Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Letter
  • Published:

Parental presence switches avoidance to attraction learning in children

Abstract

Attachment-related learning (that is, forming preferences for cues associated with the parent) defies the traditional rules of learning in that it seems to occur independently of apparent reinforcement1—young children prefer cues associated with their parent, regardless of valence (rewarding or aversive), despite the diversity of parenting styles2. This obligatory attraction for parental cues keeps the child nearby and safe to explore the environment; thus, it is critical for survival and sets the foundation for normal human cognitive–emotional behaviour. Here we examined the learning underlying this attraction in preschool-age children. Young children underwent an aversive conditioning procedure either in the parent’s presence or alone. We showed that despite disliking the aversive unconditioned stimulus, children exhibited a behavioural approach for conditioned stimuli that were acquired in the parent’s presence and an avoidance for stimuli acquired in the parent’s absence, an effect that was strongest among those with the lowest cortisol levels. The results suggest that learning systems during early childhood are constructed to permit modification by parental presence.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Schematic of study design.
Fig. 2: Behavioural and physiological evidence of CS+/CS− learning.
Fig. 3: Effect of parental presence on behavioural preference.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Code availability

The code that supports the findings of this study is available from the author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Bowlby, J. Attachment and Loss (Basic Books, 1969).

  2. Rajecki, D. W., Lamb, M. E. & Obmascher, P. Towards a general theory of infantile attachment: a comparative review of aspects of the social bond. Behav. Brain Sci. 3, 417–464 (1978).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Stafleu, A., Van Staveren, W. A., De Graaf, C., Burema, J. & Hautvast, J. G. Family resemblance in beliefs, attitudes and intentions towards consumption of 20 foods; a study among three generations of women. Appetite 25, 201–206 (1995).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Krumhansl, C. L. & Zupnick, J. A. Cascading reminiscence bumps in popular music. Psychol. Sci. 24, 2057–2068 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Ainsworth, M. D. Object relations, dependency, and attachment: a theoretical review of the infant–mother relationship. Child Dev. 40, 969–1025 (1969).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Moriceau, S. & Sullivan, R. M. Maternal presence serves as a switch between learning fear and attraction in infancy. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1004–1006 (2006).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Stronach, E. P. et al. Child maltreatment, attachment security, and internal representations of mother and mother–child relationships. Child Maltreat. 16, 137–145 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Ainsworth, M. D. & Bell, S. M. Attachment, exploration, and separation: illustrated by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. Child Dev. 41, 49–67 (1970).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Clancy, B., Finlay, B. L., Darlington, R. B. & Anand, K. J. Extrapolating brain development from experimental species to humans. Neurotoxicology 28, 931–937 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Thompson, J. L. & Nelson, A. J. Middle childhood and modern human origins. Hum. Nat. 22, 249–280 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Hoffman, K. T., Marvin, R. S., Cooper, G. & Powell, B. Changing toddlers’ and preschoolers’ attachment classifications: the circle of security intervention. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 74, 1017–1026 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Fisher, P. A. & Kim, H. K. Intervention effects on foster preschoolers’ attachment-related behaviors from a randomized trial. Prev. Sci. 8, 161–170 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Callaghan, B. L. et al. The developmental ecology of fear neurobiology across development. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 15, 345–369 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Bailey, D. et al. Marked biological variance in endocrine and biochemical markers in childhood: establishment of pediatric reference intervals using healthy community children from the CALIPER cohort. Clin. Chem. 59, 1393–1405 (2013).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Gee, D. G. et al. A developmental shift from positive to negative connectivity in human amygdala–prefrontal circuitry. J. Neurosci. 33, 4584–4593 (2013).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Silvers, J. A. et al. The transition from childhood to adolescence is marked by a general decrease in amygdala reactivity and an affect-specific ventral-to-dorsal shift in medial prefrontal recruitment. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 128–137 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Hostinar, C. E., Johnson, A. E. & Gunnar, M. R. Parent support is less effective in buffering cortisol stress reactivity for adolescents compared to children. Dev. Sci. 18, 281–297 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Gee, D. G. et al. Maternal buffering of human amygdala–prefrontal circuitry during childhood but not during adolescence. Psychol. Sci. 25, 2067–2078 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Callaghan, B. L. et al. Decreased amygdala reactivity to parent cues protects against anxiety following early adversity: an examination across 3 years. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.02.001 (in the press).

  20. Casey, B. J. & Richards, J. E. Sustained visual attention in young infants measured with an adapted version of the visual preference paradigm. Child Dev. 59, 1514–1521 (1988).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Bach, D. R., Flandin, G., Friston, K. J. & Dolan, R. J. Modelling event-related skin conductance responses. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 75, 349–356 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Flannery, J. E. et al. Diurnal cortisol after early institutional care-age matters. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 160–166 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Vasey, M. W., el-Hag, N. & Daleiden, E. L. Anxiety and the processing of emotionally threatening stimuli: distinctive patterns of selective attention among high- and low-test-anxious children. Child Dev. 67, 1173–1185 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Raineki, C. et al. Paradoxical neurobehavioral rescue by memories of early-life abuse: the safety signal value of odors learned during abusive attachment. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 906–914 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A. & Spinrad, T. L. Parental socialization of emotion. Psychol. Inq. 9, 241–273 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Sroufe, L. A., Coffino, B. & Carlson, E. A. Conceptualizing the role of early experience: lessons from the minnesota longitudinal study. Dev. Rev. 30, 36–51 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Campos, J. J. Human Emotions: Their New Importance and Their Role in Social Referencing. Annual Report 1–7 (Research & Clinical Center for Child Development, 1981).

  28. Kernberg, O. F. Early ego integration and object relations. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 193, 233–247 (1972).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Winnicott, D. W. The theory of the parent-infant relationship. Int. J. Psychoanal. 41, 585–595 (1960).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Baumrind, D. Effects of authoritative parental control on child behavior. Child Dev. 37, 887–907 (1966).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Roth, T. L. & Sullivan, R. M. Consolidation and expression of a shock-induced odor preference in rat pups is facilitated by opioids. Physiol. Behav. 78, 135–142 (2003).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Kertes, D. A. et al. Inhibited temperament and parent emotional availability differentially predict young children’s cortisol responses to novel social and nonsocial events. Dev. Psychobiol. 51, 521–532 (2009).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Schore, A. N. All our sons: the developmental neurobiology and neuroendocrinology of boys at risk. Infant Ment. Health J. 38, 15–52 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Caldwell, B. M., . & Bradley, R. H. Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment. (University of Arkansas at Little Rock:, 1984).

  35. Baharudin, R. & Luster, T. Factors related to the quality of the home environment and children’s achievement. J. Fam. Issues 19, 375–403 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., Ball, R. & Ranieri, W. Comparison of Beck depression inventories YIA and YII in psychiatric outpatients. J. Pers. Assess. 67, 588–597 (1996).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Spielberger, C. D. Manual for the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory. Psychological Assessment Resources (1988).

  38. Coddington, R. D. The significance of life events as etiologic factors in the diseases of children. II. A study of a normal population. J. Psychosom. Res. 16, 205–213 (1972).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Achenbach, T. M. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile. Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont (1991).

  40. Rickel, A. U. & Biasatti, L. L. Modification of the block child rearing practices report. J. Clin. Psychol. 38, 129–134 (1982).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Neumann, D. L. & Waters, A. M. The use of an unpleasant sound as an unconditional stimulus in a human aversive Pavlovian conditioning procedure. Biol. Psychol. 73, 175–185 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Silvers, J. A. et al. Previous Institutionalization is followed by broader amygdala–hippocampal–PFC network connectivity during aversive learning in human development. J. Neurosci. 36, 6420–6430 (2016).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Ham, J. & Tronick, E. A procedure for the measurement of infant skin conductance and its initial validation using clap induced startle. Dev. Psychobiol. 50, 626–631 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Kirschbaum, C. & Hellhammer, D. H. Salivary cortisol. Encycl. Stress 3, 379–383 (2000).

    Google Scholar 

  45. Watamura, S. E., Donzella, B., Kertes, D. A. & Gunnar, M. R. Developmental changes in baseline cortisol activity in early childhood: relations with napping and effortful control. Dev. Psychobiol. 45, 125–133 (2004).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Sumner, M. M., Bernard, K. & Dozier, M. Young children’s full-day patterns of cortisol production on child care days. Arch. Pedia. Adolesc. Med. 164, 567–571 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported in part by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Grant R37HD083217 (to R.M.S.) and the National Institute of Mental Health Grant R01MH091864 (to N.T.) The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript. The authors thank A. Galván for her helpful feedback on previous drafts.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

N.T., M.S. and R.M.S. designed the study. N.T., M.S., J.F. and C.C. performed the experiments, data processing and analysis. N.T. and R.M.S. wrote the manuscript and provided overall direction.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nim Tottenham.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Peer review information: Primary Handling Editor: Aisha Bradshaw

Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Methods, Supplementary Results, Supplementary References

Reporting Summary

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tottenham, N., Shapiro, M., Flannery, J. et al. Parental presence switches avoidance to attraction learning in children. Nat Hum Behav 3, 1070–1077 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0656-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0656-9

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing