Abstract
The brain predicts the sensory consequences of our movements and uses these predictions to attenuate the perception of self-generated sensations. Accordingly, self-generated touch feels weaker than an externally generated touch of identical intensity. In schizophrenia, this somatosensory attenuation is substantially reduced, suggesting that patients with positive symptoms fail to accurately predict and process self-generated touch. If an impaired prediction underlies the positive symptoms of schizophrenia, then a similar impairment should exist in healthy nonclinical individuals with high positive schizotypal traits. One hundred healthy participants (53 female), assessed for schizotypal traits, underwent a well-established psychophysics force discrimination task to quantify how they perceived self-generated and externally generated touch. The perceived intensity of tactile stimuli delivered to their left index finger (magnitude) and the ability to discriminate the stimuli (precision) was measured. We observed that higher positive schizotypal traits were associated with reduced somatosensory attenuation and poorer somatosensory precision of self-generated touch, both when treating schizotypy as a continuous or categorical variable. These effects were specific to positive schizotypy and were not observed for the negative or disorganized dimensions of schizotypy. The results suggest that positive schizotypal traits are associated with a reduced ability to predict and process self-generated touch. Given that the positive dimension of schizotypy represents the analogue of positive psychotic symptoms of schizophrenia, deficits in processing self-generated tactile information could indicate increased liability to schizophrenia.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Distinguishing between the two causes of our sensory input—the self and the environment—is fundamental for survival. First, it enables the nervous system to detect physically harmful situations for the organism and to act accordingly1,2,3: for example, the touch of a spider crawling up one’s arm (externally generated touch) elicits a dramatically different response from the same touch applied by one’s other hand (self-generated touch). Second, this distinction is a prerequisite for maintaining our self-consciousness and consequently our mental health because it allows us to delimit our own intentions, sensations, actions, thoughts, and emotions from those of others4,5,6. For example, we do not mistake our thoughts for the voices of other people we simultaneously have conversation with, because we attribute the cause of our thoughts to ourselves (self-generated ‘voices’) and the cause of the voices we hear to others (externally generated voices).
How do we make this distinction? The brain uses internal forward models to predict the sensory consequences of movements (corollary discharge) using copies of the motor commands (efference copy)3,7,8. These predictions are essential for the fast, online control of movements because they allow the brain to estimate and correct the body’s state despite the inherent delays in the sensory system3,9,10,11. Importantly, these predictions allow the brain to differentiate between self-generated and externally generated sensations: accordingly, those sensations that match the sensory predictions are self-generated, while those that deviate from the predicted ones, or have not been predicted, are attributed to external causes12. Moreover, the brain uses these predictions to attenuate the intensity of the self-generated signals, thereby amplifying the difference between self-generated and externally generated information8,13,14,15. In the tactile domain, this attenuation manifests as perceiving self-generated touch as weaker than an externally generated touch of the same intensity15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26 and in yielding weaker activity in the secondary somatosensory cortex and the cerebellum23,27 and increased functional connectivity between the two areas23. This somatosensory attenuation is considered one of the reasons why we cannot tickle ourselves8,28,29.
In contrast to healthy individuals, patients with schizophrenia show significantly less attenuation of self-generated tactile sensations at the behavioral level30 and do not exhibit attenuation of somatosensory cortical activation for self-generated forces as healthy controls do31. Moreover, patients with positive symptoms, such as auditory hallucinations and delusions of control, often fail to attenuate self-generated touch and perceive it as if it were externally generated32. Critically, this failure of attenuation is positively correlated with the severity of their hallucinations: the more severe the hallucinations, the lower the somatosensory attenuation31.
These findings have supported the neuropsychiatric view that the positive symptoms of schizophrenia can be explained by a deficit in predicting and processing self-generated sensations33,34. Such a deficit should hinder the distinction between self-generated and externally generated sensations35, reduce the sense of agency6,36, and produce perceptual aberrations37, including delusions of control5 and auditory hallucinations36. Consequently, schizophrenia is tightly linked to an atypical perception of self-generated sensations but not externally generated sensations. Despite the heterogeneity of symptoms, schizophrenia has been primarily described as a disorder of the sense of self38,39,40, and self-disorders have been shown to constitute a crucial, trait-like phenotype of the schizophrenia spectrum41.
If the positive symptoms of schizophrenia are intrinsically linked to deficits in predicting and processing self-generated somatosensation, then a similar relationship should exist between positive schizotypy and impaired prediction and processing of self-generated somatosensation in nonclinical individuals. Importantly, this approach circumvents many of the methodological confounds arising from patient studies, such as antipsychotic treatment, hospitalization, and disease chronicity, that the patient groups are typically subjected to42. Schizotypy, or psychosis-proneness, describes subclinical psychosis-like symptoms or personality characteristics, including peculiar beliefs, unusual sensory experiences and odd behavior43,44, that apply to the general population45,46,47,48,49,50. Schizotypal traits are presumed to originate from the same combination of genetic, neurodevelopmental and psychosocial factors as schizophrenia51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59, they lie on a continuum with schizophrenia50 and are considered a valid phenotypic indicator for the liability to psychosis spectrum disorders and for understanding the underlying psychopathology45,46,48,57. Similar to schizophrenia symptom clusters, schizotypy consists of three dimensions, positive, negative, and disorganized45,50,60, that broadly correspond to the positive (e.g., hallucinations and delusions), negative (e.g., alogia and apathy) and disorganized symptoms of schizophrenia (e.g., thought disorder and bizarre behavior)46,61,62,63,64,65,66.
Here, we investigated the relationship between schizotypal traits and the perception of self-generated and externally generated somatosensation in 100 healthy individuals. We hypothesized that high positive schizotypy would be associated with reduced somatosensory attenuation and lower precision of self-generated touch.
Materials and methods
Participants
The data of one hundred and two participants were used in the present study. Current or history of psychological or neurological conditions, as well as the use of any psychoactive drugs or medication, were criteria for exclusion. All participants reported being completely healthy without neurological or psychiatric disorders or taking any medication to treat such conditions. Our sample size was based on two previous studies assessing the relationship between schizotypy and tactile perception in non-clinical samples67,68. The data were pooled from three studies (40, 30, and 32 subjects), all including the same psychophysics task and schizotypy measure, and identical experimental conditions. Two participants were excluded because of missing data in the schizotypy measure. Thus, the final sample consisted of one hundred (100) adults (53 women and 47 men; 91 right-handed, 5 ambidextrous, and 4 left-handed; age range: 18–40 years). Handedness was assessed using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory69. All participants provided written informed consent, and the Swedish Ethical Review Authority (https://etikprovningsmyndigheten.se/) approved all three studies (#2020-03647, #2020-03186, #2020-05457).
Psychophysical task
The psychophysical paradigm was a two-alternative forced-choice force-discrimination task that has been extensively used to assess somatosensory attenuation15,18,24,25,70,71. On each trial, the participants received two taps (test and comparison taps) on the pulp of their left index fingers, and they had to verbally indicate which felt stronger: the first or the second tap. The intensity of the test tap was set to 2 N, while the intensity of the comparison tap was systematically varied among seven force levels (1, 1.5, 1.75, 2, 2.25, 2.5 or 3 N). In the externally generated touch condition (Fig. 1a), the participants kept both of their hands relaxed while receiving the test tap and comparison taps on their left index finger. In the self-generated touch condition (Fig. 1b), the participants actively tapped a force sensor with their right index finger and triggered the test tap on their left index finger. Then, they remained relaxed while receiving the comparison tap. Each condition consisted of 70 trials, resulting in 140 trials per participant. The order of the intensities was randomized across participants. The order of the conditions was counterbalanced across participants. For detailed description, see Supplementary Material.
Psychophysical fits
In each condition, the participants’ responses were fitted with a generalized linear model using a logit link function (Eq. 1)
Two parameters of interest were extracted. The point of subjective equality \(\left( {\rm{PSE} = - \frac{{\beta 0}}{{\beta 1}}} \right)\) represents the intensity at which the test tap felt as strong as the comparison tap (p = 0.5) and quantifies the participants’ perceived intensity of the test tap. Subsequently, somatosensory attenuation is calculated as the difference between the PSEs of the two conditions (PSEexternal – PSEself)15,18,24,25,70,71. The just noticeable difference parameter \(\left( {\rm{JND} = \frac{{{{{\mathrm{log}}}}(3)}}{{\beta 1}}} \right)\) reflects the participants’ sensitivity in the psychophysics task and quantifies their somatosensory precision in each condition. The PSE and JND are independent qualities of sensory judgments.
Schizotypal traits
After the psychophysical task, participants completed the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ)44, a 74-item self-report schizotypy assessment instrument with excellent internal reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.91) and test-retest reliability (0.82)44. It was developed on the basis of the nine features of schizotypal personality disorder, as defined by the DSM-III-R criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1987)44. We used the three-factor model to partition the dimensions of the construct of schizotypy60,61,63,72,73,74,75, and we calculated the total score for the cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal and disorganized factors that reflect the positive, negative, and disorganized dimensions of schizotypy, respectively. There has been discussion as to whether schizotypy constitutes a continuous or a categorical construct46,68,76,77,78. In line with the predominant conceptualization of schizotypy as a continuous variable within the general population46,47,50,79, our main analysis comprised treating positive schizotypal traits as a continuous variable across the entire sample. Nonetheless, to attain methodological rigor and to account for both notions, we performed a secondary analysis treating schizotypy as a categorical variable.
Statistical analysis
Data were analyzed using R80 and JASP81. Data normality was assessed using the Shapiro–Wilk test, and planned comparisons were made using parametric (independent or paired t-test) or nonparametric (Mann–Whitney or Wilcoxon) statistical tests. For each test, 95% confidence intervals (CI95) are reported. Depending on the data normality, effect sizes are given by Cohen’s d or by the matched rank biserial correlation rrb. For the ANOVAs, effect sizes are given by the partial eta-squared (ηp2). Spearman correlation coefficients were used as the data were not normally distributed. Model comparison was performed using the Akaike information criterion. A Bayesian factor analysis was carried out for all statistical comparisons of our categorical analyses (default Cauchy priors with a scale of 0.707) and correlations (Kendall’s tau-b) to provide information about the level of support for the null hypothesis compared to the alternative hypothesis (BF01) given the data. All statistical tests were two-tailed.
Results
Somatosensory attenuation and precision across the entire sample
The PSE was significantly lower in the self-generated touch condition than in the externally generated touch condition across the entire sample: n = 100, V = 625, p < 0.001, CI95 = [−0.185, −0.105], rrb = −0.747, BF01 < 0.001 (Fig. 1c, d). This indicates that self-generated tactile stimuli felt weaker than externally generated stimuli of identical intensity, replicating previous findings15,18,24,25,70,71. When removing the extreme PSE value of one participant (Fig. 1c), the same results were obtained: n = 99, V = 625, p < 0.001, CI95 = [−0.180, −0.105], rrb = −0.742, BF01 < 0.001. Attenuation was observed in 80% of participants (Fig. 1e).
JNDs did not significantly differ between the two conditions: n = 100, V = 2592, p = 0.335, CI95 = [−0.01, 0.03], rrb = 0.113 (Fig. 1f, g). This was strongly supported by a Bayesian analysis (BF01 = 5.417) indicating that self-generated and externally generated taps were perceived with similar sensory precision, in line with previous studies24,25. When removing the extreme JND value of one participant (Fig. 1f), the same results were obtained: n = 99, V = 2592, p = 0.247, CI95 = [−0.005, 0.03], rrb = 0.137, BF01 = 3.480. As seen in Fig. 1h, approximately half of the participants increased and half decreased their JNDs between the conditions (44% increased, 52% decreased, 4% remained unchanged).
No significant correlation was observed between the PSEs and JNDs in either the self-generated touch condition (n = 100, rho = 0.079, p = 0.437) or in the externally generated touch condition (n = 100, rho = 0.046, p = 0.647), and this was strongly confirmed by a Bayesian analysis (BF01 = 5.452 for the self-generated touch condition, and BF01 = 6.560 for the externally generated touch condition). This corroborates the notion that sensory magnitude (PSE) and precision (JND) are independent measures, and is in line with previous findings25. No order effects were detected neither in the PSEs nor in the JNDs. Supplementary Material shows all individual fits.
Schizotypal traits and somatosensory attenuation
Figure 2a–d shows the distribution of the total SPQ scores (μ = 20.87, σ = 12.165, range = 0–53, Cronbach’s alpha = 0.821), as well as those of the cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal, and disorganized factors in our sample. Our schizotypy distributions were very similar to those of previous studies using random sampling methods, both in terms of mean and variability (e.g.,68,82). The sample had comparable levels of positive, negative, and disorganized schizotypy. For details, see Supplementary Material.
Confirming our first hypothesis, we observed a negative correlation between somatosensory attenuation and schizotypal traits (n = 100, rho = −0.215, p = 0.031, BF01 = 0.865) (Fig. 2e), which was driven by the scores of the cognitive-perceptual factor (i.e., the positive dimension of schizotypy) (Fig. 2f): n = 100, rho = −0.259, p = 0.009, BF01 = 0.243. This means that the higher the positive schizotypal traits of the participants, the lower their somatosensory attenuation. The individual PSEs did not significantly correlate with positive schizotypy (self-generated touch condition: n = 100, rho = −0.097, p = 0.335; externally generated touch condition: n = 100, rho = −0.180, p = 0.074). The absence of these significant correlations was supported by a Bayesian analysis (BF01 = 4.794 for the self-generated touch condition and BF01 = 1.502 for the externally generated touch condition), indicating that positive schizotypal traits are associated with the perceived difference between the intensities of a self-generated and an externally generated touch (i.e., somatosensory attenuation). Critically, the relationship between attenuation and schizotypy was found only for positive schizotypy and not for the negative (i.e., interpersonal factor) (n = 100, rho = −0.179, p = 0.074) (Fig. 2g) or the disorganized dimension (i.e., disorganized factor) (n = 100, rho = −0.106, p = 0.294) (Fig. 2h), and a Bayesian analysis further supported the absence of these relationships (BF01 = 1.552 for the negative and BF01 = 4.337 for the disorganized dimension).
To test the predictive power of each schizotypy dimension on somatosensory attenuation, we built three different linear models with the positive, negative, and disorganized schizotypy as independent predictors of somatosensory attenuation (difference in the PSEs), respectively. In all three models, three participants (out of 100) were considered outlier values based on a normal Q-Q plot and were removed. Residual errors were normally distributed. When comparing the three models, the Akaike information criterion favored the one with positive schizotypy (AIC = −55.548), followed by the one with disorganized schizotypy (AIC = −49.981) and then the one with negative schizotypy (AIC = −49.961). To further test whether positive schizotypy was a better predictor of somatosensory attenuation, over-and-above the other two schizotypy dimensions, we built a model with all three schizotypal dimensions included as simultaneous predictors. All three regressors had low variance inflation factors (<1.86) and the residual errors were normally distributed. Positive schizotypy was a significant negative regressor on somatosensory attenuation (n = 97, t = −2.292, p = 0.024) but neither the negative (n = 97, t = 0.547, p = 0.586), nor the disorganized dimensions of schizotypy (n = 97, t = −0.372, p = 0.711) were significant predictors of attenuation. These results suggest that the predictive power of positive schizotypy is higher than that of negative and disorganized schizotypy in accounting for the attenuation effect and demonstrates the specificity of the positive subscale.
Schizotypal traits and somatosensory precision
Confirming our second hypothesis, we observed a positive correlation between the JND of self-generated touch and positive schizotypal traits (n = 100, rho = 0.339, p < 0.001, BF01 = 0.018) (Fig. 2j), which effectively is a negative correlation between the somatosensory precision of self-generated touch and positive schizotypal traits. In other words, the higher the positive schizotypal traits of the participants, the lower their somatosensory precision of self-generated touch. In contrast, somatosensory precision for externally generated touch did not correlate with positive schizotypy (n = 100, rho = 0.114, p = 0.257, BF01 = 3.639), suggesting that positive schizotypy does not generically influence the precision with which touch is perceived but only that of self-generated touch. Finally, the somatosensory precision of self-generated touch significantly correlated only with positive schizotypy but not with the full SPQ (n = 100, rho = 0.167, p = 0.096) (Fig. 2i), or the negative (n = 100, rho = 0.039, p = 0.699) (Fig. 2k) or disorganized dimension (n = 100, rho = 0.092, p = 0.364) (Fig. 2l). As above, the Bayesian analyses strongly supported the absence of these relationships (BF01 = 6.600 for the negative dimension and BF01 = 4.682 for the disorganized dimension).
As with somatosensory attenuation, to test the predictive power of each schizotypy dimension on the precision of self-generated touch, we built three different linear models with the positive, negative, and disorganized schizotypy as independent predictors of somatosensory precision (JNDself), respectively. In all three models, three participants (out of 100) were considered outlier values based on a normal Q-Q plot and were removed. Residual errors were normally distributed for the positive and the negative schizotypy. They were not normally distributed for the disorganized dimension but followed a bell-shaped curve. When comparing the three models, the Akaike information criterion favored the one with positive schizotypy (AIC = −218.68), followed by the one with negative schizotypy (AIC = −207.732) and then the one with disorganized schizotypy (AIC = −207.322). To further test whether positive schizotypy was a better predictor of the precision of self-generated touch, over-and-above the other two schizotypy dimensions, we built a model with all three schizotypal dimensions included as simultaneous predictors. All three regressors had low variance inflation factors (<1.69). The residuals were not normally distributed but followed a bell-shaped curve. Similar to somatosensory attenuation, positive schizotypy was a significant regressor on somatosensory precision (n = 97, t = 8.436, p = 0.005) while neither the negative (n = 97, t = 0.353, p = 0.725), nor the disorganized dimensions of schizotypy (n = 97, t = −1.062, p = 0.291) were significant regressors. Together, these results suggest that the predictive power of positive schizotypy is higher than that of negative and disorganized schizotypy in accounting for the lower precision of self-generated touch and demonstrates the specificity of the positive subscale.
Schizotypy as a categorical variable
Finally, we treated positive schizotypal traits as a categorical variable. Given the absence of established cut-off values for the SPQ estimates, we split the sample into 3 subgroups with equal numbers of participants based on their scores in the cognitive-perceptual factor: the low (nlow = 34), medium (nmed = 33), and high (nhigh = 33) positive schizotypy groups (Fig. 3a). This approach was deemed appropriate to discern the differences between the two extremes (i.e., low and high).
For the PSEs, a mixed ANOVA with condition (self-generated versus externally generated) as the within-subjects’ factor, and positive schizotypy group (high versus low) as the between subjects’ factor revealed a significant main effect of condition (F(1,65) = 25.94, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.285), a non-significant effect of schizotypy group (F(1,65) = 0.041, p = 0.840, ηp2 < 0.001), and a significant interaction (F(1,65) = 6.402, p = 0.014, ηp2 = 0.090). The interaction was driven by a significantly higher somatosensory attenuation for the low positive schizotypy group compared to the high positive schizotypy group (Fig. 3b): nlow = 34, nhigh = 33, W = 770, p = 0.009, CI95 = [0.030, 0.230], rrb = 0.373, BF01 = 0.280.
For the JNDs, the mixed ANOVA revealed a non-significant main effect of condition (F(1,65) = 1.890, p = 0.174, ηp2 = 0.028), a significant effect of schizotypy group (F(1,65) = 9.508, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.128), and a significant interaction (F(1,65 = 8.346, p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.114). The interaction term was driven by lower JNDs in the self-generated touch condition for the low positive schizotypy group compared to the high positive schizotypy group (nlow = 34, nhigh = 33, t(48.7) = −3.626, p < 0.001, CI95 = [−0.133, −0.038], Cohen’s d = −0.89, BF01 = 0.018) (Fig. 3c). In contrast, JNDs in the externally generated touch condition did not significantly differ between the two groups (nlow = 34, nhigh = 33, W = 477.5, p = 0.296, CI95 = [−0.050, 0.020], rrb = −0.149, BF01 = 2.46).
Figure 3d–f illustrates these effects for the entire sample (Fig. 3d), the low (Fig. 3e) and the high positive schizotypy subgroups (Fig. 3f). In the entire sample, the psychometric curve shifted to the left for the self-generated touch condition compared to the externally generated touch condition without any changes in the slope; thus, self-generated touch felt weaker than external touch, but they were perceived with similar precision (Fig. 3d). Critically, as seen in Fig. 3e and f, the high positive schizotypy group showed less of a shift between the PSEs in the self-generated and externally generated touch conditions (less attenuation) and a flatter curve in the self-generated touch condition (higher JND) compared to the low schizotypy group.
Discussion
The present study has two main findings. First, individuals with higher positive schizotypal traits exhibited less attenuation of their self-generated touch than individuals with low positive schizotypal traits. This result strongly mirrors previous clinical findings of reduced somatosensory attenuation in patients with schizophrenia30,31,32. This is also in line with earlier observations that nonclinical individuals with high schizotypy subjectively rate self-generated touch as more ticklish78 and intense68 than those with low schizotypy. Second, our experimental task (i.e., the force-discrimination task) enabled the measurement not only of the perceived magnitude but also of somatosensory precision and consequently, the assessment of its relationship with schizotypy. Individuals with higher positive schizotypal traits perceived self-generated touch with less sensory precision than individuals with lower positive schizotypal traits, without any effect on the precision of externally generated touch. This result indicates for the first time that high positive schizotypal traits are not accompanied by generic deficits in processing afferent somatosensory information but only self-generated somatosensory feedback and enforces the view that self-disorders lie at the core of the schizophrenia spectrum38,39,41,83,84. Critically, both in terms of attenuation and precision of self-generated touch, it was the positive dimension of schizotypy that drove the effects and not the negative or disorganized dimensions. This parallels the association previously observed between somatosensory attenuation and the severity of hallucinations31, as well as the delusional ideation85,86 and passivity experiences78 of nonclinical individuals.
Deficits in somatosensory attenuation and precision can fall within the scope of subtle neurological aberrations in sensorimotor performance87,88, that are present with variable severity across the psychosis continuum89,90,91,92,93. Neurological soft signs have been repeatedly associated with the negative symptoms of schizophrenia and negative schizotypy in non-clinical individuals94,95,96,97,98,99,100,101,102,103,104, and less robustly with the positive and disorganized dimensions89,105,106,107. Instead, our data revealed a relationship of somatosensory attenuation and precision only with positive schizotypy, and not with the negative and the disorganized dimensions. Consequently, our findings suggest that somatosensory attenuation and precision constitute a special category of neurological soft signs that is specifically related to the self and the positive dimension of psychotic and psychotic-like symptoms.
Our results provide important insights for understanding the mechanism underlying the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. From a computational perspective, our effects can be explained by a deficit in the internal forward model that predicts the somatosensory consequences of the movement. Earlier studies have shown that somatosensory attenuation relies on spatiotemporal motor predictions18,19,71 and not on postdictive processes25,70, and it requires conditions where the received touch can be predicted by the motor command15,17,18,20,21,22,24,70. In our study, reduced attenuation indicates that with the same motor command, the brain of an individual with high positive schizotypy does not accurately predict the sensory consequences of the voluntary movement, leading to less attenuation of the self-generated somatosensory feedback compared to an individual with low positive schizotypy. The combination of this inaccurately predicted somatosensory information with the actual somatosensory feedback further leads to the decreased precision of self-generated touch. Within a Bayesian framework where prediction corresponds to prior expectations and sensory feedback to sensory evidence35, our study indicates that high positive schizotypy is related to atypical prior expectations (generated by the internal forward model) and atypical combinations of prior knowledge with sensory evidence.
The cerebellum has been repeatedly implicated in predicting the sensory consequences of one’s own actions11,70,108,109,110,111,112, and we previously showed that participants with stronger functional connectivity between the cerebellum and the primary and secondary somatosensory cortices during self-generated touch compared to externally generated touch, show greater somatosensory attenuation23. Schizophrenia is also strongly associated with alterations in structural and functional cerebellar connectivity113,114. Patients show impairments in cerebellar-mediated motor tasks115, deficits in the integrity of the cerebellar white matter tracts116,117, and altered cerebellar connectivity118,119,120,121,122,123,124 and activation125 compared to healthy controls. Individuals at ultra-high-risk for psychosis have decreased resting-state cerebellocortical connectivity compared to controls126, and their functional and structural cerebellocortical connectivity relates to their positive symptom progression127. Based on our findings, we speculate that positive schizotypy and consequently the positive symptoms of schizophrenia are related to altered corticocerebellar connectivity.
Future efforts should exploit the perception of self-generated somatosensation as a potential indicator of psychosis proneness. In contrast to other markers, including prepulse inhibition, mismatch negativity and the P300128, which reflect deficits in processing externally generated information in schizophrenia, our results emphasize deficits in processing self-generated information. Furthermore, given that the positive symptoms in the prodromal phase are highly predictive of the transition from a high-risk state to schizophrenia129,130, self-generated somatosensation could function as a sign of neurocognitive impairment that, when combined with other genetic, biochemical and neuroimaging markers131,132, forms a multilayered ‘signature’ for schizophrenia liability. This could enable early detection of psychosis proneness using objective measures that are not susceptible to self-report bias like scale-based measures. So far, this perspective is still at a premature stage and the implementation in clinical settings is far from complete. Undoubtedly, appropriate clinical contextualization and validation through future longitudinal studies are necessary. Nonetheless, the present study suggests that deficits in processing self-generated touch can indicate increased liability for schizophrenia.
References
Crapse, T. B. & Sommer, M. A. Corollary discharge across the animal kingdom. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 587–600 (2008).
Brooks, J. X. & Cullen, K. E. Predictive sensing: the role of motor signals in sensory processing. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 4, 842–850 (2019).
McNamee, D. & Wolpert, D. M. Internal models in biological control. Annu. Rev. Control. Robot. Auton. Syst. 2, 339–364 (2019).
Blakemore, S. J. & Frith, C. Self-awareness and action. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 13, 219–224 (2003).
Frith, C. The neural basis of hallucinations and delusions. C. R. Biol. 328, 169–175 (2005).
Leptourgos, P. & Corlett, P. R. Embodied predictions, agency, and psychosis. Front. Big Data 3, 1–13 (2020).
Wolpert, D. M. & Flanagan, J. R. Motor prediction. Curr. Biol. 11, R729–R732 (2001).
Blakemore, S.-J., Wolpert, D. & Frith, C. Why canʼt you tickle yourself? Neuroreport 11, R11–R16 (2000).
Kawato, M. Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 9, 718–727 (1999).
Davidson, P. R. & Wolpert, D. M. Widespread access to predictive models in the motor system: a short review. J. Neural Eng. 2, S313–S319 (2005).
Shadmehr, R., Krakauer, J. W., Neuroanatomy, A. C. & Motor, F. O. R. A computational neuroanatomy for motor control. Exp. Brain Res. 185, 359–381 (2008).
Frith, C. Explaining delusions of control: the comparator model 20years on. Conscious. Cogn. 21, 52–54 (2012).
Bäß, P. et al. Suppression of the auditory N1 event-related potential component with unpredictable self-initiated tones: evidence for internal forward models with dynamic stimulation. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 70, 137–143 (2008).
Gentsch, A. & Schütz-Bosbach, S. I did it: unconscious expectation of sensory consequences modulates the experience of self-agency and its functional signature. J. Cogn. Neurosci 23, 3817–3828 (2011).
Kilteni, K., Engeler, P. & Ehrsson, H. H. Efference copy is necessary for the attenuation of self-generated touch. iScience 23, 100843 (2020).
Blakemore, S. J., Frith, C. D. & Wolpert, D. M. Spatio-temporal prediction modulates the perception of self-produced stimuli. J. Cogn. Neurosci 11, 551–559 (1999).
Shergill, S. S., Bays, P. M., Frith, C. D. & Wolpert, D. M. Two eyes for an eye: the neuroscience of force escalation. Science 301, 187 (2003).
Bays, P. M., Wolpert, D. M. & Flanagan, J. R. Perception of the consequences of self-action is temporally tuned and event driven. Curr. Biol. 15, 1125–1128 (2005).
Bays, P. M. & Wolpert, D. M. In Sensorimotor Foundations of Higher Cognition (eds. Haggard, E. P., Rosetti, Y. & Kawato, M.) 339–358 (Oxford University Press, 2008).
Kilteni, K. & Ehrsson, H. H. Body ownership determines the attenuation of self-generated tactile sensations. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114, 8426–8431 (2017).
Kilteni, K. & Ehrsson, H. H. Sensorimotor predictions and tool use: hand-held tools attenuate self-touch. Cognition 165, 1–9 (2017).
Kilteni, K., Andersson, B. J., Houborg, C. & Ehrsson, H. H. Motor imagery involves predicting the sensory consequences of the imagined movement. Nat. Commun. 9, 1617 (2018).
Kilteni, K. & Ehrsson, H. H. Functional connectivity between the cerebellum and somatosensory areas implements the attenuation of self-generated touch. J. Neurosci. 40, 894–906 (2020).
Kilteni, K., Engeler, P., Boberg, I., Maurex, L. & Ehrsson, H. H. No evidence for somatosensory attenuation during action observation of self-touch. Eur. J. Neurosci. 54, 6422–6444 (2021).
Kilteni, K. & Ehrsson, H. H. Predictive attenuation of touch and tactile gating are distinct perceptual phenomena. Iscience. 25, 104077 (2022).
Lalouni, M. et al. Predicting pain: differential pain thresholds during self-induced, externally induced, and imagined self-induced pressure pain. Pain https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002151 (2020).
Blakemore, S.-J., Wolpert, D. M. & Frith, C. D. Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 635–640 (1998).
Weiskrantz, L., Elliot, J. & Darlington, C. Preliminary observations of tickling oneself. Nature 230, 598–599 (1971).
Leavens, D. A. & Bard, K. A. Tickling. Curr. Biol. 26, R91–R93 (2016).
Shergill, S. S., Samson, G., Bays, P. M., Frith, C. D. & Wolpert, D. M. Evidence for sensory prediction deficits in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 162, 2384–2386 (2005).
Shergill, S. S. et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of impaired sensory prediction in schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 28 (2014).
Blakemore, S.-J., Smith, J., Steel, R., Johnstone, E. C. & Frith, C. D. The perception of self-produced sensory stimuli in patients with auditory hallucinations and passivity experiences: evidence for a breakdown in self-monitoring. Psychol. Med. 30, 1131–1139 (2000).
Frith, C. The self in action: lessons from delusions of control. Conscious. Cogn. 14, 752–770 (2005).
Frith, C. D. Can a problem with corollary discharge explain the symptoms of schizophrenia? Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 4, 768–769 (2019).
Fletcher, P. C. & Frith, C. D. Perceiving is believing: a Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 48–58 (2009).
Poletti, M., Tortorella, A. & Raballo, A. Impaired corollary discharge in psychosis and at-risk states: integrating neurodevelopmental, phenomenological, and clinical perspectives. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 4, 832–841 (2019).
Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S. J. & Wolpert, D. M. Explaining the symptoms of schizophrenia: abnormalities in the awareness of action. Brain Res. Rev 31, 357–363 (2000).
Sass, L. A. & Parnas, J. Schizophrenia, consciousness, and the self. Schizophr Bull. 29, 427–444 (2003).
Postmes, L. et al. Schizophrenia as a self-disorder due to perceptual incoherence. Schizophr. Res. 152, 41–50 (2014).
Park, S. & Baxter, T. Schizophrenia in the flesh: revisiting schizophrenia as a disorder of the bodily self. Schizophr. Res. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2021.12.031 (2022).
Henriksen, M. G., Raballo, A. & Nordgaard, J. Self-disorders and psychopathology: a systematic review. Lancet Psychiatry 8, 1001–1012 (2021).
Fervaha, G. & Remington, G. Neuroimaging findings in schizotypal personality disorder: a systematic review. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 43, 96–107 (2013).
Meehl, P. E. & Prologue, M. Toward an integrated theory of schizotaxia, schizotypy, and schizophrenia. J. Pers. Disord. 4, 1–99 (1990).
Raine, A. The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R Criteria. Schizophr. Bull. 17, 555–564 (1991).
Thomas, E. H. X. et al. Do schizotypy dimensions reflect the symptoms of schizophrenia? Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 53, 236–247 (2019).
Kwapil, T. R. & Barrantes-Vidal, N. Schizotypy: looking back and moving forward. Schizophr. Bull. 41, S366–373 (2015).
Van Os, J., Hanssen, M., Bijl, R. V. & Ravelli, A. Strauss (1969) revisited: a psychosis continuum in the general population? Schizophr. Res. 45, 11–20 (2000).
Barrantes-Vidal, N., Grant, P. & Kwapil, T. R. The role of schizotypy in the study of the etiology of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophr. Bull. 41, S408–S416 (2015).
Racioppi, A. et al. Positive and negative schizotypy prediction of prodromal symptoms and schizophrenia-spectrum personality disorder traits: a 3-year prospective study. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 265, S116 (2015).
Nelson, M. T., Seal, M. L., Pantelis, C. & Phillips, L. J. Evidence of a dimensional relationship between schizotypy and schizophrenia: a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 317–327 (2013).
Andreasen, N. C. A unitary model of schizophrenia bleuler’s ‘fragmented phrene’ as schizencephaly. Arch Gen Psychiatry 56, 781–793 (1999).
Miller, P. et al. Schizotypal components in people at high risk of developing schizophrenia: early findings from the Edinburgh High-Risk Study. Br J Psychiatry 180, 179–184 (2002).
Lenzenweger, M. F. Schizotypy an organizing framework for schizophrenia research. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 162–166 (2006).
Keshavan, M. S. In Neurodevelopment and Adult Psychopathology (eds Keshavan M. S. & Murray R. M.) (Cambridge University Press, 1997).
Meehl, P. E. Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia. Am. Psychol. 17, 827–838 (1962).
Weinberger, D. R. Implications of normal brain development for the pathogenesis of schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 44, 660–669 (1987).
Ettinger, U., Meyhöfer, I., Steffens, M., Wagner, M. & Koutsouleris, N. Genetics, cognition, and neurobiology of schizotypal personality: a review of the overlap with schizophrenia. Front. Psychiatry 5, 1–16 (2014).
Rado. Psychoanalysis of Behavior. (Grune & Stratton, 1956).
Debbané, M. et al. Developing psychosis and its risk states through the lens of schizotypy. Schizophr. Bull. 41, S396–S407 (2015).
Fonseca-Pedrero, E. et al. The structure of schizotypal personality traits: a cross-national study. Psychol. Med. 48, 451–462 (2018).
Reynolds, C. A., Raine, A., Mellingen, K., V’enables, P. H. & Mednick, S. A. Three^factor model of schizotypal personality: invariance across culture, gender, religious affiliation, family adversity, and psychopathology. Schizophr. Bull. 26, 603–618 (2000).
Wuthrich, V. M. & Bates, T. C. Confirmatory factor analysis of the three-factor structure of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire and Chapman schizotypy scales. J. Pers. Assess. 87, 292–304 (2006).
Raine, A. et al. Cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal, and disorganized features of schizotypal personality. Schizophr. Bull. 20, 191–201 (1994).
Rossi, A. & Daneluzzo, E. Schizotypal dimensions in normals and schizophrenic patients: a comparison with other clinical samples. Schizophr. Res. 54, 67–75 (2002).
Liddle, P. F. The symptoms of chronic schizophrenia a re-examination of the positive-negative dichotomy. Br. J. Psychiatry 151, 151145 (1987).
Stuart, G. W., Pantelis, C., Klimidis, S. & Minas, I. H. The three-syndrome model of schizophrenia: meta-analysis of an artefact. Schizophr. Res. 39, 233–242 (1999).
Lenzenweger, M. F. Two-point discrimination thresholds and schizotypy: illuminating a somatosensory dysfunction. Schizophr. Res. 42, 111–124 (2000).
Whitford, T. J., Mitchell, A. M. & Mannion, D. J. The ability to tickle oneself is associated with level of psychometric schizotypy in non-clinical individuals. Conscious. Cogn. 52, 93–103 (2017).
Oldfield, R. C. R. C. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113 (1971).
Bays, P. M., Flanagan, J. R. & Wolpert, D. M. Attenuation of self-generated tactile sensations is predictive, not postdictive. PLoS Biol. 4, 281–284 (2006).
Kilteni, K., Houborg, C. & Ehrsson, H. H. Rapid learning and unlearning of predicted sensory delays in self-generated touch. Elife 8, 1–17 (2019).
Tsaousis, I., Zouraraki, C., Karamaouna, P., Karagiannopoulou, L. & Giakoumaki, S. G. The validity of the schizotypal personality questionnaire in a Greek sample: tests of measurement invariance and latent mean differences. Compr. Psychiatry 62, 51–62 (2015).
Rabella, M. et al. Validation of a Spanish version of the schizotypal personality questionnaire (SPQ): psychometric characteristics and underlying factor structure derived from a healthy university student sample. Actas Esp. Psiquiatr. 46, 159–173 (2018).
Fonseca-Pedrero, E. et al. Cross-cultural invariance of the factor structure of the schizotypal personality questionnaire across Spanish and American college students. Psychiatry Res. 220, 1071–1076 (2014).
Fonseca-Pedrero, E. et al. Schizotypal personality questionnaire: new sources of validity evidence in college students. Psychiatry Res 219, 214–220 (2014).
Mason, O. J. The duality of schizotypy: is it both dimensional and categorical? Front. Psychiatry 5, 5–8 (2014).
Lenzenweger, M. F. Thinking clearly about schizotypy: Hewing to the schizophrenia liability core, considering interesting tangents, and avoiding conceptual quicksand. Schizophr. Bull. 41, S483–S491 (2015).
Lemaitre, A.-L., Luyat, M. & Lafargue, G. Individuals with pronounced schizotypal traits are particularly successful in tickling themselves. Conscious. Cogn. 41, 64–71 (2016).
Claridge, G. Single indicator of risk for schizophrenia: probable fact or likely myth? Schizophr. Bull. 20, 151–168 (1994).
Team, R. C. R.: A language and environment for statistical computing, version 3.3. 1. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2016 (2019).
JASP & JASP Team. JASP. [Computer software] (2019).
Fonseca-Pedrero, E. et al. Comparisons of schizotypal traits across 12 countries: results from the International Consortium for Schizotypy Research. Schizophr. Res. 199, 128–134 (2018).
Borda, J. P. & Sass, L. A. Phenomenology and neurobiology of self disorder in schizophrenia: primary factors. Schizophr. Res. 169, 464–473 (2015).
Northoff, G., Sandsten, K. E., Nordgaard, J., Kjaer, T. W. & Parnas, J. The self and its prolonged intrinsic neural timescale in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 47, 170–179 (2021).
Teufel, C., Kingdon, A., Ingram, J. N., Wolpert, D. M. & Fletcher, P. C. Deficits in sensory prediction are related to delusional ideation in healthy individuals. Neuropsychologia 48, 4169–4172 (2010).
Palmer, C. E., Davare, M. & Kilner, J. M. Physiological and perceptual sensory attenuation have different underlying neurophysiological correlates. J. Neurosci. 36, 10803–10812 (2016).
Buchanan, R. W. & Heinrichs, D. W. The neurological evaluation scale (NES): a structured instrument for the assessment of neurological signs in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 27, 335–350 (1989).
Schröder, J. et al. Neurological soft signs in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 6, 25–30 (1991).
Mechri, A. et al. Neurological soft signs and schizotypal dimensions in unaffected siblings of patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 175, 22–26 (2010).
Janssen, J. et al. Brain morphology and neurological soft signs in adolescents with first-episode psychosis. Br. J. Psychiatry 195, 227–233 (2009).
Gaha, S. B., Dhouib, S. H., Amado, I. & Bouden, A. Signes neurologiques mineurs dans la schizophrénie précoce. Encephale. 41, 209–214 (2015).
Herold, C. J., Duval, C. Z. & Schröder, J. Neurological soft signs and cognition in the late course of chronic schizophrenia: a longitudinal study. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 271, 1465–1473 (2021).
Chan, R. C. K. et al. Neurological soft signs precede the onset of schizophrenia: a study of individuals with schizotypy, ultra-high-risk individuals, and first-onset schizophrenia. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 268, 49–56 (2018).
Kaczorowski, J. A., Barrantes-Vidal, N. & Kwapil, T. R. Neurological soft signs in psychometrically identified schizotypy. Schizophr. Res. 115, 293–302 (2009).
Hembram, M., Simlai, J., Chaudhury, S. & Biswas, P. First rank symptoms and neurological soft signs in schizophrenia. Psychiatry J 2014, 1–11 (2014).
Cvetić, T. et al. Comparative analysis of soft neurological signs in positive and negative subtype of schizophrenia. Psychiatr. Danub. 21, 174–178 (2009).
Prikryl, R. et al. Dynamics of neurological soft signs and its relationship to clinical course in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 200, 67–72 (2012).
Varambally, S., Venkatasubramanian, G., Thirthalli, J., Janakiramaiah, N. & Gangadhar, B. N. Cerebellar and other neurological soft signs in antipsychotic-naïve schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 114, 352–356 (2006).
Theleritis, C. et al. Neurological soft signs and psychometrically identified schizotypy in a sample of young conscripts. Psychiatry Res. 198, 241–247 (2012).
Bombin, I., Arango, C. & Buchanan, R. W. Significance and meaning of neurological signs in schizophrenia: two decades later. Schizophr. Bull. 31, 962–977 (2005).
Yazici, A. H., Demir, B., Yazici, K. M. & Gö̌üş, A. Neurological soft signs in schizophrenic patients and their nonpsychotic siblings. Schizophr. Res. 58, 241–246 (2002).
Tosaro, S. & Dazzan, P. The psychopathology of schizophrenia and the presence of neurological soft signs: a review. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry. 18, 285–288 (2005).
Whitty, P. et al. Diagnostic specificity and predictors of neurological soft signs in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and other psychoses over the first 4 years of illness. Schizophr. Res. 86, 110–117 (2006).
Chan, R. C. K. et al. Course of neurological soft signs in first-episode schizophrenia: relationship with negative symptoms and cognitive performances. Sci. Rep. 5, 1–11 (2015).
de Leede-Smith, S. et al. Neurological soft signs: effects of trait schizotypy, psychological distress and auditory hallucination predisposition. Schizophr. Res. Cogn. 7, 1–7 (2017).
Barkus, E., Stirling, J., Hopkins, R. & Lewis, S. The presence of neurological soft signs along the psychosis proneness continuum. Schizophr. Bull. 32, 573–577 (2006).
Ojagbemi, A., Esan, O., Emsley, R. & Gureje, O. Motor sequencing abnormalities are the trait marking neurological soft signs of schizophrenia. Neurosci. Lett. 600, 226 (2015).
Blakemore, S.-J. J., Frith, C. D. & Wolpert, D. M. The cerebellum is involved in predicting the sensory consequences of action. Neuroreport 12, 1879–1884 (2001).
Blakemore, S. J., Wolpert, D. M. & Frith, C. D. The cerebellum contributes to somatosensory cortical activity during self-produced tactile stimulation. Neuroimage 10, 448–459 (1999).
Shadmehr, R., Smith, M. A. & Krakauer, J. W. Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 33, 89–108 (2010).
Wolpert, D. M. et al. Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2, 338–347 (1998).
Mizusawa, H. & Kakei, S. In Cerebellum as a CNS Hub 371–390 (Springer, 2021).
Kim, S. E., Jung, S., Sung, G., Bang, M. & Lee, S. H. Impaired cerebro-cerebellar white matter connectivity and its associations with cognitive function in patients with schizophrenia. npj Schizophr. 7, 1–7 (2021).
Moberget, T. & Ivry, R. B. Prediction, psychosis, and the cerebellum. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 4, 820–831 (2019).
Bernard, J. A. & Mittal, V. A. Cerebellar motor dysfunction in schizophrenia and psychosis risk: the importance of regional cerebellar analysis approaches. Front. Psychiatry 5, 1–14 (2014).
Kanaan, R. A. A. et al. Microstructural organization of cerebellar tracts in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 66, 1067–1069 (2009).
Kyriakopoulos, M. & Frangou, S. Recent diffusion tensor imaging findings in early stages of schizophrenia. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 22, 168–176 (2009).
Anteraper, S. A. et al. Abnormal function in dentate nuclei precedes the onset of psychosis: a resting-state fMRI study in high-risk individuals. Schizophr. Bull. 47, 1421–1430 (2021).
Shinn, A. K., Baker, J. T., Lewandowski, K. E., Öngür, D. & Cohen, B. M. Aberrant cerebellar connectivity in motor and association networks in schizophrenia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9, 1–16 (2015).
Repovs, G., Csernansky, J. G. & Barch, D. M. Brain network connectivity in individuals with schizophrenia and their siblings. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 967–973 (2011).
Collin, G. et al. Impaired cerebellar functional connectivity in schizophrenia patients and their healthy siblings. Front. Psychiatry 2, 1–12 (2011).
Kim, D. J. et al. Cerebellar–cortical dysconnectivity in resting-state associated with sensorimotor tasks in schizophrenia. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41, 3119–3132 (2020).
Walther, S. et al. Aberrant hyperconnectivity in the motor system at rest is linked to motor abnormalities in schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophr. Bull 43, 982–992 (2017).
Moussa-Tooks, A. B. et al. Impaired effective connectivity during a cerebellar-mediated sensorimotor synchronization task in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 45, 531–541 (2019).
Bernard, J. A. & Mittal, V. A. Dysfunctional activation of the cerebellum in schizophrenia: a functional neuroimaging meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 3, 545–566 (2015).
Bernard, J. A. et al. Cerebellar networks in individuals at ultra high-risk of psychosis: Impact on postural sway and symptom severity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 4064–4078 (2014).
Bernard, J. A., Orr, J. M. & Mittal, V. A. Cerebello-thalamo-cortical networks predict positive symptom progression in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis. NeuroImage Clin 14, 622–628 (2017).
Donati, F. L., D’Agostino, A. & Ferrarelli, F. Neurocognitive and neurophysiological endophenotypes in schizophrenia: an overview. Biomarkers Neuropsychiatry 3, 100017 (2020).
Klosterkötter. Prediction and prevention of schizophrenia: What has been achieved and where to go next? World Psychiatry 11, 62 (2012).
Meisenzahl, E., Walger, P., Schmidt, S. J., Koutsouleris, N. & Schultze-Lutter, F. Early recognition and prevention of schizophrenia and other psychoses. Nervenarzt 91, 10–17 (2020).
Kim, H. K., Blumberger, D. M. & Daskalakis, Z. J. Neurophysiological Biomarkers in Schizophrenia—P50, Mismatch Negativity, and TMS-EMG and TMS-EEG. Front. Psychiatry 11, 795 (2020).
Kraguljac, N. V. et al. Neuroimaging biomarkers in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 178, 509–521 (2021).
Acknowledgements
E.A. was supported by the Åke Wibergs Foundation (M20-0038) and X.J. and K.K. by the Swedish Research Council (VR Starting Grant 2019-01909 granted to K.K.). Experimental costs were covered by the Swedish Research Council.
Funding
Open access funding provided by Karolinska Institute.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
K.K., X.J., and E.A. conceived and designed the experiment. X.J. and E.A. collected the data. K.K., E.A., and X.J. conducted the statistical analysis. E.A., K.K., and X.J. wrote the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Asimakidou, E., Job, X. & Kilteni, K. The positive dimension of schizotypy is associated with a reduced attenuation and precision of self-generated touch. Schizophr 8, 57 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41537-022-00264-6
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41537-022-00264-6
This article is cited by
-
Dynamic changes in somatosensory and cerebellar activity mediate temporal recalibration of self-touch
Communications Biology (2024)
-
Graph-based analysis of EEG for schizotypy classification applying flicker Ganzfeld stimulation
Schizophrenia (2023)