Abstract
This study investigated the factors affecting adolescents’ well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic from the perspectives of their participation in digital activities, emotional regulation, self-regulated learning, and parental involvement. Using self-reported data from 932 pairs of adolescents and their parents, we performed multiple-group structural equation modeling, which revealed that self-efficacy in online learning during school suspension was a key factor influencing adolescents’ perceived worries after schools resumed. During school suspension, boys’ cognitive-emotional regulation played a protective role in their well-being, helping them to avoid cyberbullying incidents, while girls’ participation in leisure-oriented digital activities compromised their self-efficacy in online learning and led to cyberbullying incidents. Furthermore, improvement in parent–child relationships during school suspension encouraged adolescents to use more positive emotional regulation strategies, enhanced their self-efficacy in online learning, and reduced their leisure-time digital activities. The findings indicate that the effective regulation of adolescents’ online behaviors, emotions, and self-efficacy, especially when combined with an emotionally secure family relationship, can ensure adolescents’ well-being.
Introduction
Because of COVID-19-related school suspensions, adolescents in Hong Kong lacked the normal face-to-face schooling and socialization for a prolonged period. Instead, the government policy of “suspending classes without suspending learning” meant that students engaged in more online learning and socialization1. The effect of such changes on adolescents’ studying and their daily life deserves our attention because their experiences (e.g., negative affection2) during school suspensions may influence their well-being after schools resumed3.
To cope with the challenges of online study or non-study activities, aspects of adolescents’ ability to self-regulate during school suspension were viewed as direct or indirect factors that would protect their well-being after schools reopened3. Moreover, from the perspective of the family, parental support was also important in maintaining their children’s well-being. In this study, we investigated the association between adolescents’ well-being and their self-regulation in terms of behavior, emotions, and cognition, and their parents’ support (see Fig. 1 for the conceptual framework).
Protective factors from family during school suspension were supposed to be associated with both risk and protective factors from adolescents themselves. Adolescents' protective factors were assumed to be associated with their risk factors. Both risk and protective factors were directly associated with adolescents' well being after school resumption.
Greater engagement in digital activities while schools were suspended during the pandemic brought with it potential risks, difficulties, and conflicts. For instance, more participation in digital activities for entertainment (e.g., online gaming or browsing social media) was associated with increased psychological distress and anxiety in adolescents1. In addition, negative experiences from digital-social activities may have increased their exposure to cyberbullying4,5 (e.g., sending or receiving hostile or aggressive messages intended to inflict harm or discomfort on each other6), which may have further impaired their psychological well-being7. Furthermore, with more time spent on digital socialization, adolescents had less time for study, which could have resulted in lower academic self-efficacy8. As lower learning self-efficacy may influence adolescents’ learning performance, school suspension may have increased their worries about their learning outcomes and daily life after schools resumed7,9.
In the face of negative events during school suspension, whether online or face-to-face, it is important for adolescents to be able to use appropriate self-regulatory strategies to cope with the influx of emotionally charged information. Emotional regulation, which is defined as the “conscious, mental strategies individuals use to handle the intake of emotionally arousing information”10, is an important self-regulatory process developed in adolescence when individuals learn to cope with emotional experiences arising from negative events11.
Emotional regulation strategies can protect against emotional problems12 and enhance well-being13. For instance, the capacity for self-regulatory resilience and positivity plays a crucial protective role in ensuring emotional and cognitive well-being after involvement in cyberbullying incidents14,15. For academic study, emotional regulation may modulate the effects of emotional states on adolescents’ academic self-efficacy16.
Increased opportunities for interaction between parents and children during the COVID-19 school suspension may have improved their relationships17 but also cause more family conflicts. To understand how adolescents’ well-being was affected during school suspension, family factors cannot be ignored. According to spillover theory18, individuals are embedded in various interdependent social systems. Changes in one system (e.g., family relationships) can alter the correlates (e.g., emotions) that can affect social interactions in other systems (e.g., peer relationships).
Improvement in parent–child relationships and parents’ appropriate monitoring may have benefited their children’s self-regulation19. A healthy parent–child relationship and parental monitoring have both been found to be associated with improving adolescents’ emotional regulation strategies20,21 and helping to maintain their social-emotional wellness in other connected systems. For instance, in seeking to ameliorate the feelings of emptiness and loneliness caused by a poor parent–child relationship22, adolescents may become addicted to online socialization as they share and communicate their feelings with others23.
Different types of parental monitoring such as restrictive21 or neglectful styles23 can affect adolescents’ participation in both academic and non-academic digital activities. For instance, a good parent–child relationship and school-related parental monitoring can have positive effects on children’s academic self-efficacy24,25. In contrast, ineffective parental monitoring may result in children’s involvement in cyberbullying incidents26,27,28.
Sex differences have been shown to influence both adolescents’ individual factors (e.g., adolescents’ participation in digital activities29,30, involvement15 in cyberbullying31,32, academic self-efficacy33, psychological status34,35, use of cognitive emotional regulation strategies36,37) and family factors (e.g., parental involvement38,39). Sex differences also exist in the interactions of these factors15,29,39,40. From a developmental perspective, adolescents respond to regulations on emotions36,41, behaviors42, and cognition43 differently as their age increases.
As self-regulatory processes can vary across environments, the conditions during the COVID-19 pandemic can provide new insights into how adolescents perceive, think, and behave during epidemiological disasters, as well as the effects on their daily life and their studies after the pandemic. This study aimed to understand the effect of adolescents’ individual and family factors on online and offline social-ecological systems during school suspensions and the links of those systems to adolescents’ well-being after school resumption.
First, studies1,4,5 have shown that it is unclear whether and to what extent participation in digital-social activities, particularly cyberbullying incidents (i.e., negative experiences), were associated with adolescents’ self-regulation in their online studies and emotions during the COVID-19-related school suspension and their well-being after school resumption. The present study presents us with an opportunity to investigate these associations, which can help protect adolescents’ well-being in the future when facing the changes and challenges of daily life and study during difficult periods.
Second, we do not know whether adolescents’ self-regulatory behaviors, emotions, and cognition can be influenced or protected by the quality of parent–child relationships and parental monitoring27,44,45,46, particularly with parents and children spending more time together during COVID-19 lockdowns17. From the perspective of parental involvement, the COVID-19-related school suspension has provided us with a natural setting to investigate whether and how these changes benefit or impede children’s self-regulatory processes toward changes or challenges in the future.
In addition, the findings on sex and age differences within such factors and associations are inconclusive47. Sex and age perspectives must be taken into account to provide insights into how best to support adolescents in future. This study conducted a multiple group analysis to investigate the above associations in relation to sex and age differences.
The results of this exploration can provide insights for parents, teachers, and school leaders from the perspectives of both the family and the school to equip adolescents with the self-regulatory capacity they need when facing similar challenges in the future.
Results
Self-reported data were obtained from the “eCitizen Education 360” project, which explored how to help students adjust to their studies and daily life in the New Normal brought about by the pandemic. A total of 932 students from 23 secondary schools in Hong Kong completed surveys on cognitive-emotional regulation strategies (CER), participation in digital activities for socialization and entertainment (DSE), online learning self-efficacy (OSE), cyberbullying involvement (CyI), and their perceived worries for future study and life (WOR). Their parents completed surveys on the perceived improvement in the parent–child relationship (PCR) and parental monitoring (PM) of their children’s online activities.
The goodness of the model fit
The goodness of the model fit of all the confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) models was satisfactory, which was supported by a comparative fit index (CFI) above .95 and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) below .08 (see Table 1). The model fit for cyberbullying involvement was examined by reviewing the residual plots for various ability subgroups, which sufficiently supported the model fit. The reliability for each scale was satisfied with values of ΩCER = 0.71, ΩDSE = 0.74, ΩOSE = 0.82, ΩWOR = 0.73, ΩPCR = 0.84, and ΩPM = 0.74 and empirical reliability of CyI = 0.79. Detailed psychometric analysis procedures, the results of the model fit, and the empirical reliability of CyI, can be found in the method section. This study performed multiple-group structural equation modeling (MG-SEM) for female and male students based on these factors. The model fit of MG-SEM was satisfactory (RMSEA = 0.048, CFI = 0.903, standardized root mean squared residual (SRMR) = 0.065).
Adolescents’ participation in digital activity during school suspension and their well-being
The participation of both girls (see Fig. 2) and boys (see Fig. 3) in DSE during the COVID-19-related school suspension was not significantly associated with adolescents’ perceived worries for study and life (WOR) after school resumed.
PCR Parent-child relationship improvement, PM Parental monitoring, CER Cognitive-emotional regulation,; DSE Digital activities for socialization and entertainment, OSE Online learning self-efficacy, CyI Cyberbullying involvement, WOR Perceived worries for future study and life. Black arrows represent statistically significant direct effect, dotted arrows represent statistically insignificant direct effect, and red arrow represents statistically significant different direct effect between male group and female group. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01.
PCR Parent-child relationship improvement, PM Parental monitoring, CER Cognitive-emotional regulation, DSE Digital activities for socialization and entertainment, OSE Online learning self-efficacy, CyI Cyberbullying involvement, WOR Perceived worries for future study and life. Black arrows represent statistically significant direct effect, dotted arrows represent statistically insignificant direct effect, and red arrow represents statistically significant different direct effect between male group and female group. *p < 0.05. **p < 0.01.
Girls’ DSE was negatively associated with OSE (β = −0.22, p < 0.01) but positively associated with CyI (β = 0.15, p < 0.01). Boys’ DSE was not significantly associated with any other variable (i.e., OSE, CyI, and WOR).
CyI was positively associated with WOR for boys (β = 0.20, p < 0.05), while there was no significant association between CyI and WOR for girls (β = 0.06, p = 0.17).
Adolescents’ self-regulation during school suspension and their well-being
Use of CER during school suspension was positively associated with adolescents’ OSE (βgirls = 0.47, p < 0.01; βboys = 0.43, p < 0.01). OSE was significantly associated with perceived WOR both for girls (β = −0.64, p < 0.01) and boys (β = −0.42, p < 0.01).
The association between OSE during school suspension and their WOR after school resumption for girls was significantly stronger (DOSE→WOR = 0.35, p < 0.05) than for boys, controlling for student age. CyI was significantly associated with CER for boys (β = −0.20, p < .05) but not for girls (β = −0.04, p = 0.43).
Effects of adolescents’ age and overall effects of digital activity and self-regulation factors
Age was not significantly associated with the variables of DSE, CyI, CER, and OSE during school suspension for boys. These four variables together explained 24.2% of the variance in boys’ WOR after school resumption.
Girls’ age was significantly associated with their DSE (β = 0.13, p < 0.01) and CyI (β = −0.16, p < 0.01) during school suspension, and with WOR (β = 0.11, p < 0.01) after school resumption. The variables DSE, CyI, CER, and OSE during school suspension explained 46.5% of the variance in WOR for girls after school suspension.
Family factors in adolescents’ digital activity participation and self-regulation during school suspension
During the school suspension, girls’ relationship with their parents (PCR) was positively associated with their use of CER (β = .11, p < .05) and OSE (β = 0.18, p < 0.01) and negatively associated with their DSE (β = −0.15, p < 0.01) but was not significantly associated with CyI (β = −0.05, p = 0.29). Girls’ PM was not significantly associated with any of the digital activity and self-regulation factors (i.e., CER, OSE, CyI, and DSE). For boys, neither their PCR nor PM was significantly associated with any of these four factors.
Mediator effects of adolescents’ digital activities participation and self-regulation on their well-being
Using CER and the improvement in girls’ PCR during school suspension were both negatively associated with their WOR after school resumed through their OSE during school suspension (CER: β = −0.35, p < 0.01; PCR: β = −0.11, p < 0.01), whereas the DSE of girls was positively associated with WOR through OSE (β = 0.13, p < 0.01).
The PCR of girls during school suspension was negatively associated with WOR through CER and OSE during school suspension in sequence (β = −0.03, p < 0.05) and through DSE and OSE during school suspension in sequence (β = −0.02, p < 0.05).
Boys’ CER during school suspension was negatively associated with WOR after school resumption and through OSE during school suspension (β = −.19, p < .01).
The indirect effects from CER to WOR through OSE (DCER → OSE → WOR = 0.16, p < 0.05, see Table 2), DSE to WOR through OSE (DDES → OSE → WOR = −0.11, p < 0.05), and PCR to WOR through DSE and OSE (D PCR → DES → OSE → WOR = 0.02, p < 0.05) were significantly stronger in female adolescents than in males.
Discussion
This study demonstrated that adolescents’ well-being after school resumed was affected by risk and protective factors from the adolescents themselves and by protective factors from their families during the COVID-19 school suspension. As a potential risk factor, adolescents engaging in more DSE during school suspension was not a direct risk factor affecting their perceived WOR after school resumption, as is usually assumed. Although engaging in more DSE during school suspension may have potentially increased girls’ CyI, the increase did not appear to influence their WOR after school resumption. In contrast, boys who engaged in more DSE during the period did not increase their CyI, but their CyI may have negatively influenced their WOR after school resumption.
As a protective factor, higher OSE during school suspension may have protected adolescents by reducing their WOR after school resumption. However, the protective effect was higher for girls, although girls’ DSE engagement may have been a risk factor that lowered their OSE during school suspension. As another protective factor, adolescents’ use of more CER when faced with negative events during school suspension was positively associated with their OSE. For boys, CER even protected them from CyI during school suspension.
A supportive family environment (i.e., PCR improvement) during school suspension helped safeguard girls’ behavioral and emotional well-being, leading to less DSE involvement, more CER use, and improvement in OSE. Furthermore, as a protective factor, PCR was also helpful in strengthening girls’ OSE during school suspension and reducing their WOR after school resumption. However, this protective effect was not found in the boys group.
As highlighted by Zimmerman when writing about the importance of self-regulation48, adolescents’ OSE (i.e., their motivational beliefs about self-learning) during school suspension was found to have a large influence over their WOR after school resumed. In particular, the effect of OSE on these worries was much stronger in female adolescents. Yeo et al.47 explained that girls were developmentally more likely to develop a greater sense of personal responsibility for negative events in general. In another study of Hong Kong students, Hui indicated that female adolescents made more internal attributions—focusing on one’s own abilities and efforts—and shouldered more personal responsibility for their concerns49. As studying went online, students needed to perform more self-directed learning, and girls’ regulatory capability may have been stimulated more at this stage of development. Furthermore, the current study demonstrated that OSE explains about double the variance in adolescents’ WOR after school resumption. This may be because female adolescents exhibited higher academic self-regulation—greater ability to focus their attention and to monitor themselves, and more self-control42—in an online learning context at home, which left them well-prepared for the change from school to home study.
As shown in a recent study50, the use of positive CER to deal with COVID-19-related experiences may have a positive association with adolescents’ beliefs in their online learning capabilities during school suspension, especially in female adolescents. This process of positive emotional regulation plays a role in buffering adolescents’ intense moods that might otherwise lead to affective influences on cognition16, thereby protecting them against emotional and academic exhaustion50,51.
Positive CER protected male adolescents from involvement in CyI during the school suspension and reduced their worries about daily life after the school resumption. According to Gianesini and Brighi15, various emotional regulation skills in cyberbullying victims or perpetrators could greatly influence adolescents’ resilience levels, which may further cushion them from the negative influences of peer violence during adolescence.
Echoing research1,5 showing the negative influence on adolescents’ studies and daily life of participation in digital activities, this study indicated that DSE resulted in adolescents showing lower OSE during school suspension and probably resulted in a greater involvement in CyI, and that girls are probably more vulnerable than boys to negative influences from the Internet52. From the cognitive perspective, brain structure and development in males and females may be different, with a greater influence on the regions related to cognitive control and reward/loss processing in females52. However, from a cultural perspective, this is probably because Chinese parents traditionally allocate more resources and guidance to their sons than to their daughters53. Parents monitor their sons and stop them from over-engaging in leisure-oriented digital activities, instead encouraging them to gain digital skills for studying.
However, as suggested in a study by Barlett et al.4, which did not consider the issue of age, this study showed that cyberbullying issues decreased with age for girls but not for boys. According to the Pew Research Center54, young girls experience particularly severe forms of online harassment. However, girls might develop self-regulatory digital resilience14,15 (e.g., understanding more about different types of cyberbullying and knowing how to handle them) earlier than boys to reduce their CyI. For boys, DSE was insignificantly associated with CyI, indicating that there might be factors in their other offline social systems18 (e.g., family and peer relationships) that must be taken into account for a comprehensive investigation.
Consistent with Kaufman et al.18, changes in one system (e.g., the relationship between parents and children) can alter the correlates affecting other systems (e.g., adolescents’ self-regulation). This study suggests that improvement in parent–child relationships can influence children’s behavior (i.e., participation in online activities), emotion (i.e., CER), and cognition (i.e., OSE24,25). Teenagers benefit from an emotionally secure family environment55,56. A friendly family relationship builds an environment that is more open to discussion among family members, and this kind of family system is likely to be more resilient and to be able to adapt to changes. Therefore, adolescents from such families can allocate more of their cognitive load to focus more effectively on their studies and daily life.
The current study has several limitations that could be addressed in future studies. First, the parents reported the parent–child relationship and parental monitoring, but their children may have different perceptions of these two parenting variables. Future studies could explore the effect of the perceived differences between parents and children in terms of the parenting variables. Second, our cross-sectional study may not reflect the long-term changes in adolescents’ well-being. A longitudinal study of the potential risk or protective factors that can affect adolescents’ well-being could be conducted to investigate the long-term changes in adolescents’ self-regulatory behavior, emotions, and cognition resulting from the pandemic, through the post-pandemic period, and into the New Normal. Third, this study did not take into account teacher-level or school-level factors. We cannot assume that all parents are naturally skilled at parent–child relationship management and parental monitoring. Teachers should provide professional support to educate parents on this theme, especially in a digital context. Therefore, from a multi-level perspective, a future study could include school-related factors (e.g., family–school cooperation in the digital realm), in addition to parent-related factors, to provide more insights into ways of supporting adolescents’ well-being during school suspension and after its resumption.
Methods
Context
This study was part of Hong Kong’s “eCitizen Education 360” project. It involved five comprehensive surveys of students, parents, teachers, school leaders, and ICT (information and communications technology) coordinators. Its aim was to prepare students for the changes in study and daily life in the new normal and to comprehensively enhance our community’s ability to improve educational opportunities, digital competence, and the well-being of students.
Data collection procedure
All of the primary and secondary schools in all of Hong Kong’s 18 districts were invited to participate in this study on a voluntary basis through our project website (https://www.ecitizen.hk/360/call-for-participation). A person in charge (PIC) from each participating school notified us of their intention to take part. The specific grade levels and numbers of classes participating were decided by the individual schools based on their needs and administrative capacity. The links to the surveys for different roles were sent to the PIC, who helped to distribute the surveys to the corresponding participants for completion. Students took part in the survey with the assistance of their teachers.
Consent was obtained from the parents of participating students. Ethics approval for the study was obtained from the authors’ institution (the University of Hong Kong) before data were collected. The participants gave written informed consent to complete the survey and participate in the study.
All of the surveys were conducted anonymously through an online survey system, Qualtrics, from June 8, 2020 in secondary schools and from June 20, 2020 in primary schools. Respondents could choose to respond to the English or Chinese version of the survey. The Chinese version was translated from the original English version and was validated by back-translation. Data collected up to July 14 were included in our analysis, which excluded primary school students. About 550 school leaders, 790 teachers, 1300 parents, and 6300 students participated in the project.
Samples
In this study, our sample consisted of 932 students and their parents from 23 secondary schools. Over 66% of the students were from junior secondary schools, and the rest were from senior secondary schools. The female students had a mean age of 14.80 years (SD = 1.56), and the male students had a mean age of 15.00 years (SD = 1.60). The parents had a diverse range of educational attainment (see Table 3).
Measures
Student surveys
The OSE survey was adapted from the Self-Efficacy Questionnaire for Children57. The survey consisted of six items that measured students’ beliefs in their capacity for online learning during the school suspension (i.e., “I could focus on my studies when there were other interesting things to do.”) scored on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree).
The WOR survey consisted of six items designed to measure students’ concerns about their studies and life after school resumption (e.g., “I cannot catch up with my schoolwork.”) scored on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree).
The CER survey consisted of three items adapted from the cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire10. The items measured the extent to which students were able to use cognitive strategies to cope with unpleasant events during school suspension (e.g., “I thought that I must accept it.”) scored on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never, 5 = always).
The DSE survey consisted of five items that measured the extent to which a student participated in online socialization and entertainment activities daily during school suspension (e.g., chatting with friends via WhatsApp) scored on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all, 5 = more than 5 times a day).
The CyI survey was adapted from a validated instrument58 and measured students’ involvement as a perpetrator of cyberbullying, a victim, and a bystander. The participants reported their experiences of six cyberbullying events (e.g., something embarrassing or mean about another person) based on their roles in these events (i.e., bully, victim, both bully and victim, bystander, or never experienced).
Parent surveys
The PCR survey evaluated the parent’s relationship with their child during school suspension using four items (e.g., “I have understood my child’s ability more.”) scored on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree).
The PM survey consisted of three items that measured the extent to which parents monitored their children’s online behavior (e.g., “I monitored my child’s apps/websites/YouTube channels.”) scored on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = never, 5 = always).
Analytic plan
Examining latent constructs and reliability
The latent construct of each factor, except for items that measured students’ CyI, was examined by applying a CFA using robust maximum likelihood estimation. The responses of these scales had acceptable ranges for normal distribution, with skewness ranging between −1 and 2 and kurtosis ranging between −2 and 259. All of the CFA models were identified by setting the latent factor mean to 0 and the latent factor variances to 1. All of the item intercepts, item factor loadings, and item residual variances were freely estimated. The goodness of model fit was assessed by CFI, RMSEA, and SRMR. A CFI greater than .90 and RMSEA and SRMR values of less than .08 were considered as satisfying the model fit60. McDonald’s omega61 was used to calculate the reliability for each CFA model.
The general expression of reliability Omega is shown as follows:
Where, λi represents the factor loading for Item i, θii and θij represent the error variance and error covariance, respectively. As a result, coefficient omega represents the proportion of variance in the observed total score attributable to all “modeled” sources of common variance. The coefficient omega ranges from 0 to 1, and a higher value indicates higher reliability of the scale. The coefficient omega listed above assumes uni-dimensionality, therefore, it is suitable for the single-factor model. The Omega hierarchical was adopted to estimate the reliability of the higher-order factor in the higher-order model in the current study62.
As there is no clear consensus on different categories of cyberbullying experience, this study used the nominal response model (NRM63) to measure the extent of CyI. The NRM is a flexible polytomous Item Response Theory model that does not require the ordering of item responses. For example, it does not assume the order of each category of cyberbullying experience before the estimation, but it estimates the order of each category based on the data. The current NRM assumes that a continuous latent trait (θ) influences unordered item responses, which is identified by setting the variance of θ equal to 1 and the mean of θ equal to 0, and then determines the estimate by maximum marginal likelihood using the Expectation-Maximization algorithm64. The Expected A-Posteriori method65 was adopted to compute factor scores for each participant, with a higher score indicating the participant had greater involvement in cyberbullying.
The nominal response model63 assumes a continuous latent factor (θ) accounts for the covariances among items without assuming the orders (i.e., nominal responses). Assume Xi is the response on the ith item and p(Xi = k|θ) is the conditional probability of selecting response category k (k = 1,…,mi) for item i. the categories are nomial, meaning no assumption on selecting category 2 reflects a higher level of θ than selecting category 1. The NRM could be expressed as
Where aik and cik are the category slope and category intercept parameters for the kth category of item I, respectively (see Table 4). In NRM, the probability that a person with trait-level θ chooses option k on item the expression on the right gives me, the ratio of selecting one category over the sum of all the other categories, as a function of θ with a varying category slope parameter (aik) and a varying category intercept parameters cik for each of the mi response categories. To identify the model, the mean and variance of θ are fixed to 0 and 1, respectively. Within each item, the order for the response categories with respect to the latent trait is determined by the value of aik, a larger value of aik indicates a higher level of θ. For example, if aik > aiq, thus response k indicates higher θ than response q. The intercept parameter, cik reflect the relative frequency of selecting category k, where larger cik reflects relatively higher frequency for category k. After fitting the NRM using Eq. 3, item parameters and person’s latent trait could be estimated66. The empirical reliability of the cyberbullying involvement scale was estimated in the following way.
From the NRM model in Eq. 3, we obtained the individual’s level of cyberbullying involvement (θ) and the standard error (SE(θ)) using the EAP method. Then, the empirical reliability \({\uprho}_{{{{\mathrm{xx}}}}^\prime }\) was calculated as the ratio of the true score variance and the total score variance, the sum of the true score variance and error variance67.
Where, \(\widehat {SE}\left( {\hat \theta } \right)^2\)was obtained by averaging across the N standard errors of respective \(\widehat {\theta _i}\) and squaring it.
Item fit of NRM was examined by viewing the residual plots of each plot. The difference between the line (model implied) and dots (data implied) represents the residuals. The smaller the residuals are, the better model-data fit is. The figures below presented the Empirical plots of all items of NRM. As shown in the Fig. 4 from item 1 to 6, the residuals were small enough to support achieving a good model fit63.
Exploring the relationships among parent and student factors
MG-SEM was performed using Mplus (version 8.1) to address the direct effects, indirect effects, and corresponding differences between male and female students. Bias-corrected confidence intervals with 10,000 bootstrapped samples were used to test the indirect effects. The measurement invariance between the sexes was examined and the partial measurement invariance was also determined. The structural relationships between the factors in the four groups were uncontaminated by measurement errors or measurement differences68. The male group was set as the reference group by fixing the factor means and variance to 0 and 1 respectively, to allow the estimation of female factor means and variances.
A standard stepwise procedure of testing measurement invariance between two gender groups was followed, in which the measurement invariance test started from a configural invariant model, a metric invariant model, and a scale invariant model.
Step 0. Test the CFA model in the whole, male, and female samples, respectively.
Step 1. A configural invariant model was specified. The CFA model for two gender groups was estimated simultaneously. All factor variances were fixed to 1, and factor means were fixed to 0 to set the scale. All loadings, residual variances, intercepts, and factor covariances were freely estimated. The model fit indices were checked to ensure the acceptable model fit was achieved.
Step 2. A metric invariant model was examined by constraining the items to have equal loadings across time. The factor variance was fixed to 1 in the male group but was freely estimated in the female group. All factor means were fixed to 0. Other parameters were constrained as the configural model. Then a Likelihood Ratio Test (LRT) was conducted to test if the metric invariant model did not fit worse than the configural model.
Step 3. A scalar invariant model was examined by constraining the same items had to have equal intercepts across genders; the factor means in the male group was fixed at 0, but other factor means were freely estimated. Other parameters constraints were set the same as the previous model. Then the LRT was conducted to test if the scalar invariant model did not fit worse than the metric model.
A similar procedure was conducted for each factor. In the empirical studies, when the full invariant model cannot be achieved, previous studies showed that a partial invariant model (a partial factor loading and partial intercept invariant model) could ensure the meaningful comparison of latent factor means68. Therefore, when the full invariant model was not achieved, we freely estimated some items across the group based on the modification indices and a content review. As a result, we freely estimated one item of measuring students’ digital socialization.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Data availability
Data analyzed in the project are available upon request. Please contact the corresponding author.
Code availability
The code for data analysis used in the project is available upon request. Please contact the corresponding author.
References
Al-Dwaikat, T. N., Aldalaykeh, M. & Rababa, M. The relationship between social networking sites usage and psychological distress among undergraduate students during COVID-19 lockdown. Heliyon 6, e05695 (2020).
Rogers, A. A., Ha, T. & Ockey, S. Adolescents’ perceived socio-emotional impact of COVID-19 and implications for mental health: Results from a US-based mixed-methods study. J. Adolesc. Health 68, 43–52 (2021).
Liu, Y. et al. Associations between feelings/behaviors during COVID-19 pandemic lockdown and depression/anxiety after lockdown in a sample of Chinese children and adolescents. J. Affect. Disord. 284, 98–103 (2021).
Barlett, C. P., Simmers, M. M., Roth, B., & Gentile, D. Comparing cyberbullying prevalence and process before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Soc. Psychol. 161, 408–418 (2021).
Tao, S., Reichert, F., Law, N. & Rao, N. Digital technology use and cyberbullying among primary school children: Digital literacy and parental mediation as moderators. Cyberpsychology, Behav., Soc. Netw. 25, 571–579 (2022).
Tokunaga, R. S. Following you home from school: A critical review and synthesis of research on cyberbullying victimization. Comput. Hum. Behav. 26, 277–287 (2010).
Alhujailli, A., Karwowski, W., Wan, T. T. & Hancock, P. Affective and stress consequences of cyberbullying. Symmetry 12, 1536 (2020).
McNallie, J., Timmermans, E., Dorrance Hall, E., Van den Bulck, J. & Wilson, S. R. Social media intensity and first-year college students’ academic self-efficacy in Flanders and the United States. Commun. Q. 68, 115–137 (2020).
Gambin, M. & Święcicka, M. Construction and validation of Self-Efficacy Scale for early school-aged children. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 9, 723–729 (2012).
Garnefski, N. & Kraaij, V. Bully victimization and emotional problems in adolescents: Moderation by specific cognitive coping strategies? J. Adolescence 37, 1153–1160 (2014).
Hoge, E., Bickham, D. & Cantor, J. Digital media, anxiety, and depression in children. Pediatrics 140, S76–S80 (2017).
Garnefski, N. & Kraaij, V. The cognitive emotion regulation questionnaire. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 23, 141–149 (2007).
Karademas, E. C. Positive and negative aspects of well-being: Common and specific predictors. Personal. Individ. Differ. 43, 277–287 (2007).
Vranjes, I., Erreygers, S., Vandebosch, H., Baillien, E. & De Witte, H. Patterns of cybervictimization and emotion regulation in adolescents and adults. Aggress. Behav. 44, 647–657 (2018).
Gianesini, G., & Brighi, A. Cyberbullying in the era of digital relationships: The unique role of resilience and emotion regulation on adolescents’ adjustment. In Technology and Youth: Growing Up in a Digital World (Emerald Group Publishing Limited, 2015).
Medrano, L. A., Flores-Kanter, E., Moretti, L. & Pereno, G. L. Effects of induction of positive and negative emotional states on academic self-efficacy beliefs in college students. Psicol.ía Educativa 22, 135–141 (2016).
Uzun, H., Karaca, N. H. & Metin, Ş. Assessment of parent-child relationship in Covid-19 pandemic. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 120, 105748 (2021).
Kaufman, T. M., Kretschmer, T., Huitsing, G. & Veenstra, R. Caught in a vicious cycle? Explaining bidirectional spillover between parent-child relationships and peer victimization. Dev. Psychopathol. 32, 11–20 (2020).
Cho, M. K., Kim, H. Y. & Moon, S. S. The longitudinal effects of influence of parental attachment and emotion regulation on Internet delinquency in early adolescence. J. Hum. Behav. Soc. Environ. 26, 587–596 (2016).
Chen, V. H. H. & Chng, G. S. Active and restrictive parental mediation over time: Effects on youths’ self-regulatory competencies and impulsivity. Computers Educ. 98, 206–212 (2016).
Hapunda, G., Mahama, S., Mesurado, B., Verma, S. K. & Koller, S. Cultural variability and consistency in adolescents’ emotional regulation and relationship with their parents: data from Argentina, Ghana, India and Zambia. Int. J. Adolesc. Youth 24, 333–350 (2019).
Janssens, J. J. et al. The impact of COVID‐19 on adolescents’ daily lives: The role of parent–child relationship quality. J. Res. Adolescence 31, 623–644 (2021).
Szwedo, D. E., Mikami, A. Y. & Allen, J. P. Qualities of peer relations on social networking websites: Predictions from negative mother–teen interactions. J. Res. Adolescence 21, 595–607 (2011).
Yuan, S., Weiser, D. A. & Fischer, J. L. Self-efficacy, parent–child relationships, and academic performance: A comparison of European American and Asian American college students. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 19, 261–280 (2016).
Affuso, G., Bacchini, D. & Miranda, M. C. The contribution of school-related parental monitoring, self-determination, and self-efficacy to academic achievement. J. Educ. Res. 110, 565–574 (2017).
Hellfeldt, K., López-Romero, L. & Andershed, H. Cyberbullying and psychological well-being in young adolescence: The potential protective mediation effects of social support from family, friends, and teachers. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 45 (2020).
Livazović, G. & Bojčić, K. Revisiting the clockwork orange: A review of theories of aggressive behaviour from the perspective of the ecological systems theory. Polic. i Sigur. 29, 0–0 (2020).
Shadmanfaat, S. M. et al. Cyberbullying perpetration: An empirical test of social learning theory in Iran. Deviant Behav. 41, 278–293 (2020).
Lai, C. H. & Gwung, H. L. The effect of gender and Internet usage on physical and cyber interpersonal relationships. Comput. Educ. 69, 303–309 (2013).
Yuen, A. H., Lau, W. W., Park, J. H., Lau, G. K. & Chan, A. K. Digital equity and students’ home computing: A Hong Kong study. Asia-Pac. Educ. Researcher 25, 509–518 (2016).
Baldry, A. C., Sorrentino, A. & Farrington, D. P. Cyberbullying and cybervictimization versus parental supervision, monitoring and control of adolescents’ online activities. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 96, 302–307 (2019).
Chen, J. K. & Chen, L. M. Cyberbullying among adolescents in Taiwan, Hong Kong, and Mainland China: A cross-national study in Chinese societies. Asia Pac. J. Soc. Work Dev. 30, 227–241 (2020).
McKay, M. T., Dempster, M. & Byrne, D. G. An examination of the relationship between self-efficacy and stress in adolescents: The role of gender and self-esteem. J. Youth Stud. 17, 1131–1151 (2014).
Martínez-Monteagudo, M. C., Inglés, C. J., Granados, L., Aparisi, D. & García-Fernández, J. M. Trait emotional intelligence profiles, burnout, anxiety, depression, and stress in secondary education teachers. Personal. Individ. Differ. 142, 53–61 (2019).
Sun, X., Chan, D. W. & Chan, L. K. Self-compassion and psychological well-being among adolescents in Hong Kong: Exploring gender differences. Personal. Individ. Differ. 101, 288–292 (2016).
Sanchis-Sanchis, A., Grau, M. D., Moliner, A. R. & Morales-Murillo, C. P. Effects of age and gender in emotion regulation of children and adolescents. Front. Psychol. 11, 946 (2020).
te Brinke, L. W., Menting, A. T., Schuiringa, H. D., Zeman, J. & Deković, M. The structure of emotion regulation strategies in adolescence: Differential links to internalizing and externalizing problems. Soc. Dev. 30, 536–553 (2021).
Shek, D. T. A longitudinal study of perceived parental psychological control and psychological well‐being in Chinese adolescents in Hong Kong. J. Clin. Psychol. 63, 1–22 (2007).
Song, H., Lee, Y. & Kim, J. Gender differences in the link between cyberbullying and parental supervision trajectories. Crime. Delinquency 66, 1914–1936 (2020).
Li, J., Yao, M. & Liu, H. From social support to adolescents’ subjective well-being: The mediating role of emotion regulation and prosocial behavior and gender difference. Child Indic. Res. 14, 77–93 (2021).
Silvers, J. A. et al. Age-related differences in emotional reactivity, regulation, and rejection sensitivity in adolescence. Emotion 12, 1235 (2012).
Tetering, M. V., Laan, A. V. D., Kogel, C. D., Groot, R. D. & Jolles, J. Sex differences in self-regulation in early, middle and late adolescence: A large-scale cross-sectional study. PLoS One 15, e0227607 (2020).
DeSmet, A., Rodelli, M., Walrave, M., Soenens, B. & Cardon, G. & De Bourdeaudhuij, I. Cyberbullying and traditional bullying involvement among heterosexual and non-heterosexual adolescents, and their associations with age and gender. Comput. Hum. Behav. 83, 254–261 (2018).
Brumariu, L. E. Parent–child attachment and emotion regulation. In G. Bosmans & K. A. Kerns (Eds.), Attachment in middle childhood: Theoretical advances and new directions in an emerging field. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 148, 31–45, (2015).
Moilanen, K. L., Padilla-Walker, L. M. & Blaacker, D. R. Dimensions of short-term and long-term self-regulation in adolescence: Associations with maternal and paternal parenting and parent-child relationship quality. J. Youth Adolescence 47, 1409–1426 (2018).
Mulyadi, S., Rahardjo, W. & Basuki, A. H. The role of parent-child relationship, self-esteem, academic self-efficacy to academic stress. Procedia-Soc. Behav. Sci. 217, 603–608 (2016).
Yeo, L. S. & Tan, K. Attributional style and self-efficacy in Singaporean adolescents. J. Psychol. Couns. Sch. 22, 82–101 (2012).
Zimmerman, B. J. Achieving self-regulation: The trial and triumph of adolescence. In F. Pajares & T. C. Urdan (Eds.), Adolescence and Education (Vol. 2, pp. 1–28) (Greenwich, CT: Information Age, 2002).
Hui, E. K. P. Personal concerns and their causes: Perceptions of Hong Kong Chinese adolescent students. J. Adolesc. 23, 189–203 (2000).
Usán Supervía, P. & Quílez Robres, A. Emotional Regulation and Academic Performance in the Academic Context: The Mediating Role of Self-Efficacy in Secondary Education Students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 5715 (2021).
Dominguez-Lara, S. A. Agotamiento emocional académico en estudiantes universitarios:¿ Cuánto influyen las estrategias cognitivas de regulación emocional? Educación Médica 19, 96–103 (2018).
Wang, Z. et al. Females are more vulnerable to Internet gaming disorder than males: Evidence from cortical thickness abnormalities. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 283, 145–153 (2019).
Chan, Y. M. Self-esteem: A cross-cultural comparison of British-Chinese white-British and Hong Kong Chinese children. Educ. Psychol. 20, 59–74 (2000).
Pew Research Center, “Online Harassment”. http://www.pewinternet.org/2014/10/22/online-harassment/ (2014).
Kerr, K. L. et al. Parental influences on neural mechanisms underlying emotion regulation. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 16, 100118 (2019).
Morris, A. S., Criss, M. M., Silk, J. S. & Houltberg, B. J. The impact of parenting on emotion regulation during childhood and adolescence. Child Dev. Perspect. 11, 233–238 (2017).
Muris, P. A brief questionnaire for measuring self-efficacy in youths. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 23, 145–149 (2001).
Shapka, J. D., Onditi, H. Z., Collie, R. J. & Lapidot‐Lefler, N. Cyberbullying and cybervictimization within a cross‐cultural context: A study of Canadian and Tanzanian adolescents. Child Dev. 89, 89–99 (2018).
Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., & Anderson, R. E. Multivariate data analysis: A global perspective (7th ed.). (New Jersey: Pearson Prentice Hall, 2010).
Hu, L. T. & Bentler, P. M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Modeling: A Multidiscip. J. 6, 1–55 (1999).
McDonald, R. P. Test theory: A unified approach. (pp. 89). (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Associates, 1999).
Brunner, M., Nagy, G. & Wilhelm, O. A tutorial on hierarchically structured constructs. J. Personal. 80, 796–846 (2012).
Bock, R. D. Estimating item parameters and latent ability when responses are scored in two or more nominal categories. Psychometrika 37, 29–51 (1972). 63.
Bock, R. D. & Aitkin, M. Marginal maximum likelihood estimation of item parameters: an application of an EM algorithm. Psychometrika 46, 443–459 (1981).
Thissen, D., Pommerich, M., Billeaud, K. & Williams, V. S. L. Item response theory for scores on tests including polytomous items with ordered responses. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 19, 39–49 (1995).
Thissen, D., Cai, L., & Bock, R. D. The nominal categories item response model. In M. Nering & R. Ostini (Eds.), Handbook of polytomous item response theory models (pp. 43–75). (New York, NY: Routledge, 2010).
Chalmers, R. P. mirt: A multidimensional item response theory package for the R Environment. J. Stat. Softw. 48, 1–29 (2012).
Byrne, B. M., Shavelson, R. J. & Muthén, B. Testing for the equivalence of factor covariance and mean structures: The issue of partial measurement invariance. Psychol. Bull. 105, 456–466 (1989).
Acknowledgements
This project is funded by the Research Grants Council of the HKSAR Government, #T44-707/16-N, under the Theme-based Research Scheme. The authors are grateful to the D.H. Chen Foundation for their support in the preparation and publication of this manuscript.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Conceptualization: M.L.; data analysis: Q.Q.P.; funding acquisition: N.W.Y.L.; investigation: M.L., Q.Q.P., C.Y.T., N.W.Y.L.; methodology: M.L. and Q.Q.P.; supervision: C.Y.T. and N.W.Y.L.; visualization: M.L.; writing – original draft: M.L. and Q.Q.P; and writing – review and editing: all authors (based on CRediT: Contributor Roles Taxonomy, https://casrai.org/credit/). All of the authors have agreed upon the final completed version. All of the authors hold accountability for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests. N.W.Y.L is an editor for npj Science of Learning. N.W.Y.L was not involved in the journal’s review of, or decisions related to, this manuscript.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Lan, M., Pan, Q., Tan, C.Y. et al. Understanding protective and risk factors affecting adolescents’ well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic. npj Sci. Learn. 7, 32 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41539-022-00149-4
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41539-022-00149-4