Abstract
Cognitive decline is common in Parkinson’s disease (PD) and its genetic risk factors are not well known to date, besides variants in the GBA and APOE genes. However, variation in complex traits is caused by numerous variants and is usually studied with genome-wide association studies (GWAS), requiring a large sample size, which is difficult to achieve for outcome measures in PD. Taking an alternative approach, we computed 100 polygenic scores (PGS) related to cognitive, dementia, stroke, and brain anatomical phenotypes and investigated their association with cognitive decline in six longitudinal cohorts. The analysis was adjusted for age, sex, genetic ancestry, follow-up duration, GBA and APOE status. Then, we meta-analyzed five of these cohorts, comprising a total of 1702 PD participants with 6156 visits, using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment as a cognitive outcome measure. After correction for multiple comparisons, we found four PGS significantly associated with cognitive decline: intelligence (p = 5.26e–13), cognitive performance (p = 1.46e–12), educational attainment (p = 8.52e–10), and reasoning (p = 3.58e–5). Survival analyses highlighted an offset of several years between the first and last quartiles of PGS, with significant differences for the PGS of cognitive performance (5 years) and educational attainment (7 years). In conclusion, we found four PGS associated with cognitive decline in PD, all associated with general cognitive phenotypes. This study highlights the common genetic factors between cognitive decline in PD and the general population, and the importance of the participant’s cognitive reserve for cognitive outcome in PD.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Although Parkinson’s disease (PD) is clinically defined by its cardinal motor symptoms, numerous non-motor symptoms frequently occur during the course of the disease. Among them, cognitive decline is common, with the point prevalence of PD dementia being approximately 30% and the cumulative prevalence being at least 75% for PD participants surviving more than ten years1. Cognitive decline strongly impacts the quality of life and life expectancy of the participants2,3.
The genetic risk factors of cognitive decline in PD are still mostly unknown. Genetic risk factors for cognitive decline in PD have been investigated in specific genes related to genetic forms of PD or cognitive disorders4,5. Mutations in the glucocerebrosidase (GBA) gene, responsible for the autosomal recessive Gaucher’s disease, have been demonstrated to be a strong risk factor for PD6, but have also been associated with greater cognitive decline in PD7,8,9,10,11. Polymorphisms of the apolipoprotein E (APOE) gene associated with Alzheimer’s disease12 have also been shown to be associated with cognitive decline in PD11,13,14,15,16,17. Investigations in other genes, including microtubule-associated protein tau (MAPT)15,16,17, leucine-rich repeat serine/threonine-protein kinase 2 (LRRK2)18,19,20, α-synuclein (SNCA)16,21, catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT)14,15,17, and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BNDF)22,23, have led to conflicting results. Genome-wide investigation of cognitive decline in PD has been limited. No genome-wide significant association with cognitive decline could be reported in two different GWAS24,25. Other GWAS confirmed the association with mutations in GBA and APOE in PD11,26 and Lewy body dementia (LBD)27, and reported genome-wide significant associations with Apolipoprotein C1 (APOC1), translocase of outer mitochondrial membrane 40 (TOMM40), the regulating synaptic membrane exocytosis 2 (RIMS2) genes, as well as suggestive associations in transmembrane protein 108 (TMEM108) and WW domain-containing oxidoreductase (WWOX) genes in PD, but with limited effect sizes11,26. Another study reported significant associations between PD dementia and variants in the mitochondrial E3 ubiquitin protein ligase 1 (MUL1), zinc fingers and homeoboxes 2 (ZHX2) and endoplasmic reticulum resident protein 29 (ERP29) genes28. Finally, a recent study showed an association between cognitive decline in PD and mitochondrial haplogroups29. All these studies suffered from limited power due to the limited number of PD participants included in the analyses and highlighted the limited effect sizes of individual variants.
Variation in complex phenotypes is caused by numerous genetic variants, each one usually carrying only a small relative risk. However, the combination of the risk of numerous low-risk variants can explain a substantial proportion of the genetic variance. Polygenic scores (PGS) additively combine the weighted risk of every trait-associated genetic variant into a single score. PGS is computed by estimating the joint effects of individual genotypes from the marginal effects obtained from summary statistics of large-scale genome-wide association studies (GWAS).
In this study, we performed a proxy-analysis of the genetics of cognitive decline in PD through PGS. We used clinical and genetic data from six longitudinal cohorts. We computed PGS from publicly available summary statistics for a broad range of phenotypes and investigated their associations with longitudinal cognitive scores. Our objective was to identify the genetic similarity between cognitive decline in PD and other phenotypes.
Results
Participants
In total, 2089 PD participants and 8141 visits were included in our analyses. The details on the inclusion and exclusion criteria of each cohort are provided in supplementary materials. A flowchart describing the number of participants at each step of the quality control is given in Fig. 1. Table 1 presents the characteristics of the participants in each cohort. There were differences across cohorts in terms of age, sex, length of follow-up, interval between visits, baseline MoCA scores, as well as baseline and lifetime cognitive decline that we adjusted for further analyses. The list of GBA mutations is described in Supplementary Table 1.
Genome-wide association studies
We identified 100 GWAS matching the defined criteria. The corresponding phenotypes consisted of height30, body mass index31, memory performance32, reasoning32, reaction time33, cognitive performance34, educational attainment34, intelligence35, Parkinson’s disease or first-degree relation to an individual with Parkinson’s disease36, Alzheimer’s disease37, Alzheimer’s disease or family history of Alzheimer’s disease38, Lewy body dementia39, five stroke subtypes40, major depressive disorder41, anxiety disorder42, sleeplessness or insomnia43, trouble falling asleep43, white matter hyperintensities44, intracranial volume45, subcortical volumes in seven brain regions46, and cortical surface areas and thicknesses in the whole brain and 34 brain regions47. Supplementary Table 2 provides detailed information about the phenotypes, the estimated SNP heritability, the number of participants, and the number of SNPs for each GWAS. Supplementary Table 3 provides the number of SNPs involved in each computed PGS.
Association analyses
Partial correlation coefficients between the real phenotypes of height and body mass index and the corresponding PGS were coherent with the literature (r = 0.60 [0.57–0.63] for height, r = 0.26 [0.21–0.31] for body mass index), suggesting good PGS computation with regards to the current state of the art30,31.
Since DIGPD was the only cohort in which the MoCA was not used as the cognitive screening test, it was not included in the meta-analysis. The meta-analysis including the other five cohorts, for a total of 1702 PD patients with 6156 visits, revealed four significant associations, corresponding to the PGS of intelligence, cognitive performance, educational attainment, and reasoning (Table 2). All the associations were in the same direction as protective factors (the higher the PGS, the higher the cognitive scores, thus the less cognitive decline). The heterogeneity p-values were low for several PGS, suggesting heterogeneity in the results (Supplementary Table 5). Figure 2 illustrates the forest plots for the significant associations, confirming the heterogeneity in the effects with outlying values most found in LCC and Iceberg cohorts. Nonetheless, the directions were always identical in the five cohorts (Table 2).
Several significant associations were also obtained in the independent analyses in each cohort (Table 2 and Supplementary Table 4): the PGS for intelligence, cognitive performance, educational attainment, reasoning, and white matter hyperintensities in PPMI, the PGS for intelligence, cognitive performance and educational attainment in PDBP, the PGS for cognitive performance and intelligence in SURE-PD3, and the PGS for intelligence in LCC. The directions for significant associations in different cohorts were always identical. In particular, associations with PGS of cognitive phenotypes (intelligence, cognitive performance, educational attainment, and reasoning) all had positive directions, meaning that these PGS were protective factors (the higher the PGS, the higher the cognitive scores, the less cognitive decline). The models’ residuals were normally distributed (Supplementary Figs. 1–4).
We also performed additional analyses and ablation experiments. We investigated the potential associations with interaction terms between the APOE and GBA covariates and each PGS, but did not obtain any significant association in the meta-analysis after correction for multiple comparisons (Supplementary Table 6). We also performed the same analyses without the APOE and GBA covariates and obtained the same four significant PGS in the meta-analysis (Supplementary Table 7). We finally investigated the cumulative predictive power of the model with four significant PGS compared to the models with each single PGS. The model including the four significant PGS was significantly better than three models including a single PGS, for the PGS of reasoning (p = 3.97e−8), educational attainment (p = 1.34e−4) and cognitive performance (p = 0.0076). However, the combined model was not significantly better than the model with only the PGS of intelligence (p = 0.069).
Figure 3 highlights the survival plots on the whole population (six cohorts) for the four significant associations in the meta-analysis, with survivability being defined as not having a cognitive score below the defined cutoff values. Participants were grouped into four groups based on the quartiles of each PGS. Survival plots were significantly different between quartiles for the PGS of cognitive performance (p = 1.50e−4) and educational attainment (p = 1.68e−5), but not for the PGS of intelligence (p = 0.02) and reasoning (p = 0.02) after correction for multiple comparisons. Participants from a higher quartile tend to remain cognitively unimpaired longer than participants from a lower quartile, by the protective aspect of the four associations. The difference in years between the fourth and first quartile, for the probability of not experiencing any cognitive disorder yet equal to 0.5, was equal to 2 years for the PGS of intelligence, 5 years for cognitive performance, 7 years for educational attainment and 2 years for reasoning.
Discussion
This study demonstrates that genetic variants linked to higher cognitive or educational performance in healthy individuals are also associated with reduced cognitive decline in PD.
We report four significant associations with PGS, all corresponding to phenotypes related to cognition. The results were consistent across the cohorts despite their heterogeneity in terms of cognitive scales used and baseline characteristics. Survival plots highlighted an offset of several years between the first and last quartiles of PGS, especially for the PGS of cognitive performance and educational attainment. Importantly, the known mutations in the GBA and APOE genes were not involved in the PGS computation, and the associations were corrected for these mutations, implying that these significant associations involve other genetic variants. These corrections may explain why we did not find any association with PGS of disease-related dementia phenotypes such as in Alzheimer’s disease (AD), AD or a family history of AD, and Lewy body dementia (LBD).
The causal relationships between genetic variants and multiple interrelated phenotypic traits are often complex. In principle, a genetic variant could increase cognitive reserve and thereby indirectly protect against cognitive decline in PD through mechanisms that are non-specific and potentially important long before PD onset. Patients with higher PGS for intelligence, cognitive performance, educational attainment, and reasoning will plausibly have had higher cognitive performance before the onset of PD-related pathology. Alternatively, variants promoting cognition in healthy individuals might also act directly on molecular disease pathways over the course of PD. Our study was not designed to differentiate between these different modes of action. If there had been available data in our PD cohorts to adjust for educational attainment or cognitive performance early in life for instance, it could have indicated of whether these variables in themselves fully account for the difference in PD cognitive outcome, or if the PGS makes an additional, independent contribution. It seems likely, however, that differences in the rate of neuropathological change is not the main driver, and that the significant PGS in our study can be thought as a proxy for cognitive reserve.
Cognitive reserve focuses on the idea that there are individual differences in adaptability of functional brain processes that allow some people to cope better than others with age- and disease-related brain change48. Higher cognitive reserve has been suggestively associated with better cognitive function and lower risk of longitudinal progression to mild cognitive impairment in PD49, notably as cognitive reserve may have greater effects on the cognitive areas mostly affected in PD50. Higher cognitive reserve has also been suggestively associated with fewer motor symptoms in PD51. Nonetheless, further studies are required to investigate the impact of cognitive reserve on PD progression.
We acknowledge that the observed associations are not necessarily specific to PD, and we do not know whether the prognostic value of these PGS extends beyond what could be captured equally well or even better with cognitive assessments. Such assessments are resource-demanding, however, and not practical as an initial screening in large cohorts. Regardless of the causal relationship, the PGS highlighted in our study provides valid information on a PD participant’s risk of cognitive decline, without the need to measure cognitive reserve, suggesting a potential tool for risk stratification.
We did not observe any significant association with PGS of brain imaging phenotypes. However, the PGS of the cortical surface area in the whole brain was close to significance. Even though DIGPD was not included in this meta-analysis due to the different cognitive scales used to assess cognition, this PGS also had one of the lowest p-values in this cohort (although not being significant after Bonferroni correction). In addition, the directions were all identical (except in the Iceberg cohort, but the effect was very close to zero) with a protective effect (the higher the PGS, the higher the cognitive scores, thus the less cognitive decline). Associations with the real (not PGS) phenotypes have been reported in the literature, with cortical thickness in the left caudal anterior cingulate, lateral occipital and right superior temporal areas being thinner in participants with mild cognitive impairment than normal older adults52. In our study, PGS associated with brain imaging features represented the majority of the PGS investigated, leading to a lower significance threshold to account for multiple comparisons and, thus less power. Further studies with larger sample sizes or fewer phenotypes are required to draw conclusions regarding the PGS of these phenotypes.
We did not either observe any significant association with the PGS of AD, AD or family history of AD, LBD and PD, whether the APOE and GBA covariates were included or not in the models. Nonetheless, the p-values of the three coefficients for the PGS of AD, AD or family history of AD, and LBD were smaller when excluding the APOE and GBA covariates, and the associations would have been significant without correction for multiple comparisons. These results show that these PGS still capture some information about the APOE and GBA status, although these variants were not included in the computation of the PGS, which might be explained by the inclusion of variants in linkage disequilibrium in the PGS computation. On the other hand, the PGS of PD was far from being significantly associated with cognitive scores, with and without APOE and GBA covariates. These results suggest that the genetics of cognitive decline in PD might be more related to the genetics of cognitive decline in general than the genetics of PD.
Our study has limitations. The total sample size is still relatively small and limits the statistical power to detect weaker associations. The variable size of the different GWAS used to compute PGS is another limitation since PGS are imperfect predictors of the genetic liability of phenotypes. Imputation may introduce noise in the PGS calculation. Nonetheless, the quality control based on the PGS of height and body mass index suggests good PGS computation (relative to the SNP heritability of each phenotype) even in the cohorts with imputed genotype data. Our effect sizes obtained were heterogeneous, which might be explained by the heterogeneity between cohorts. Our approach does not allow for identifying individual genetic variants associated with the phenotype of interest (cognitive decline in PD in our case) which is inherent to the methodology. We only performed a meta-analysis and did not perform any replication analysis in external cohorts, nor compare our results to the potential effect of these PGS in the general healthy population. The definition of cognitive decline based on cognitive score cut-offs is suboptimal, and additional assessment is further required for better diagnosis.
Our study identifies associations between cognitive scores in PD and PGS of several cognitive phenotypes, with higher PGS of cognitive phenotypes being associated with reduced cognitive impairment in PD. The real phenotypes and their PGS have also been associated with cognitive decline in the general population, suggesting genetic similarity between cognitive decline in PD and in the general population, and supporting the importance of the cognitive reserve in the susceptibility to cognitive decline in PD.
Methods
Populations
We used data from six research cohorts, including the Drug Interaction with Genes in Parkinson’s Disease (DIGPD) study53, the Iceberg study54, and four cohorts from the Accelerating Medicines Partnership® Parkinson’s disease (AMP PD) program55: the Parkinson’s Progression Markers Initiative (PPMI), the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Parkinson’s Disease Biomarkers Program (PDBP), the Study of Urate Elevation in Parkinson’s disease (SURE-PD3), and the LRRK2 Cohort Consortium (LCC).
DIGPD is a French multicenter longitudinal cohort with annual follow-up of PD patients. Eligible criteria consist in recent PD diagnosis (UK Parkinson’s Disease Society Brain Bank criteria) with a disease duration of less than 5 years at recruitment. Data was gathered during face-to-face visits every 12 months following standard procedures.
Iceberg is a French longitudinal cohort with annual follow-up of idiopathic PD patients, patients with a genetic form of PD, and patients with idiopathic rapid eye movement sleep disorders. Data was gathered during face-to-face visits every 12 months following standard procedures.
PPMI is a multicenter observational clinical study using advanced imaging, biologic sampling and clinical and behavioral assessments to identify biomarkers of PD progression. Data was gathered during face-to-face visits every 6-12 months. PD subjects were de-novo and drug-naïve at baseline.
PDBP is an American clinical study developed to accelerate the discovery of promising new diagnostic and progression biomarkers for Parkinson’s disease.
SURE-PD3 is a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of urate-elevating inosine treatment to slow clinical decline in early PD.
LCC consists of three closed studies: the LRRK2 cross-sectional study, the LRRK2 longitudinal study and the 23andMe Blood Collection Study. The LCC followed standardized data acquisition protocols.
All studies were conducted according to good clinical practice, obtained approval from local ethic committees and regulatory authorities, and all participants provided informed consent before inclusion.
Key inclusion criteria
Further details of inclusion and exclusion criteria for AMP-PD cohorts can be found at https://amp-pd.org/unified-cohorts. As several of these studies included multiple study arms, only the inclusion criteria for the idiopathic PD study arm are summarized here as these are the patients we included for our analysis.
-
PPMI: PD subjects must have 2 of the following symptoms: resting tremor, bradykinesia, rigidity, OR either asymmetric resting tremor or asymmetric bradykinesia. PD participants were required to be 30 years or older at time of PD diagnosis, have a diagnosis of PD for 2 years or less at screening, Hoehn and Yahr stage I or II at baseline, confirmation of dopamine transporter deficit by DaTSCAN, and not expected to require PD medication for at least 6 months from baseline visit.
-
SURE-PD3: PD subjects had to fulfill diagnostic criteria for idiopathic PD with at least 2 of the cardinal signs of PD (resting tremor, bradykinesia, and rigidity), Hoehn and Yahr stage 1 to 2.5 (inclusive), absence of current or imminent PD disability requiring dopaminergic therapy (within 90 days of enrollment), aged 30 years or older at time of PD diagnosis, a diagnosis of PD within 3 years prior to the screening visit, and non-fasting serum urate ≤ 5.7 mg/dL at the first screening visit.
-
LCC: Idiopathic PD participants were eligible for this study if they were of Ashkenazi Jewish (AJ) descent and have PD or parkinsonism, aged 18 years or older, and no history of neurological or psychological illness.
-
PDBP: Clinically diagnosed with PD and aged 21 years or older.
-
DIGPD: Subjects were recruited in this longitudinal multicenter study at 4 University Hospitals and 4 General hospital in France between 2009 and 2013 and followed annually for up to 7 years. Inclusion criteria were patients with a diagnosis of Parkinson’s disease according to the UK Parkinson’s disease Society Brain Bank criteria with a disease duration of less than 6 years at baseline. Exclusion criteria were atypical parkinsonism or a history of treatment with neuroleptics. Patients for whom the diagnosis was revised to atypical parkinsonism during the follow-up of the study were excluded from the analysis. A complete description of the population is available elsewhere53.
-
Iceberg: This is an ongoing monocenter longitudinal clinical study conducted at the Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, France. Inclusion criteria for PD patients were a diagnosis of Parkinson’s disease according to UK Parkinson’s Disease Society Brain Bank criteria with a disease duration of less than 4 years at baseline. Exclusion criteria were atypical parkinsonism such as multiple system atrophy, supranuclear palsy, dementia with Lewy bodies, or a history of treatment with neuroleptics.
Participants
For our analysis, inclusion criteria consisted of having (i) a PD diagnosis, (ii) at least one visit assessing cognition with a cognitive scale, and (iii) genetic data available. Participants recruited in the genetically enriched arms (for carrying specific genetic mutations) of any cohort were excluded. Cognition was assessed using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) in DIGPD and the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in the other cohorts. As a measure of cognitive outcome, we used time from diagnosis to MMSE ≤ 27, or MoCA ≤ 24, as previously proposed as cut-off to define mild cognitive impairment in PD56.
Genotyping and quality control
Genotype data were acquired using Illumina Multi-Ethnic Genotyping Arrays in the DIGPD cohort (1,779,819 variants), Illumina NeurochipHumanCore-24-v1_A Genotyping Arrays in the Iceberg cohort (487,687 variants) and Illumina HiSeq XTen sequencer in the AMP PD cohorts (whole genome).
Standard quality control steps were performed in each cohort using PLINK57. We excluded variants with missing rates greater than 2% and variants deviating from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (p < 1e−8). We excluded related individuals (third-degree family relationships), individuals with mismatching between reported sex and genetically determined sex, and individuals with outlying heterozygosity (±3 standard deviations). For cohorts without whole-genome sequencing (DIGPD and Iceberg), we imputed missing SNPs using the Sanger Imputation Server58 for DIGPD and the Michigan Imputation Server59 for Iceberg, using the reference panel of the Haplotype Reference Consortium (release 1.1)58, then selected SNPs that were imputed with sufficient accuracy (INFO Score > 0.9 for DIGPD, R2 > 0.7 for Iceberg).
Genetic ancestry
To estimate the genetic ancestry of the participants, we used raw genotype data from the HapMap3 project to learn a low-dimensional representation of the genetic data, which captures the main dimension of ancestry, using principal component analysis. We then projected the raw genotype data of the participants onto the main principal components to identify in which clusters the participants were the closest to. Participants projected too far away (further than 6 standard deviations) from the European cluster were excluded. In further analyses, genetic ancestry was defined as the first four components of the principal component analysis.
GBA and APOE mutations
Specific GBA sequencing was performed in DIGPD and Iceberg. GBA mutations were extracted from such sequencing in DIGPD and Iceberg. For AMP PD cohorts, GBA mutations were extracted from whole-genome sequencing although this method could not formally distinguish these variants from variants of the pseudogene. GBA mutations were classified based on their association with PD severity60 and the numbers of mild, severe and undetermined GBA mutations were respectively computed.
The two SNPs involved in the APOE allelic variants associated with modified risks of developing Alzheimer’s disease (rs7412 and rs429358)61 were extracted from raw genotype data if available or from imputed genotype data otherwise.
Phenotypes and genome-wide association studies
We used the NHGRI-EBI GWAS Catalog62 to select the largest GWAS to date on samples of European ancestry for each phenotype of interest. From this database, we selected all phenotypes based on their known or putative implication as factors clinically associated with cognitive decline in PD and the general population, such as educational attainment, stroke, and Alzheimer’s disease (AD). A total of 19 such phenotypes were selected among 12 available GWAS. In addition, we selected the 79 brain anatomical phenotypes in all GWAS (such as white matter hyperintensities, subcortical volumes as well as cortical surface areas and thicknesses in several regions of the brain), as there is growing evidence of associations with brain anatomical phenotypes in PD63 and the general population52. Finally, two more general phenotypes (height and body mass index) were also considered, not only because height has been inversely associated with dementia in men64, but also because the real phenotypes were available and could be used as a sanity check of our methodology by assessing the quality of the computed PGS for these phenotypes.
Altogether, a total of 100 phenotypes were selected for this analysis among the 18 GWAS available in this database. When summary statistics from several GWAS were available for a given phenotype, we only included the largest study.
Polygenic scores
We used the LDpred2 algorithm65 implemented in the bigsnpr R package to compute all the PGS. More precisely, we used the LDpred2-auto variant which does not require any tuning samples65. This criterion was necessary as we computed PGS for phenotypes that were not assessed (i.e., the real phenotypes were not available).
The objective of the algorithm is to derive the joint effects (i.e., the coefficients in the PGS computation) from the marginal effects (i.e., the coefficients from the summary statistics of a GWAS). We used the linkage disequilibrium (LD) reference provided in the software, which is computed based on genetic data of 362,320 individuals enrolled in the UK BioBank study. The list of SNPs used to compute each PGS in each cohort consisted of the intersection of (i) the list of SNPs available in the given cohort, (ii) the list of SNPs in the LD reference (i.e., the list of SNPs from the HapMap3 project) and (iii) the list of SNPs in the summary statistics of the given GWAS, minus the SNPs matching exclusion criteria as recommended in the quality control step preceding the LDpred2 algorithm. No SNP is excluded based on their p-value with the LDpred2 algorithm: the p-value is used as a confidence measure of the marginal effect when deriving the joint effect. Such methods have been proven to generally perform better than clumping & thresholding65,66. None of the extracted mutations in the GBA and APOE genes were included in the PGS computation, as they are not part of the list of SNPs from the HapMap3 project.
Statistical analyses
Participants’ characteristics in all the cohorts were compared with chi-squared tests for categorical variables and analysis of variance F-tests for continuous variables. The quality of the height and body mass index PGS was assessed using partial correlation coefficients with correction for sex, age, age at PD diagnosis, and genetic ancestry. Longitudinal analyses were performed using linear fixed effects models to investigate associations between cognitive scores and each PGS, with correction for age at PD diagnosis, sex, time from PD onset, genetic ancestry, number of mild, severe, and undetermined GBA mutations, and APOE status. Visits with any missing clinical value among the variables used in the longitudinal analyses were excluded. Meta-analysis for cohorts using the same cognitive screening test was performed with linear fixed effects models. The usual 0.05 threshold was used to determine the significance of any statistical test, and per-sample Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was applied, leading to a significance threshold of 0.0005 for potential GWAS associations (100 GWAS included, see Results). Associations for significant PGS for the meta-analysis were visually inspected using forest plots. Survival plots were generated for such PGS, grouping participants each time into four groups (corresponding to the four quartiles for each PGS), and groups were compared using the log-rank test.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Data availability
The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon request (jean-christophe.corvol@aphp.fr). The genotype and clinical data for the AMP PD cohorts (PPMI, PDBP, Sure-PD3 and LCC) are available through the Accelerating Medicine Partnership® (AMP®) Parkinson’s Disease (AMP PD) Knowledge Platform. For up-to-date information on the study, visit https://www.amp-pd.org. Clinical longitudinal data and genotyping data for the other cohorts included are accessible through appropriate data-sharing agreements that protect participant privacy with the institutions that conducted or are conducting study consents and clinical assessments under local institutional review board approvals.
Code availability
The source code associated with the analyses performed is publicly available at https://github.com/johannfaouzi/genetics_cognitive_decline_PD.
References
Litvan, I. et al. MDS task force on mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: Critical review of PD-MCI. Mov. Disord. 26, 1814–1824 (2011).
Baiano, C., Barone, P., Trojano, L. & Santangelo, G. Prevalence and clinical aspects of mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: A meta-analysis. Mov. Disord. 35, 45–54 (2020).
Levy, G. et al. The association of incident dementia with mortality in PD. Neurology 59, 1708–1713 (2002).
Aarsland, D. et al. Cognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 13, 217–231 (2017).
Fagan, E. S. & Pihlstrøm, L. Genetic risk factors for cognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease: a review of the literature. Eur. J. Neurol. 24, 561–e20 (2017).
Hruska, K. S., LaMarca, M. E., Scott, C. R. & Sidransky, E. Gaucher disease: mutation and polymorphism spectrum in the glucocerebrosidase gene (GBA). Hum. Mutat. 29, 567–583 (2008).
Alcalay, R. N. et al. Cognitive performance of GBA mutation carriers with early-onset PD. Neurology 78, 1434–1440 (2012).
Setó-Salvia, N. et al. Glucocerebrosidase mutations confer a greater risk of dementia during Parkinson’s disease course. Mov. Disord. 27, 393–399 (2012).
Winder-Rhodes, S. E. et al. Glucocerebrosidase mutations influence the natural history of Parkinson’s disease in a community-based incident cohort. Brain 136, 392–399 (2013).
Mata, I. F. et al. GBA Variants are associated with a distinct pattern of cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 31, 95–102 (2016).
Liu, G. et al. Genome-wide survival study identifies a novel synaptic locus and polygenic score for cognitive progression in Parkinson’s disease. Nat. Genet 53, 787–793 (2021).
Yamazaki, Y. et al. and Alzheimer disease: pathobiology and targeting strategies. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 15, 501–518 (2019).
Williams-Gray, C. H. et al. Apolipoprotein E genotype as a risk factor for susceptibility to and dementia in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. 256, 493–498 (2009).
Nombela, C. et al. Genetic impact on cognition and brain function in newly diagnosed Parkinson’s disease: ICICLE-PD study. Brain 137, 2743–2758 (2014).
Morley, J. F. et al. Genetic influences on cognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 27, 512–518 (2012).
Mata, I. F. et al. APOE, MAPT, and SNCA genes and cognitive performance in Parkinson’s disease. JAMA Neurol. 71, 1405–1412 (2014).
Paul, K. C. et al. APOE, MAPT, and COMT and Parkinson’s disease susceptibility and cognitive symptom progression. J. Parkinsons Dis. 6, 349–359 (2016).
Srivatsal, S. et al. Cognitive profile of LRRK2-related Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 30, 728–733 (2015).
Shanker, V. et al. Mood and cognition in Leucine-rich repeat Kinase 2 G2019S Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 26, 1875–1880 (2011).
Ben Sassi, S. et al. Cognitive dysfunction in Tunisian LRRK2 associated Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 18, 243–246 (2012).
Somme, J. H. et al. Initial neuropsychological impairments in patients with the E46K mutation of the α-synuclein gene (PARK 1). J. Neurol. Sci. 310, 86–89 (2011).
Svetel, M. et al. No association between brain-derived neurotrophic factor G196A polymorphism and clinical features of Parkinson’s disease. Eur. Neurol. 70, 257–262 (2013).
Białecka, M. et al. BDNF G196A (Val66Met) polymorphism associated with cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Neurosci. Lett. 561, 86–90 (2014).
Iwaki, H. et al. Genomewide association study of Parkinson’s disease clinical biomarkers in 12 longitudinal patients’ cohorts. Mov. Disord. 34, 1839–1850 (2019).
Park, K. W. et al. Genomic association study for cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Front Neurol. 11, 579268 (2020).
Tan, M. M. X. et al. Genome-wide association studies of cognitive and motor progression in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 36, 424–433 (2021).
Rongve, A. et al. GBA and APOE ε4 associate with sporadic dementia with Lewy bodies in European genome wide association study. Sci. Rep. 9, 7013 (2019).
Jo, S. et al. Microarray genotyping identifies new Loci associated with Dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Genes 12, 1975 (2021).
Liu, G. et al. Mitochondrial haplogroups and cognitive progression in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 146, 42–49 (2023).
Yengo, L. et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for height and body mass index in ∼700,000 individuals of European ancestry. Hum. Mol. Genet. 27, 3641–3649 (2018).
Pulit, S. L. et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for body fat distribution in 694 649 individuals of European ancestry. Hum. Mol. Genet. 28, 166–174 (2019).
Davies, G. et al. Genome-wide association study of cognitive functions and educational attainment in UK Biobank (N=112151). Mol. Psychiatry 21, 758–767 (2016).
Davies, G. et al. Study of 300,486 individuals identifies 148 independent genetic loci influencing general cognitive function. Nat. Commun. 9, 2098 (2018).
Lee, J. J. et al. Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nat. Genet. 50, 1112–1121 (2018).
Savage, J. E. et al. Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence. Nat. Genet. 50, 912–919 (2018).
Nalls, M. A. et al. Identification of novel risk loci, causal insights, and heritable risk for Parkinson’s disease: a meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies. Lancet Neurol. 18, 1091–1102 (2019).
Kunkle, B. W. et al. Genetic meta-analysis of diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease identifies new risk loci and implicates Aβ, tau, immunity and lipid processing. Nat. Genet. 51, 414–430 (2019).
Schwartzentruber, J. et al. Genome-wide meta-analysis, fine-mapping and integrative prioritization implicate new Alzheimer’s disease risk genes. Nat. Genet. 53, 392–402 (2021).
Chia, R. et al. Genome sequencing analysis identifies new loci associated with Lewy body dementia and provides insights into its genetic architecture. Nat. Genet. 53, 294–303 (2021).
Malik, R. et al. Multiancestry genome-wide association study of 520,000 subjects identifies 32 loci associated with stroke and stroke subtypes. Nat. Genet. 50, 524–537 (2018).
Howard, D. M. et al. Genome-wide meta-analysis of depression identifies 102 independent variants and highlights the importance of the prefrontal brain regions. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 343–352 (2019).
Otowa, T. et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies of anxiety disorders. Mol. Psychiatry 21, 1391–1399 (2016).
Neale lab. UK Biobank. Neale lab http://www.nealelab.is/uk-biobank (2018).
Sargurupremraj, M. et al. Cerebral small vessel disease genomics and its implications across the lifespan. Nat. Commun. 11, 6285 (2020).
Adams, H. H. H. et al. Novel genetic loci underlying human intracranial volume identified through genome-wide association. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 1569–1582 (2016).
Satizabal, C. L. et al. Genetic architecture of subcortical brain structures in 38,851 individuals. Nat. Genet. 51, 1624–1636 (2019).
Grasby, K. L. et al. The genetic architecture of the human cerebral cortex. Science 367, eaay6690 (2020).
Perneczky, R. et al. Translational research on reserve against neurodegenerative disease: consensus report of the International Conference on Cognitive Reserve in the Dementias and the Alzheimer’s Association Reserve, Resilience and Protective Factors Professional Interest Area working groups. BMC Med 17, 47 (2019).
Gu, L. & Xu, H. Effect of cognitive reserve on cognitive function in Parkinson’s disease. Neurol. Sci. 43, 4185–4192 (2022).
Ciccarelli, N. et al. The role of cognitive reserve in cognitive aging: what we can learn from Parkinson’s disease. Aging Clin. Exp. Res 30, 877–880 (2018).
Lee, P.-C. et al. Examining the reserve hypothesis in Parkinson’s disease: a longitudinal study. Mov. Disord. 34, 1663–1671 (2019).
Cheng, C. P.-W. et al. Relationship between cortical thickness and neuropsychological performance in normal older adults and those with mild cognitive impairment. Aging Dis. 9, 1020–1030 (2018).
Corvol, J.-C. et al. Longitudinal analysis of impulse control disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 91, e189–e201 (2018).
Czernecki, V. et al. Social cognitive impairment in early Parkinson’s disease: A novel “mild impairment”? Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 85, 117–121 (2021).
Iwaki, H. et al. Accelerating medicines partnership: Parkinson’s disease. Genetic resource. Mov. Disord. 36, 1795–1804 (2021).
Hoops, S. et al. Validity of the MoCA and MMSE in the detection of MCI and dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 73, 1738–1745 (2009).
Chang, C. C. et al. Second-generation PLINK: rising to the challenge of larger and richer datasets. Gigascience 4, 7 (2015).
McCarthy, S. et al. A reference panel of 64,976 haplotypes for genotype imputation. Nat. Genet. 48, 1279–1283 (2016).
Das, S. et al. Next-generation genotype imputation service and methods. Nat. Genet. 48, 1284–1287 (2016).
Höglinger, G. et al. GBA-associated PD: chances and obstacles for targeted treatment strategies. J. Neural Transm. 129, 1219–1233 (2022).
Liu, C.-C., Kanekiyo, T., Xu, H., Bu, G. & Apolipoprotein, E. and Alzheimer disease: risk, mechanisms and therapy. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 9, 106–118 (2013).
Buniello, A. et al. The NHGRI-EBI GWAS Catalog of published genome-wide association studies, targeted arrays and summary statistics 2019. Nucleic Acids Res 47, D1005–D1012 (2019).
Sasikumar, S. & Strafella, A. P. Imaging mild cognitive impairment and dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Front Neurol. 11, 47 (2020).
Jørgensen, T. S. H., Okholm, G. T., Christensen, K., Sørensen, T. I. & Osler, M. Body height in young adult men and risk of dementia later in adult life. eLife 9, e51168 (2020).
Privé, F., Arbel, J. & Vilhjálmsson, B. J. LDpred2: better, faster, stronger. Bioinformatics 36, 5424–5431 (2020).
Ni, G. et al. A comparison of ten polygenic score methods for psychiatric disorders applied across multiple cohorts. Biol. Psychiatry 90, 611–620 (2021).
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Florian Privé for his support on using LDpred2. This work was supported by the French government under management of Agence Nationale de la Recherche as part of the “Investissements d’avenir” program, reference ANR-19-P3IA-0001 (PRAIRIE 3IA Institute) and reference ANR-10-IAIHU-06 (Agence Nationale de la Recherche-10-IA Institut Hospitalo-Universitaire-6), by the European Union H2020 programme (grant number 826421, project TVB-Cloud), by the ERA PerMed EU-wide project DIGIPD (01KU2110), by the Intramural Research Program of the NIH, National Institute on Aging (NIA), National Institutes of Health, Department of Health and Human Services; project numbers ZO1 AG000535 and ZO1 AG000949, as well as the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke. M.T. and L.P. were supported by the South-Eastern Regional Health Authority, Norway. Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Accelerating Medicine Partnership® (AMP®) Parkinson’s Disease (AMP PD) Knowledge Platform. For up-to-date information on the study, visit https://www.amp-pd.org. The AMP® PD program is a public-private partnership managed by the Foundation for the National Institutes of Health and funded by the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) in partnership with the Aligning Science Across Parkinson’s (ASAP) initiative; Celgene Corporation, a subsidiary of Bristol-Myers Squibb Company; GlaxoSmithKline plc (GSK); The Michael J. Fox Foundation for Parkinson’s Research; Pfizer Inc.; Sanofi US Services Inc.; and Verily Life Sciences. ACCELERATING MEDICINES PARTNERSHIP and AMP are registered service marks of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Clinical data and biosamples used in preparation of this article were obtained from the (i) Michael J. Fox Foundation for Parkinson’s Research (MJFF) and National Institutes of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) BioFIND study, (ii) Harvard Biomarkers Study (HBS), (iii) National Institute on Aging (NIA) International Lewy Body Dementia Genetics Consortium Genome Sequencing in Lewy Body Dementia Case-control Cohort (LBD), (iv) MJFF LRRK2 Cohort Consortium (LCC), (v) NINDS Parkinson’s Disease Biomarkers Program (PDBP), (vi) MJFF Parkinson’s Progression Markers Initiative (PPMI), and (vii) NINDS Study of Isradipine as a Disease-modifying Agent in Subjects With Early Parkinson Disease, Phase 3 (STEADY-PD3) and (viii) the NINDS Study of Urate Elevation in Parkinson’s Disease, Phase 3 (SURE-PD3). BioFIND is sponsored by The Michael J. Fox Foundation for Parkinson’s Research (MJFF) with support from the National Institute for Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS). The BioFIND Investigators have not participated in reviewing the data analysis or content of the manuscript. For up-to-date information on the study, visit michaeljfox.org/news/biofind.” “Genome sequence data for the Lewy body dementia case-control cohort were generated at the Intramural Research Program of the U.S. National Institutes of Health. The study was supported in part by the National Institute on Aging (program #: 1ZIAAG000935) and the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (program #: 1ZIANS003154). The Harvard Biomarker Study (HBS) is a collaboration of HBS investigators [full list of HBS investigators found at https://www.bwhparkinsoncenter.org/biobank/ and funded through philanthropy and NIH and Non-NIH funding sources. The HBS Investigators have not participated in reviewing the data analysis or content of the manuscript. Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from The Michael J. Fox Foundation-sponsored LRRK2 Cohort Consortium (LCC). The LCC Investigators have not participated in reviewing the data analysis or content of the manuscript. For up-to-date information on the study, visit https://www.michaeljfox.org/biospecimens. PPMI is sponsored by The Michael J. Fox Foundation for Parkinson’s Research and supported by a consortium of scientific partners: [list the full names of all of the PPMI funding partners found at https://www.ppmi-info.org/about-ppmi/who-we-are/study-sponsors]. The PPMI investigators have not participated in reviewing the data analysis or content of the manuscript. For up-to-date information on the study, visit https://www.ppmi-info.org. The Parkinson’s Disease Biomarker Program (PDBP) consortium is supported by the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) at the National Institutes of Health. A full list of PDBP investigators can be found at https://pdbp.ninds.nih.gov/policy. The PDBP investigators have not participated in reviewing the data analysis or content of the manuscript.” “The Study of Isradipine as a Disease-modifying Agent in Subjects With Early Parkinson Disease, Phase 3 (STEADY-PD3) is funded by the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) at the National Institutes of Health with support from The Michael J. Fox Foundation and the Parkinson Study Group. For additional study information, visit https://clinicaltrials.gov, NCT02168842. The STEADY-PD3 investigators have not participated in reviewing the data analysis or content of the manuscript. The Study of Urate Elevation in Parkinson’s Disease, Phase 3 (SURE-PD3) is funded by the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) at the National Institutes of Health with support from The Michael J. Fox Foundation and the Parkinson Study Group. For additional study information, visit https://clinicaltrials.gov, NCT02642393. The SURE-PD3 investigators have not participated in reviewing the data analysis or content of the manuscript. The authors would like to thank the DIGPD study group members:
• Steering committee: Jean-Christophe Corvol, MD, PhD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, principal investigator of DIGPD), Alexis Elbaz, MD, PhD (CESP, Villejuif, member of the steering committee), Marie Vidailhet, MD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, member of the steering committee), Alexis Brice, MD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, member of the steering committee and PI for genetic analysis).
• Statistical analyses: Alexis Elbaz, MD, PhD (CESP, Villejuif, PI for statistical analyses), Fanny Artaud, PhD (CESP, Villejuif, statistician).
• Principal investigators for sites (alphabetical order): Frédéric Bourdain, MD (CH Foch, Suresnes, PI for site), Jean-Philippe Brandel, MD (Fondation Rothschild, Paris, PI for site), Jean-Christophe Corvol, MD, PhD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, PI for site), Pascal Derkinderen, MD, PhD (CHU Nantes, PI for site), Franck Durif, MD (CHU Clermont-Ferrand, PI for site), Richard Levy, MD, PhD (CHU Saint-Antoine, Paris, PI for site), Fernando Pico, MD (CH Versailles, PI for site), Olivier Rascol, MD (CHU Toulouse, PI for site).
• Co-investigators (alphabetical order): Anne-Marie Bonnet, MD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, site investigator), Cecilia Bonnet, MD, PhD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, site investigator), Christine Brefel-Courbon, MD (CHU Toulouse, site investigator), Florence Cormier-Dequaire, MD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, site investigator), Bertrand Degos, MD, PhD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, site investigator), Bérangère Debilly, MD (CHU Clermont-Ferrand, site investigator), Alexis Elbaz, MD, PhD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, site investigator), Monique Galitsky (CHU de Toulouse, site investigator), David Grabli, MD, PhD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, site investigator), Andreas Hartmann, MD, PhD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, site investigator), Stephan Klebe, MD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, site investigator), Julia Kraemmer, MD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, site investigator), Lucette Lacomblez, MD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, site investigator), Sara Leder, MD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, site investigator), Graziella Mangone, MD, PhD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, site investigator), Louise-Laure Mariani, MD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, site investigator), Ana-Raquel Marques, MD (CHU Clermont Ferrand, site investigator), Valérie Mesnage, MD (CHU Saint Antoine, Paris, site investigator), Julia Muellner, MD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, site investigator), Fabienne Ory-Magne, MD (CHU Toulouse, site investigator), Violaine Planté-Bordeneuve, MD (Henri Mondor Hospital, Créteil, site investigator), Emmanuel Roze, MD, PhD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, site investigator), Melissa Tir, MD (CH Versailles, site investigator), Marie Vidailhet, MD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, site investigator), Hana You, MD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, site investigator).
• Neuropsychologists: Eve Benchetrit, MS (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, neuropsychologist), Julie Socha, MS (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, neuropsychologist), Fanny Pineau, MS (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, neuropsychologist), Tiphaine Vidal, MS (CHU Clermont-Ferrand, neuropsychologist), Elsa Pomies (CHU de Toulouse, neuropsychologist), Virginie Bayet (CHU de Toulouse, neuropsychologist).
• Genetic core: Alexis Brice (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, PI for genetic studies), Suzanne Lesage, PhD (INSERM, ICM, Paris, genetic analyses), Khadija Tahiri, PhD (INSERM, ICM, Paris, lab technician) Hélène Bertrand, MS (INSERM, ICM, Paris, lab technician), Graziella Mangone, MD, PhD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, genetic analyses).
• Sponsor activities and clinical research assistants: Alain Mallet, PhD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, sponsor representative), Coralie Villeret (Hôpital Saint Louis, Paris, Project manager), Merry Mazmanian (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, project manager), Hakima Manseur (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, clinical research assistant), Mostafa Hajji (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, data manager), Benjamin Le Toullec, MS (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, clinical research assistant), Vanessa Brochard, PhD (Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, project manager), Monica Roy, MS (CHU de Nantes, clinical research assistant), Isabelle Rieu, PhD (CHU Clermont-Ferrand, clinical research assistant), Stéphane Bernard (CHU Clermont-Ferrand, clinical research assistant), Antoine Faurie-Grepon (CHU de Toulouse, clinical research assistant). The authors would like to thank the Iceberg study group members:
• Nominated consortia representative: Prof. Marie Vidailhet, MD—Département de Neurologie, Groupe Hospitalier Pitié-Salpêtrière, 47-83 boulevard de l’Hôpital, 75651 PARIS Cedex 13, France, marie.vidailhet@aphp.fr.
• Steering committee: Marie Vidailhet, MD [(principal investigator of ICEBERG), (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France)], Jean-Christophe Corvol, MD, PhD [(scientific lead), (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France)], Isabelle Arnulf, MD, PhD (member of the steering committee), (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France; AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Sleep Disorders Unit, Paris, France), Stéphane Lehéricy, MD, PhD (member of the steering committee), (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière—ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France; AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neuroradiology, Paris, France);
• Clinical data: Marie Vidailhet, MD [(coordination), (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France)], Graziella Man- gone, MD, PhD [(co-coordination), (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France)], Jean-Christophe Corvol, MD, PhD (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France), Isabelle Arnulf, MD, PhD (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France; AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Sleep Disorders Unit, Paris, France), Jonas Ihle, MD (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France), Caroline Weill, MD (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France), David Grabli, MD, PhD (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France), Florence Cormier-Dequaire, MD, PhD (AP-HP Sor- bonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sor- bonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France), Louise Laure Mariani, MD, PhD (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié- Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France), Bertrand Degos, MD, PhD (Service de Neurologie, Hôpital Avicenne, Hôpitaux Universitaires de Paris—Seine Saint Denis, AP-HP, Bobigny, France; Center for Inter- disciplinary Research in Biology, Collège de France, INSERM U1050, CNRS UMR7241, Labex Memolife, Paris Sciences et Lettres, Paris, France);
• Neuropsychological data: Richard Levy, MD, PhD [(coordination), (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France)], Fanny Pineau, MSc [(neuropsychologist), (AP-HP Sor- bonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sor- bonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France)], Julie Socha, MSc [(neuropsychologist), (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France)], Eve Benchetrit, MSc [(neuropsychologist), (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié- Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France)], Virginie Czernecki, PhD [(neuropsychologist), (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France)]; Eye movement: Sophie Rivaud-Pechoux, PhD (coordination), (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France); Elodie Hainque, MD, PhD (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié- Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cer- veau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France); Sleep assessment: Isabelle Arnulf, MD, PhD (coordination), (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France; AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Sleep Disorders Unit, Paris, France), Smaranda Leu Semenescu, MD (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France; AP-HP Sorbonne Uni- versité, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Sleep Disorders Unit, Paris, France), Pauline Dodet, MD (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France; AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Sleep Disorders Unit, Paris, France);
• Genetic data: Jean-Christophe Corvol, MD, PhD [(coordination), (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France)], Graziella Mangone, MD, PhD [(co-coordination), (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France)], Samir Bekadar, MSc [(biostatistician), (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France)], Alexis Brice, MD (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France; AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Genetics, Paris, France), Suzanne Lesage, PhD (genetic analyses) (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France); Metabolomics: Fanny Mochel, MD, PhD (coordination), (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France; AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Genetics, Paris, France), Farid Ichou, PhD (ICAN, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris), (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France; AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Genetics, Paris, France), Vincent Perlbarg, PhD (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France), Benoit Colsch, PhD (CEA-INRA UMR 0496, DRF/Institut Joliot/SPI, Université Paris-Saclay, MetaboHUB, France), Arthur Tenenhaus, PhD (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France; L2S Laboratory at CentraleSupélec, France);
• Brain MRI data: Stéphane Lehericy, MD, PhD (coordination), (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France; AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neuroradiology, Paris, France), Rahul Gaurav, MS, [(data analysis), (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France)], Nadya Pyatigorskaya, MD, PhD (data analysis), (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France; AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neuroradiology, Paris, France), Lydia Yahia- Cherif, PhD [(Biostatistics), (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France)], Romain Valabregue, PhD (data analysis) (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France), Cécile Galléa, PhD (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France);
• Datscan imaging data: Marie-Odile Habert, MD (coordination), (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Nuclear Medicine, Paris, France; Sor- bonne Université, CNRS, INSERM, Laboratoire d’Imagerie Biomédicale, LIB, Paris, France); Voice recording: Dijana Petrovska, PhD [(Telecom Sud Paris, Evry, coordination), (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France; TELECOM SudParis, CNRS SAMOVAR UMR 5157, Département Electronique et Physique, MINES TELECOM, Evry, France)], Laetitia Jeancolas, PhD (Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France; TELECOM SudParis, CNRS SAMOVAR UMR 5157, Département Electronique et Physique, MINES TELECOM, Evry, France);
• Study management: Vanessa Brochard [(coordination), (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France)], Alizé Chalançon [(clinical research assistant), (AP-HP Sor- bonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sor- bonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France)], Car- ole Dongmo-Kenfack [(clinical research assistant), (AP-HP Sorbonne Université, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Department of Neurology, Paris, France; Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, ICM, Sorbonne Université, INSERM U1127, CNRS 7225, Paris, France)];
• Study sponsoring: INSERM, Paris.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
J.F. and M.T. are equal contributors to this work and are designated as co-first authors. O.C., L.P. and J.C.C. are their supervisors, are equal contributors to this work, and are designated as co-last authors. • J.F. designed and conceptualized the study, analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript for intellectual content. • M.T. designed and conceptualized the study, analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript for intellectual content. • F.C. analyzed the data and revised the manuscript for intellectual content. • S.L. analyzed the data and revised the manuscript for intellectual content. • C.T. analyzed the data and revised the manuscript for intellectual content. • A.B. revised the manuscript for intellectual content. • G.M. analyzed the data and revised the manuscript for intellectual content. • L.M.M. analyzed the data and revised the manuscript for intellectual content. • H.I. designed and conceptualized the study, and revised the manuscript for intellectual content. • O.C. designed and conceptualized the study, and revised the manuscript for intellectual content. • L.P. revised the manuscript for intellectual content. • J.C.C. designed and conceptualized the study, and revised the manuscript for intellectual content.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
Johann Faouzi—Reports no disclosures. Manuela Tan—Reports no disclosures. Fanny Casse—Reports no disclosures. Suzanne Lesage—Reports no disclosures. Christelle Tesson—Reports no disclosures. Alexis Brice—Reports no disclosures. Graziella Mangone—Reports no disclosures. Louise-Laure Mariani—Reports no disclosures related to the present work. Competing financial interests unrelated to the present work: received research support grants from INSERM, JNLF, The L’Oreal Foundation, the French Parkinson Association, Fondation of France, Paris Brain Institute BBT and Neurocatalyst calls; speech honoraria from CSL, Sanofi-Genzyme, Lundbeck, Teva; consultant for Accure therapeutics, Sanofi and received travel funding from the Movement Disorders Society, ANAINF, Merck, Merz, Medtronic, Teva and AbbVie, outside the submitted work. Hirotaka Iwaki—Reports no disclosures related to the present work. Olivier Colliot—Reports no disclosures related to the present work. Competing financial interests unrelated to the present work: O.C. received consulting fees from AskBio and Therapanacea and received fees for writing a lay audience short paper from Expression Santé. His laboratory received grants (paid to the institution) from Qynapse. Members from his laboratory have co-supervised a PhD thesis with myBrainTechnologies. O.C.’s spouse is an employee of myBrainTechnologies. O.C. holds a patent registered at the International Bureau of the World Intellectual Property Organization (PCT/IB2016/0526993, Schiratti J-B, Allassonniere S, Colliot O, Durrleman S, A method for determining the temporal progression of a biological phenomenon and associated methods and devices). Lasse Pihlstrøm—Reports no disclosures related to the present work. Has served as consultant for Roche, for which a fee was paid to his institution, and received grants from the South-Eastern Regional Health Authority, Norway, National Health Association, Norway and the Michael J. Fox Foundation. Jean-Christophe Corvol—Reports no disclosures related to the present work. Has served in scientific advisory boards for Biogen, Denali, Ever Pharma, Isdorsia, Prevail Therapeutics, UCB, and received grants from Sanofi, the Michael J Fox Foundation, ANR, France Parkinson, the French Ministry of Health.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Faouzi, J., Tan, M., Casse, F. et al. Proxy-analysis of the genetics of cognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease through polygenic scores. npj Parkinsons Dis. 10, 8 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41531-023-00619-5
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41531-023-00619-5