Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Female cuckoo calls misdirect host defences towards the wrong enemy

Abstract

Prey are sensitive to even subtle cues of predation risk, which provides the evolutionary potential for parasites to exploit host risk perception. Brood parasitic common cuckoos (Cuculus canorus) lay their eggs in the nests of host species and their secretive laying behaviour enables them to evade host defences. Therefore, it seems paradoxical that female cuckoos often give a conspicuous ‘chuckle’ call after parasitizing a host’s clutch. Here, we show that this hawk-like chuckle call increases the success of parasitism by diverting host parents’ attention away from the clutch and towards their own safety. In our field experiments, reed warbler (Acrocephalus scirpaceus) hosts paid no more attention to the ‘cuck-oo’ call of the male common cuckoo than the call of a harmless dove. However, the chuckle call of the female cuckoo had the same effect as the call of a predatory hawk in distracting the warblers’ attention and reducing rejection of a foreign egg. Our results show that the female cuckoo enhances her success by manipulating a fundamental trade-off in host defences between clutch and self-protection.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Rent or buy this article

Prices vary by article type

from$1.95

to$39.95

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Reed warblers and tits were more likely to become vigilant in response to female cuckoo and hawk calls than calls of a male cuckoo or dove.
Fig. 2: Reed warblers were more likely to accept a foreign egg after playback of female cuckoo or hawk calls than after the calls of a male cuckoo or dove.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Hughes, D. P., Brodeur, J. & Thomas, F. Host Manipulation by Parasites (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 2012).

  2. Schmid Hempel, P. & Schmid-Hempel, P. Evolutionary Parasitology: the Integrated Study of Infections, Immunology, Ecology, and Genetics (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 2011).

  3. Ghalambor, C. K. & Martin, T. E. Fecundity–survival trade-offs and parental risk-taking in birds. Science 292, 494–497 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Magrath, R. D., Haff, T. M., Horn, A. G. & Leonard, M. L. Calling in the face of danger: predation risk and acoustic communication by parent birds and their offspring. Adv. Stud. Behav. 41, 187–253 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Kilner, R. M. & Langmore, N. E. Cuckoos versus hosts in insects and birds: adaptations, counter-adaptations and outcomes. Biol. Rev. 86, 836–852 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Welbergen, J. A. & Davies, N. B. Strategic variation in mobbing as a front line of defense against brood parasitism. Curr. Biol. 19, 235–240 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Feeney, W. E., Welbergen, J. A. & Langmore, N. E. The frontline of avian brood parasite–host coevolution. Anim. Behav. 84, 3–12 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Davies, N. B. & Brooke, M. de L. Cuckoos versus reed warblers: adaptations and counteradaptations. Anim. Behav. 36, 262–284 (1988).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Davies, N. B. & Brooke, M. de L. An experimental study of co-evolution between the cuckoo Cuculus canorus and its hosts. 1. Host discrimination. J. Anim. Ecol. 58, 207–224 (1989).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Moksnes, A. et al. Behavioural responses of potential hosts towards artificial cuckoo eggs and dummies. Behaviour 116, 64–89 (1991).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Thorogood, R. & Davies, N. B. Reed warbler defenses track three decades of cuckoo decline. Evolution 67, 3545–3555 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Thorogood, R. & Davies, N. B. Combining personal with social information facilitates host defences and explains why cuckoos should be secretive. Sci. Rep. 6, 19872 (2016).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Bártol, I., Karcza, Z., Moskát, C., Røskaft, E. & Kisbenedek, T. Responses of great reed warblers Acrocephalus arundinaceus to experimental brood parasitism: the effects of a cuckoo Cuculus canorus dummy and egg mimicry. J. Avian Biol. 33, 420–425 (2002).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Stokke, B. G. et al. Predictors of resistance to brood parasitism within and among reed warbler populations. Behav. Ecol. 19, 612–620 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Stoddard, M. C. & Stevens, M. Avian vision and the evolution of egg color mimicry in the common cuckoo. Evolution 65, 2004–2013 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Chance, E. P. The Truth About the Cuckoo (Country Life, London, 1940).

    Google Scholar 

  17. Wallace, A. R. Darwinism: an Exposition of the Theory of Natural Selection With Some of its Applications (Macmillan, London, 1889).

    Google Scholar 

  18. Welbergen, J. A. & Davies, N. B. A parasite in wolf’s clothing: hawk mimicry reduces mobbing of cuckoos by hosts. Behav. Ecol. 22, 574–579 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Gentle, L. K. & Gosler, A. G. Fat reserves and perceived predation risk in the great tit, Parus major. Proc. R. Soc. B 268, 487–491 (2001).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Trnka, A. & Grim, T. Testing for correlations between behaviours in a cuckoo host: why do host defences not covary? Anim. Behav. 92, 185–193 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Moskát, C., Elek, Z., Bán, M., Geltsch, N. & Hauber, M. E. Can common cuckoos discriminate between neighbours and strangers by their calls? Anim. Behav. 126, 253–260 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Wyllie, I. The Cuckoo (Batsford, London, 1981).

  23. Požgayová, M., Procházka, P., Polačiková, L. & Honza, M. Closer clutch inspection—quicker egg ejection: timing of host responses toward parasitic eggs. Behav. Ecol. 22, 46–51 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Flower, T. P., Gribble, M. & Ridley, A. R. Deception by flexible alarm mimicry in an African bird. Science 344, 513–516 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Payne, R. B. The Cuckoos (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 2005).

  26. Odom, K. J., Hall, M. L., Riebel, K., Omland, K. E. & Langmore, N. E. Female song is widespread and ancestral in songbirds. Nat. Commun. 5, 3379 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Thorogood, R. & Davies, N. B. Cuckoos combat socially transmitted defenses of reed warbler hosts with a plumage polymorphism. Science 337, 578–580 (2012).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Thorogood, R. & Davies, N. B. Hawk mimicry and the evolution of polymorphic cuckoos. Chinese Birds 4, 39–50 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Sherry, D. F., Forbes, M. R., Khurgel, M. & Ivy, G. O. Females have a larger hippocampus than males in the brood-parasitic brown-headed cowbird. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 90, 7839–7843 (1993).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Cuthill, I. C. Evolution: the mystery of imperfect mimicry. Curr. Biol. 24, R364–R366 (2014).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Dalziell, A. H. & Welbergen, J. A. Mimicry for all modalities. Ecol. Lett. 19, 609–619 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Roche, D. P., McGhee, K. E. & Bell, A. M. Maternal predator-exposure has lifelong consequences for offspring learning in threespined sticklebacks. Biol. Lett. 8, 932–935 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Suraci, J. P., Clinchy, M., Dill, L. M., Roberts, D. & Zannette, L. Y. Fear of large carnivores causes a trophic cascade. Nat. Commun. 7, 10698 (2016).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Wiley, R. H. Noise Matters: the Evolution of Communication (Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, MA, 2015).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  35. Billings, A. C., Greene, E. & De La Lucia Jensen, S. M. Are chickadees good listeners? Antipredator responses to raptor vocalizations. Anim. Behav. 110, 1–8 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Raven Pro Interactive Sound Analysis Software v. 1.5 (Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, 2014).

    Google Scholar 

  37. Liang, W. et al. Geographic variation in egg ejection rate by great tits across 2 continents. Behav. Ecol. 27, 1405–1412 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Davies, N. B. & Welbergen, J. A. Social transmission of a host defense against cuckoo parasitism. Science 324, 1318–1320 (2009).

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Welbergen, J. A. & Davies, N. B. Reed warblers discriminate cuckoos from sparrowhawks with graded alarm signals that attract mates and neighbours. Anim. Behav. 76, 811–822 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria, 2017). https://www.R-project.org/.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Hartig, F. DHARMa: Residual Diagnostics for Hierarchical (Multi-Level/Mixed) Regression Models (2016); https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=DHARMa.

  42. Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Crawley, M. The R Book (John Wiley & Sons, Chichester, 2007).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the National Trust for permission to work on Wicken Fen, Natural England for the licenses, H. Rowland, J. Mackenzie and T. Dixit for field assistance, C. Spottiswoode and A. Jungwirth for comments, and especially D. Cram for comments and assistance throughout. This work was funded by Natural Environment Research Council grant NE/M00807X/1.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

J.E.Y. and N.B.D. contributed equally to the field experiments and writing of the manuscript. J.E.Y. analysed the data.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jenny E. York.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information, Supplementary Figures, and Supplementary References

Supplementary Video

Examples of reed warbler vigilance responses to cuckoo calls

Supplementary Data

Dataset supporting analyses in the main text, from each of the three experiments: Experiment 1 (tab 1), vigilance in reed warblers (cuckoo hosts); Experiment 2 (tab 2), vigilance in great tits and blue tits (not cuckoo hosts); Experiment 3 (tab 3), nest defences in reed warbler hosts

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

York, J.E., Davies, N.B. Female cuckoo calls misdirect host defences towards the wrong enemy. Nat Ecol Evol 1, 1520–1525 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41559-017-0279-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41559-017-0279-3

This article is cited by

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing