Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Letter
  • Published:

The evolution of language families is shaped by the environment beyond neutral drift

Abstract

There are more than 7,000 languages spoken in the world today1. It has been argued that the natural and social environment of languages drives this diversity2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13. However, a fundamental question is how strong are environmental pressures, and does neutral drift suffice as a mechanism to explain diversification? We estimate the phylogenetic signals of geographic dimensions, distance to water, climate and population size on more than 6,000 phylogenetic trees of 46 language families. Phylogenetic signals of environmental factors are generally stronger than expected under the null hypothesis of no relationship with the shape of family trees. Importantly, they are also—in most cases—not compatible with neutral drift models of constant-rate change across the family tree branches. Our results suggest that language diversification is driven by further adaptive and non-adaptive pressures. Language diversity cannot be understood without modelling the pressures that physical, ecological and social factors exert on language users in different environments across the globe.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Density distributions of phylogenetic signals for K and λ, and violin plots with distributions per environmental factor.
Fig. 2: Environmental factors reflected on family trees.
Fig. 3: Percentages of subsets in line with evolutionary hypotheses.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

Data availability is detailed in Supplementary Methods 1. Individual data files are described in Supplementary Data 17 in the Guide to the Supplementary Information.

References

  1. Hammarström, H., Forkel, R., Haspelmath, M. & Bank, S. Glottolog 3.2 (Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, 2018); http://glottolog.org

  2. Nichols, J. Linguistic diversity and the first settlement of the New World. Language 66, 475–521 (1990).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Nettle, D. Using social impact theory to simulate language change. Lingua 108, 95–117 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Coupé, C., Hombert, J.-M., Marsico, E. & Pellegrino, F. in East Flows the Great River: Festschrift in Honor of Prof. William S-Y. WANG on his 80th Birthday (eds Peng, G. & Shi, F.) 76–103 (City Univ. Hong Kong Press, Hong Kong, 2013).

  5. Gavin, M. C. et al. Toward a mechanistic understanding of linguistic diversity. Bioscience 63, 524–535 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Mace, R. & Pagel, M. A latitudinal gradient in the density of human languages in North America. Proc. Biol. Sci. 261, 117–121 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Collard, I. F. & Foley, R. A. Latitudinal patterns and environmental determinants of recent human cultural diversity: do humans follow biogeographical rules? Evol. Ecol. Res. 4, 371–383 (2002).

    Google Scholar 

  8. Moore, J. L. et al. The distribution of cultural and biological diversity in Africa. Proc. Biol. Sci. 269, 1645–1653 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Dimmendaal, G. J. Language ecology and linguistic diversity on the African continent. Lang. Linguist. Compass 2, 840–858 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Axelsen, J. B. & Manrubia, S. River density and landscape roughness are universal determinants of linguistic diversity. Proc. Biol. Sci. 281, 20133029 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Gavin, M. C. & Sibanda, N. The island biogeography of languages. Glob. Ecol. Biogeogr. 21, 958–967 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Gavin, M. C. et al. Process-based modelling shows how climate and demography shape language diversity. Glob. Ecol. Biogeogr. 26, 584–591 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Currie, T. E. & Mace, R. Political complexity predicts the spread of ethnolinguistic groups. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 7339–7344 (2009).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., Menozzi, P. & Piazza, A. The History and Geography of Human Genes (Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, 1994).

  15. Everett, C., Blasi, D. E. & Roberts, S. G. Climate, vocal folds, and tonal languages: connecting the physiological and geographic dots. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 1322–1327 (2015).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Lupyan, G. & Dale, R. Why are there different languages? The role of adaptation in linguistic diversity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 649–660 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Dediu, D., Janssen, R. & Moisik, S. R. Language is not isolated from its wider environment: vocal tract influences on the evolution of speech and language. Lang. Commun. 54, 9–20 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Welmers, W. E. African Language Structures (University of California Press, Berkeley and Los Angeles, 1973).

  19. McMahon, A. M. Understanding Language Change (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1994).

  20. Sapir, E. Language: An Introduction to the Study of Speech (Harcourt, Brace & World, New York, 1921).

  21. Jones, M. C. & Singh, I. Exploring Language Change (Routledge, New York, 2005).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  22. Blomberg, S. P., Garland, T. Jr., Ives, A. R. & Crespi, B. Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data: behavioral traits are more labile. Evolution 57, 717–745 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Symonds, M. R. & Blomberg, S. P. in Modern Phylogenetic Comparative Methods and Their Application in Evolutionary Biology (ed. Garamszegi, L. Z.) 105–130 (Springer, Heidelberg, 2014).

  24. Verkerk, A. Diachronic change in Indo-European motion event encoding. J. Hist. Linguist. 4, 40–83 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Verkerk, A. The correlation between motion event encoding and path verb lexicon size in the Indo-European language family. Folia Linguist. Hist. 35, 307–358 (2014).

    Google Scholar 

  26. Bentz, C., Verkerk, A., Kiela, D., Hill, F. & Buttery, P. Adaptive communication: languages with more non-native speakers tend to have fewer word forms. PLoS ONE 10, e0128254 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Everett, C. Evidence for direct geographic influences on linguistic sounds: the case of ejectives. PLoS ONE 8, e65275 (2013).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Lupyan, G. & Dale, R. Language structure is partly determined by social structure. PLoS ONE 5, e8559 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Dale, R. & Lupyan, G. Understanding the origins of morphological diversity: the linguistic niche hypothesis. Adv. Complex Syst. 15, 1150017 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Bentz, C. & Winter, B. Languages with more second language speakers tend to lose nominal case. Lang. Dynam. Change 3, 1–27 (2013).

    Google Scholar 

  31. Revell, L. J., Harmon, L. J. & Collar, D. C. Phylogenetic signal, evolutionary process, and rate. Syst. Biol. 57, 591–601 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Thomason, S. G. & Kaufman, T. Language Contact, Creolization, and Genetic Linguistics (Univ. California Press, Berkeley & Oxford, 1988).

  33. Diamond, J. M. Guns, Germs and Steel: The Fates of Human Societies (W. W. Norton, New York & London, 1999).

  34. Güldemann, T. & Hammarström, H. in Language Dispersal, Diversification and Contact (eds Crevels, M. & Muysken, P.) (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 2017).

  35. Lupyan, G. & Dale, R. in Language Structure and Environment (eds De Busser, R. & LaPolla, R. J.) 289–316 (John Benjamins Publishing Company, Amsterdam, 2015).

  36. Dediu, D. Making genealogical language classifications available for phylogenetic analysis: Newick trees, unified identifiers, and branch length. Lang. Dynam. Change 8, 1–21 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Lewis, M. P., Simons, G. F. & Fenning, C. D. Ethnologue: Languages of the World 17th edn (SIL International, Dallas, 2013); http://www.ethnologue.com

  38. Dryer, M. S. & Haspelmath, M. The World Atlas of Language Structures Online (Max Planck Digital Library, 2013); http://wals.info/

  39. Nichols, J., Witzlack-Makarevich, A. & Bickel, B. The AUTOTYP Genealogy and Geography Database 2013 Release (2013); https://www.spw.uzh.ch/autotyp/

  40. Jäger, G. Global-scale phylogenetic linguistic inference from lexical resources. Preprint at http://arxiv.org/abs/1802.06079 (2018).

  41. Münkemüller, T. et al. How to measure and test phylogenetic signal. Methods Ecol. Evol. 3, 743–756 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. R Development Core Team R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2017).

  43. Chamberlain, S. rgbif: Interface to the Global ‘Biodiversity’ Information Facility ‘API’, R package version 0.9.5 (2016); https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=rgbif

  44. Pagel, M. Inferring evolutionary processes from phylogenies. Zool. Scr. 26, 331–348 (1997).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Pagel, M. Inferring the historical patterns of biological evolution. Nature 401, 877–884 (1999).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Freckleton, R. P., Harvey, P. H. & Pagel, M. Phylogenetic analysis and comparative data: a test and review of evidence. Am. Nat. 160, 712–726 (2002).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Revell, L. J. phytools: an R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things). Methods Ecol. Evol. 3, 217–223 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Wickham, H. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis (Springer, New York, 2016).

  49. Yu, G., Smith, D. K., Zhu, H., Guan, Y. & Lam, T. T-Y. ggtree: an R package for visualization and annotation of phylogenetic trees with their covariates and other associated data. Methods Ecol. Evol. 8, 28–36 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Kahle, D. & Wickham, H. ggmap: spatial visualization with ggplot2. R. J. 5, 144–161 (2013).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

C.B. and G.J. were funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG FOR 2237; project ‘Words, Bones, Genes, Tools: Tracking Linguistic, Cultural, and Biological Trajectories of the Human Past’) and the ERC Advanced Grant 324246 EVOLAEMP. D.D. was funded by The Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research VIDI grant 276-70-022 and the European Institutes for Advanced Study Fellowship Program. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

C.B. was responsible for project inception, statistical and phylogenetic analyses, and writing of the paper. D.D., A.V. and G.J. contributed data, phylogenetic analyses and writing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christian Bentz.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Results 1–4, Supplementary Methods 1–6, Supplementary Note 1

Reporting Summary

SI Guide

Supplementary Data set 1

Dediu’s forest data

Supplementary Data Set 2

Maximum likelihood trees data

Supplementary Data Set 3

Environmental variables data

Supplementary Data Set 4

All phylogenetic signals data

Supplementary Data Set 5

Phylogenetic signals for distances to lakes, rivers, and oceans data

Supplementary Data Set 6

Wilcoxon test results by tree set

Supplementary Data Set 7

Wilcoxon results by family

R code file

R analysis code files

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bentz, C., Dediu, D., Verkerk, A. et al. The evolution of language families is shaped by the environment beyond neutral drift. Nat Hum Behav 2, 816–821 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0457-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0457-6

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing