Abstract
Uncertainty about how our choices will affect others infuses social life. Past research suggests uncertainty has a negative effect on prosocial behaviour1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12 by enabling people to adopt self-serving narratives about their actions1,13. We show that uncertainty does not always promote selfishness. We introduce a distinction between two types of uncertainty that have opposite effects on prosocial behaviour. Previous work focused on outcome uncertainty (uncertainty about whether or not a decision will lead to a particular outcome). However, as soon as people’s decisions might have negative consequences for others, there is also impact uncertainty (uncertainty about how others’ well-being will be impacted by the negative outcome). Consistent with past research1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12, we found decreased prosocial behaviour under outcome uncertainty. In contrast, prosocial behaviour was increased under impact uncertainty in incentivized economic decisions and hypothetical decisions about infectious disease threats. Perceptions of social norms paralleled the behavioural effects. The effect of impact uncertainty on prosocial behaviour did not depend on the individuation of others or the mere mention of harm, and was stronger when impact uncertainty was made more salient. Our findings offer insights into communicating uncertainty, especially in contexts where prosocial behaviour is paramount, such as responding to infectious disease threats.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution
Relevant articles
Open Access articles citing this article.
-
Explaining interpersonal differences in COVID-19 disease prevention behavior based on the health belief model and collective resilience theory: a cross-sectional study from Bolivia
BMC Public Health Open Access 31 May 2022
-
The social cost of gathering information for trust decisions
Scientific Reports Open Access 21 August 2020
Access options
Access Nature and 54 other Nature Portfolio journals
Get Nature+, our best-value online-access subscription
$29.99 per month
cancel any time
Subscribe to this journal
Receive 12 digital issues and online access to articles
$119.00 per year
only $9.92 per issue
Rent or buy this article
Get just this article for as long as you need it
$39.95
Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout



References
Dana, J., Weber, R. A. & Kuang, J. X. Exploiting moral wiggle room: experiments demonstrating an illusory preference for fairness. Econ. Theory 33, 67–80 (2007).
Exley, C. L. Excusing selfishness in charitable giving: the role of risk. Rev. Econ. Stud. 83, 587–628 (2015).
Larson, T. & Capra, C. M. Exploiting moral wiggle room: illusory preference for fairness? A comment. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 4, 467–474 (2009).
Hamman, J. R., Loewenstein, G. & Weber, R. A. Self-interest through delegation: an additional rationale for the principal-agent relationship. Am. Econ. Rev. 100, 1826–1846 (2010).
Brock, J. M., Lange, A. & Ozbay, E. Y. Dictating the risk: experimental evidence on giving in risky environments. Am. Econ. Rev. 103, 415–437 (2013).
Feiler, L. Testing models of information avoidance with binary choice dictator games. J. Econ. Psychol. 45, 253–267 (2014).
Güth, W., Levati, M. V. & Ploner, M. On the social dimension of time and risk preferences: an experimental study. Econ. Inq. 46, 261–272 (2008).
Brennan, G., González, L. G., Güth, W. & Levati, M. V. Attitudes toward private and collective risk in individual and strategic choice situations. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 67, 253–262 (2008).
Andreoni, J. & Bernheim, B. D. Social image and the 50–50 norm: a theoretical and experimental analysis of audience effects. Econometrica 77, 1607–1636 (2009).
Budescu, D. V., Rapoport, A. & Suleiman, R. Resource dilemmas with environmental uncertainty and asymmetric players. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 20, 475–487 (1990).
Gustafsson, M., Biel, A. & Gärling, T. Overharvesting of resources of unknown size. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 103, 47–64 (1999).
De Kwaadsteniet, E. W., van Dijk, E., Wit, A., De Cremer, D. & de Rooij, M. Justifying decisions in social dilemmas: justification pressures and tacit coordination under environmental uncertainty. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 33, 1648–1660 (2007).
Shalvi, S., Gino, F., Barkan, R. & Ayal, S. Self-serving justifications: doing wrong and feeling moral. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 24, 125–130 (2015).
Nagel, T. What is it like to be a bat? Phil. Rev. 83, 435–450 (1974).
Harsanyi, J. C. Cardinal welfare, individualistic ethics, and interpersonal comparisons of utility. J. Polit. Econ. 63, 309–321 (1955).
Hammond, P. J. in Interpersonal Comparisons of Well-Being (eds Elster, J. & Roemer, J. E.) 200–254 (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1991).
Falk, A. & Tirole, J. Narratives, Imperatives and Moral Reasoning Working Paper (mimeo Toulouse School of Economics, 2016); https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/8a0f/87ad25a8a51c7637b32b4e2fc5080f3963c4.pdf
Mazar, N., Amir, O. & Ariely, D. The dishonesty of honest people: a theory of self-concept maintenance. J. Mark. Res. 45, 633–644 (2008).
Haisley, E. C. & Weber, R. A. Self-serving interpretations of ambiguity in other-regarding behavior. Games Econ. Behav. 68, 614–625 (2010).
Van Dijk, E., Wit, A., Wilke, H. & Budescu, D. V. in Contemporary Psychological Research on Social Dilemmas (eds Suleiman, R., Budescu, D. V., Fischer, I. & Messick, D. M.) 315–331 (Cambridge Univ. Press, New York, NY, 2004).
Krupka, E. L. & Weber, R. A. Identifying social norms using coordination games: why does dictator game sharing vary? J. Eur. Econ. Assoc. 11, 495–524 (2013).
Platt, M. L. & Huettel, S. A. Risky business: the neuroeconomics of decision making under uncertainty. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 398–403 (2008).
Buckholtz, J. W. et al. From blame to punishment: disrupting prefrontal cortex activity reveals norm enforcement mechanisms. Neuron 87, 1369–1380 (2015).
Savage, L. J. The Foundations of Statistics (Dover Publications, New York, NY, 1972).
Davis, H. L., Hoch, S. J. & Ragsdale, E. K. E. An anchoring and adjustment model of spousal predictions. J. Consum. Res. 13, 25–37 (1986).
Greenstein, M. J. & Xu, X. My mug is valuable, but my partner’s is even more so: economic decisions for close others. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 37, 174–187 (2015).
Hsee, C. K. & Weber, E. U. A fundamental prediction error: self–others discrepancies in risk preference. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 126, 45–53 (1997).
Ziegler, F. V. & Tunney, R. J. Who’s been framed? Framing effects are reduced in financial gambles made for others. BMC Psychol. 3, 9 (2015).
Andersson, O., Holm, H. J., Tyran, J.-R. & Wengström, E. Risking Other People’s Money: Experimental Evidence on Bonus Schemes, Competition, and Altruism IFN Working Paper 989 (IFN, 2013).
Beisswanger, A. H., Stone, E. R., Hupp, J. M. & Allgaier, L. Risk taking in relationships: differences in deciding for oneself versus for a friend. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 25, 121–135 (2003).
Zikmund-Fisher, B. J., Sarr, B., Fagerlin, A. & Ubel, P. A. A matter of perspective: choosing for others differs from choosing for yourself in making treatment decisions. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 21, 618–622 (2006).
Green, J. D., Burnette, J. L. & Davis, J. L. Third-party forgiveness: (not) forgiving your close other’s betrayer. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 407–418 (2008).
Shalowitz, D. I., Garrett-Mayer, E. & Wendler, D. The accuracy of surrogate decision makers: a systematic review. Arch. Intern. Med. 166, 493–497 (2006).
Kelly, B., Rid, A. & Wendler, D. Systematic review: individuals’ goals for surrogate decision-making. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 60, 884–895 (2012).
Majesko, A., Hong, S. Y., Weissfeld, L. & White, D. B. Identifying family members who may struggle in the role of surrogate decision maker. Crit. Care Med. 40, 2281–2286 (2012).
Crockett, M. J., Kurth-Nelson, Z., Siegel, J. Z., Dayan, P. & Dolan, R. J. Harm to others outweighs harm to self in moral decision making. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, 17320–17325 (2014).
Engel, C. Dictator games: a meta study. Exp. Econ. 14, 583–610 (2011).
Bartling, B. & Fischbacher, U. Shifting the blame: on delegation and responsibility. Rev. Econ. Stud. 79, 67–87 (2011).
Aguiar, F., Brañas-Garza, P. & Miller, L. M. Moral distance in dictator games. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 3, 344–354 (2008).
Brañas-Garza, P. Poverty in dictator games: awakening solidarity. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 60, 306–320 (2006).
Fong, C. & Oberholzer-Gee, F. Truth in giving: experimental evidence on the welfare effects of informed giving to the poor. J. Public Econ. 95, 436–444 (2011).
Reniers, R. L. E. P., Corcoran, R., Drake, R., Shryane, N. M. & Völlm, B. A. The QCAE: a Questionnaire of Cognitive and Affective Empathy. J. Pers. Assess. 93, 84–95 (2011).
Grossmann, I., Brienza, J. P. & Bobocel, D. R. Wise deliberation sustains cooperation. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 0061 (2017).
Kogut, T. & Ritov, I. The “identified victim” effect: an identified group, or just a single individual? J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 18, 157–167 (2005).
Small, D. A. & Loewenstein, G. Helping a victim or helping the victim: altruism and identifiability. J. Risk Uncertain. 26, 5–16 (2003).
Small, D. A., Loewenstein, G. & Slovic, P. Sympathy and callousness: the impact of deliberative thought on donations to identifiable and statistical victims. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 102, 143–153 (2007).
Ecker, U. K. H., Hogan, J. L. & Lewandowsky, S. Reminders and repetition of misinformation: helping or hindering its retraction? J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 6, 185–192 (2017).
Putnam, A. L., Wahlheim, C. N. & Jacoby, L. L. Memory for flip-flopping: detection and recollection of political contradictions. Mem. Cogn. 42, 1198–1210 (2014).
Stadtler, M., Scharrer, L., Brummernhenrich, B. & Bromme, R. Dealing with uncertainty: readers’ memory for and use of conflicting information from science texts as function of presentation format and source expertise. Cogn. Instr. 31, 130–150 (2013).
Carlyle, R. C. What are the chances of a devastating pandemic occurring in the next 50 years? Huffington Post https://www.huffingtonpost.com/quora/what-are-the-chances-of-a_b_3839785.html?guccounter=1 (2013).
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A. & Lang, A.-G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160 (2009).
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T. & Gosling, S. D. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: a new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6, 3–5 (2011).
Paolacci, G., Chandler, J. & Ipeirotis, P. G. Running experiments on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 5, 411–419 (2010).
Phillips, J. & Cushman, F. Morality constrains the default representation of what is possible. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 4649–4654 (2017).
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by a Wellcome Trust Institutional Strategic Support Fund grant (204826/Z/16/Z) awarded to J.S., M.J.C., N.S.F. and G.K., a John Fell Fund award to M.J.C., and by the Oxford Martin Programme for Collective Responsibility for Infectious Disease. The work of J.S. for this paper was funded by a Wellcome Trust grant (104848/Z/14/Z). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. The authors thank W. Sinnott-Armstrong, D. Batson, and members of the Crockett and MAD laboratories for helpful feedback.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
A.K. and M.J.C. developed the research concept. A.K., M.J.C. and A.-M.N. designed the studies. Testing and data collection were performed by A.K. and A.-M.N. A.K. performed the data analysis and interpretation in collaboration with A.-M.N. and M.J.C. A.K., A.-M.N. and M.J.C. drafted the manuscript and all other authors provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Supplementary information
Supplementary Methods, Supplementary Note, Supplementary Figure 1, Supplementary References 1−9
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Kappes, A., Nussberger, AM., Faber, N.S. et al. Uncertainty about the impact of social decisions increases prosocial behaviour. Nat Hum Behav 2, 573–580 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0372-x
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0372-x
This article is cited by
-
Explaining interpersonal differences in COVID-19 disease prevention behavior based on the health belief model and collective resilience theory: a cross-sectional study from Bolivia
BMC Public Health (2022)
-
The social cost of gathering information for trust decisions
Scientific Reports (2020)
-
Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response
Nature Human Behaviour (2020)
-
Social uncertainty is heterogeneous and sometimes valuable
Nature Human Behaviour (2019)