et al. A 61-million-person experiment in social influence and political mobilization. Nature 489, 295–298 (2012).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Allcott, H. Social norms and energy conservation. J. Public Econ. 95, 1082–1095 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Centola, D. The spread of behavior in an online social network experiment. Science 329, 1194–1197 (2010).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Borsari, B. & Carey, K. B. Descriptive and injunctive norms in college drinking: a meta-analytic integration. J. Stud. Alcohol 64, 331–341 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Botvin, G. J., Botvin, E. M., Baker, E., Dusenbury, L. & Goldberg, C. J. The false consensus effect: predicting adolescents’ tobacco use from normative expectations. Psychol. Rep. 70, 171–178 (1992).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Thompson, A. Journalists and Trump voters live in separate online bubbles. VICE News https://news.vice.com/en_us/article/d3xamx/journalists-and-trump-voters-live-in-separate-online-bubbles-mit-analysis-shows (8 December 2017).

  8. Fields, J. M. & Schuman, H. Public beliefs about the beliefs of the public. Public Opin. Q. 40, 427–448 (1976).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Ross, L., Greene, D. & House, P. The ‘false consensus effect’: an egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 13, 279–301 (1977).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Mullen, B. et al. The false consensus effect: a meta-analysis of 115 hypothesis tests. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 21, 262–283 (1985).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Krueger, J. & Clement, R. W. The truly false consensus effect: an ineradicable and egocentric bias in social perception. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 596–610 (1994).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Krueger, J. From social projection to social behaviour. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 18, 1–35 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Mullen, B., Dovidio, J. F., Johnson, C. & Copper, C. In-group-out-group differences in social projection. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 28, 422–440 (1992).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Suls, J. & Wan, C. K. In search of the false-uniqueness phenomenon: fear and estimates of social consensus. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52, 211–217 (1987).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Miller, D. T. & McFarland, C. Pluralistic ignorance: when similarity is interpreted as dissimilarity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 53, 298–305 (1987).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Prentice, D. & Miller, D. T. Pluralistic ignorance and alcohol use on campus: some consequences of misperceiving the social norm. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 64, 243–256 (1993).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Lerman, K., Yan, X. & Wu, X.-Z. The ‘majority illusion’ in social networks. PLoS One 11, e0147617 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Krueger, J. & Clement, R. W. Estimates of social consensus by majorities and minorities: the case for social projection. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 1, 299–313 (1997).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Sherman, S. J., Presson, C. C., Chassin, L., Corty, E. & Olshavsky, R. The false consensus effect in estimates of smoking prevalence: underlying mechanisms. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 9, 197–207 (1983).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Galesic, M., Olsson, H. & Rieskamp, J. A sampling model of social judgment. Psychol. Rev. 125, 363 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Juslin, P., Winman, A. & Hansson, P. The naïve intuitive statistician: a naïve sampling model of intuitive confidence intervals. Psychol. Rev. 114, 678–703 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Pachur, T., Hertwig, R. & Rieskamp, J. Intuitive judgments of social statistics: how exhaustive does sampling need to be? J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 1059–1077 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L. & Cook, J. M. Birds of a feather: homophily in social networks. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 27, 415–444 (2001).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Jadidi, M., Karimi, F., Lietz, H. & Wagner, C. Gender disparities in science? Dropout, productivity, collaborations and success of male and female computer scientists. Adv. Complex Syst. 21, 1750011 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Miller, M. K., Wang, G., Kulkarni, S. R., Poor, H. V. & Osherson, D. N. Citizen forecasts of the 2008 U.S. presidential election. Polit. Policy 40, 1019–1052 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Dawes, R. M. Statistical criteria for establishing a truly false consensus effect. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 25, 1–17 (1989).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Marks, G. & Miller, N. Ten years of research on the false-consensus effect: an empirical and theoretical review. Psychol. Bull. 102, 72 (1987).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Suls, J., Wan, C. K. & Sanders, G. S. False consensus and false uniqueness in estimating the prevalence of health-protective behaviors. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 18, 66–79 (1988).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Bianconi, G. & Barabási, A.-L. Competition and multiscaling in evolving networks. Europhys. Lett. 54, 436 (2001).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Fiedler, K. & Krueger, J. I. More than an artifact: regression as a theoretical construct. in Social Judgment and Decision Making 171–189 (Psychology Press, 2012).

  31. Fiedler, K. & Unkelbach, C. Regressive judgment: implications of a universal property of the empirical world. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 23, 361–367 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Karimi, F., Génois, M., Wagner, C., Singer, P. & Strohmaier, M. Homophily influences ranking of minorities in social networks. Sci. Rep. 8, 11077 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Newman, M. E. Mixing patterns in networks. Phys. Rev. E 67, 026126 (2003).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Aral, S. & Walker, D. Identifying influential and susceptible members of social networks. Science 337, 337–41 (2012).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Golub, B. & Jackson, M. O. Naive learning in social networks and the wisdom of crowds. Am. Econ. J. Microecon. 2, 112–149 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Becker, J., Brackbill, D. & Centola, D. Network dynamics of social influence in the wisdom of crowds. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, E5070–E5076 (2017).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. DeGroot, M. H. Reaching a consensus. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 69, 118–121 (1974).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Fiedler, K. Beware of samples! A cognitive-ecological sampling approach to judgment biases. Psychol. Rev. 107, 659 (2000).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Gigerenzer, G., Fiedler, K. & Olsson, H. Rethinking cognitive biases as environmental consequences. in Ecological Rationality: Intelligence in the World 80–110 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2012).

  40. Le Mens, G. & Denrell, J. Rational learning and information sampling: on the ‘naivety’ assumption in sampling explanations of judgment biases. Psychol. Rev. 118, 379–392 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Denrell, J. & Le Mens, G. Information sampling, belief synchronization, and collective illusions. Manag. Sci. 63, 528–547 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Krueger, J. On the perception of social consensus. in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 163–240 (Academic Press, 1998).

  43. Centola, D. An experimental study of homophily in the adoption of health behavior. Science 334, 1269–1272 (2011).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Mollica, K. A., Gray, B. & Treviño, L. K. Racial homophily and its persistence in newcomers’ social networks. Organ. Sci. 14, 123–136 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Mehra, A., Kilduff, M. & Brass, D. J. At the margins: a distinctiveness approach to the social identity and social networks of underrepresented groups. Acad. Manag. J. 41, 441–452 (1998).

    Google Scholar 

  46. Festinger, L. A theory of social comparison processes. Hum. Relat. 7, 117–140 (1954).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Suls, J., Martin, R. & Wheeler, L. Social comparison: why, with whom, and with what effect? Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 11, 159–163 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Mobilia, M. Does a single zealot affect an infinite group of voters? Phys. Rev. Lett. 91, 028701 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Mobilia, M., Petersen, A. & Redner, S. On the role of zealotry in the voter model. J. Stat. Mech. Theory Exp. 2007, P08029 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Centola, D., Becker, J., Brackbill, D. & Baronchelli, A. Experimental evidence for tipping points in social convention. Science 360, 1116–1119 (2018).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Rocha, L. E., Liljeros, F. & Holme, P. Simulated epidemics in an empirical spatiotemporal network of 50,185 sexual contacts. PLoS Comput. Biol. 7, e1001109 (2011).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  52. Holme, P., Edling, C. R. & Liljeros, F. Structure and time evolution of an internet dating community. Soc. Netw. 26, 155–174 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Holme, P., Liljeros, F., Edling, C. R. & Kim, B. J. Network bipartivity. Phys. Rev. E 68, 056107 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Traud, A. L., Mucha, P. J. & Porter, M. A. Social structure of facebook networks. Physica A 391, 4165–4180 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Karimi, F., Wagner, C., Lemmerich, F., Jadidi, M. & Strohmaier, M. Interring gender from names on the web: a comparative evaluation of gender detection. in Proceedings of WWW ’16 Companion 53–54 (International World Wide Web Conferences Steering Committee, 2016).

  56. Collaboration networks from DataBase systems and Logic Programming (DBLP). http://dblp.uni-trier.de/ (accessed 30 September 2016).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank K. Winters, J. Kohne, P. Holme and H. Olsson for insightful conversations. We thank the Complex Systems Society’s Bridge Fund and GESIS for funding E.L.’s research visit. E.L. was financially supported by a MURI grant to E. B. Falk from the Army Research Office (No. W911NF-18-1-0244), with additional financial support from grants to P. J. Mucha from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development of the National Institutes of Health (No. R01HD075712) and the James S. McDonnell Foundation (grant No. 220020315). H.-H.J. acknowledges financial support from the Basic Science Research Program through an NRF grant funded by the Ministry of Education (No. NRF-2018R1D1A1A09081919). M.G. acknowledges financial support from National Science Foundation grants Nos. 1745154 and 1757211, and United States Department of Agriculture, National Institute of Food and Agriculture grant No. 2018-67023-27677. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

E.L. and F.K. conducted the analyses of synthetic and empirical networks and wrote the code. E.L. and M.G. conducted and analysed the surveys. E.L., F.K., C.W., H.-H.J., M.S. and M.G. conceived the project, developed the argument and wrote the paper.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Eun Lee or Fariba Karimi.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Peer review information: Primary Handling Editor: Mary Elizabeth Sutherland.

Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Tables 1–3, Supplementary Figs. 1–6, Supplementary Result 1, Supplementary Method 1 and Supplementary References.

Reporting Summary

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lee, E., Karimi, F., Wagner, C. et al. Homophily and minority-group size explain perception biases in social networks. Nat Hum Behav 3, 1078–1087 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0677-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0677-4

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing