Abstract
Human brain morphology undergoes complex changes over the lifespan. Despite recent progress in tracking brain development via normative models, current knowledge of underlying biological mechanisms is highly limited. We demonstrate that human cortical thickness development and aging trajectories unfold along patterns of molecular and cellular brain organization, traceable from population-level to individual developmental trajectories. During childhood and adolescence, cortex-wide spatial distributions of dopaminergic receptors, inhibitory neurons, glial cell populations, and brain-metabolic features explain up to 50% of the variance associated with a lifespan model of regional cortical thickness trajectories. In contrast, modeled cortical thickness change patterns during adulthood are best explained by cholinergic and glutamatergic neurotransmitter receptor and transporter distributions. These relationships are supported by developmental gene expression trajectories and translate to individual longitudinal data from over 8000 adolescents, explaining up to 59% of developmental change at cohort- and 18% at single-subject level. Integrating neurobiological brain atlases with normative modeling and population neuroimaging provides a biologically meaningful path to understand brain development and aging in living humans.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
The human cerebral cortex develops in complex patterns1,2,3, giving rise to our cognitive abilities4,5. Biologically, these morphological changes are likely driven by developmental processes originating from different organizational levels. Microstructural reorganization, e.g., neuronal and glial restructuring, synaptic remodeling (“pruning”), as well as pericortical myelination, has been discussed as the main driver of cortical thickness (CT) development6,7,8,9. The neuronal component mainly consists in remodeling of dendritic arbor, with human postmortem evidence for increases of synaptic and dendrite density into childhood and early adolescence, followed by gradual decreases during adolescence10,11,12 that might extend even into adulthood13. As microscale developments on the synapse level alone are unlikely to explain macroscale CT changes, childhood and adolescence neuronal remodeling is likely to be accompanied by a changes of cortical glial cells6,14, in line with, e.g., microglia potentially playing an active role in developmental synaptic remodeling7. In contrast, pericortical myelination is thought to specifically influence magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)-based CT through myelin-dependent changes in tissue contrasts, which might result in apparent cortical thinning6,8.
Given the multitude of neurobiological mechanisms that likely shape cortex morphology over the lifetime, it is to assume that CT change patterns at any given developmental period result from several interacting biological factors jointly influencing cortical microstructure as outlined above. For example, concerted developments across cortical cell populations could be mediated via specific neurotransmitter effects projected from deeper brain regions, as was indicated in early non-human animal studies for, e.g., glutamatergic15 and serotonergic16 effects of thalamocortical projections on motor and somatosensory cortices as well as for dopaminergic effects of mesocortical projections on the prefrontal cortex17. Relatedly, neurotransmitter receptors likely play regulatory roles in cortical development as evidenced, for instance, by the effects of in-utero cocaine exposure on cortical macrostructure18, thought to be caused by a disruption D1 and D2 dopaminergic receptor influences during cortex development19. Unfortunately, as today’s neuroimaging tools do not suffice to study human cellular neurobiology in detail, we have to rely on scarce human postmortem and non-human animal data. Conversely, while structures and processes on the molecular level are partly accessible in humans with nuclear imaging, here, the exposure to radioactivity practically forbids application in typical developing children and adolescents, limiting its use to study neurodevelopment. While the study of neurobiological mechanisms underlying human brain development suffers from these practical challenges, considerable progress has been made in mapping the development and aging of human brain macrostructure, with large-scale normative models1,3,20 providing new insights in both population-level typical and individual atypical neurodevelopment21,22. Similarly, our understanding of general brain organization was significantly advanced by the availability of modern in vivo nuclear imaging atlases23,24, explaining typical brain organization and disordered brain structure to greater extents as compared to MRI-based brain structural and functional metrics25,26.
Neural cell populations and molecular-level tissue structures and processes – their neuroimaging-based correlates hereafter collectively referred to as “neurobiological markers” – are not uniformly distributed across the cortex, but show distinct spatial distributions27,28,29,30. Similarly, CT development and aging trajectories vary by cortex region3,11, resulting in distinct spatial change patterns associated with any given developmental period. We assume that these CT change patterns are not random but reflect neurobiological processes that causally influence CT changes over time. To elaborate our rationale (Supplementary Text S1.1 for a more detailed account), let X be a neurobiological entity that exhibits a non-uniform distribution across cortical regions, changes with neurodevelopment and aging, and might have direct or indirect downstream effects on CT. While X’s spatial distribution might change across the human lifespan, major distribution changes are more likely during childhood and adolescence (and again during aging) than compared to a relatively stable middle adulthood period. If changes in X lead to changes in CT, the spatial distribution of CT changes likely resembles X’s steady state, as we would assume regions with higher final density of X to have shown stronger developmental activity. We conclude that an observed “spatial colocalization” – the alignment of spatial patterns between two measured brain metrics – between (i) the distribution of X as measured during the stable period and (ii) the distribution of CT changes during a given developmental period could have resulted from a developmental or aging process that X is subject to. Notably, it is conceivable that a third process could influence both, X and CT, leading to a correlation between X and CT changes that is non-causal but still implies a neurobiological mechanism influencing both. Applying a similar reasoning, prior spatial colocalization studies have demonstrated that spatial patterns of CT development are correlated with adult distributions of glial cells, pyramidal neurons, and neuronal cell components31,32,33,34,35,36, providing the majority of recent evidence for human cortex-developmental mechanisms6,37. Of note, all cited studies are based only on bulk-sequencing postmortem gene expression data from the Allen Brain Atlas38, which may only poorly represent the in vivo expression patterns for many genes39. We emphasize that, despite causal assumptions being made on the conceptual side (see above), neither prior nor the current spatial correlation study can actually prove causal relationships between an MRI-observed change pattern and tested neurobiological markers. Relatedly, the specificity of spatial associations is inherently limited by the spatial resolution and noise associated with both correlated components.
Our current knowledge on biological factors that guide typical human CT development is severely limited by practical obstacles. Multimodal neuroimaging-based spatial colocalization approaches can provide a window into specific biological mechanisms, but – to our knowledge – developmental studies until now were limited to postmortem data. Combining these approaches with date’s availability of large-scale normative models and in vivo derived molecular brain atlases constitutes the next major step in the imaging-based study of human brain development (Fig. 1). If translated to the level of the individual subject, the approach can serve as the foundation for future neuroimaging-based yet biologically interpretable biomarkers to be tested for their clinical potential23,40,41.
Following this reasoning, in this work, we explore if and to what extent spatiotemporal patterns of CT change modeled throughout the human lifespan are explained by the spatial distributions of underlying neurobiological properties, and whether associations observed on the population level translate to individuals. We find that cortex-wide distributions of dopaminergic receptors, inhibitory neurons, glial cell populations, and brain-metabolic features can account for up to 50% of the interregional variance arising from CT development during childhood and adolescence. Cholinergic and glutamatergic neurotransmitter systems, however, best explain adult CT change patterns. The observed developmental associations replicate in independent longitudinal data, albite showing strong interindividual variance. Our study provides a foundation and blueprint for future investigations, exploring developmental spatial colocalization analyses as a tool for understanding both typical and atypical human neurodevelopment.
Results
Molecular and cellular neurobiological markers
We collected (i) 21 postmortem gene-expression “cellular markers” mapping neuronal and glial cell populations38,42,43, (ii) 27 in vivo nuclear imaging “molecular markers” of neurotransmitter receptors and transporters, synaptic density, transcriptomic activity23,24,25, as well as, to cover further potentially relevant factors, of brain metabolism and immune activity24,44,45, and (iii) an MRI-derived map of cortical microstructure (T1w/T2w)25 (Fig. 1A and S1; all derived from independent healthy adult samples: Supplementary Data S1, Supplementary Text S1.2.1 and S1.2.2). In support of our analytical rationale, three neurotransmitter receptors/transporters, for which alternative atlases from different adult age groups were available, showed high stability of spatial patterns during adulthood (Spearman’s rho ≥0.68; Text S1.2.3, Fig. S2). The analytic approach taken here establishes associations between temporospatial CT (change) patterns and brain atlases based on the similarity of cortex-wide spatial patterns (148 parcels; Destrieux parcellation46). Intercorrelation arising from spatial patterns shared between atlases on either cellular or molecular levels (Fig. S3A) was reduced by factor analyses applied independently to the cellular and molecular marker sets after parcellation of the individual maps. For each marker set, all unrotated factors that explained at least 1% of the set’s variance were retained, resulting in 10 factor-level nuclear imaging maps (ni1–10) and 10 gene expression cell marker maps (ce1–10). After promax rotation, these factors explained 90.9% and 86.9% of each marker set’s variance, respectively (Fig. S3B, C). We chose the liberal factor-number criterion to balance retaining as much of the spatial information in the neurobiological markers as possible with reducing marker multicollinearity in the following multivariate analyses. Factor solutions were successfully validated against permuted brain maps (Text S1.2.4) and factors were named based on the most closely related original atlases (Fig. S3D, E). The 20 factor scores, in addition to the marker of cortical microstructure (mr1), represented the neurobiological markers to be evaluated in subsequent main analyses (surface plots in Fig. 2 and Fig. S4). The dimensionality-reduced markers represented biologically meaningful entities with the first factor capturing the first spatial component of cortical transmitter systems (ni1), followed by more specific factors broadly representing serotonergic (ni2), dopaminergic (ni3, ni9), and cholinergic systems (ni5) as well as brain metabolism (ni4, ni6) and immunity (ni7, ni10). Similarly, mRNA expression-derived factors entailed one general neuronal dimension (ce1) and several more specific excitatory and inhibitory neuronal (ce4–10) and glial factors (ce2–3).
Mapping neurobiological markers to cortical development
In the following, we report on how these neurobiological markers colocalize and explain CT change patterns between 5 and 90 years of age (Fig. 1B), spanning developmental periods from “middle and late childhood” to “late adulthood” as defined previously1,27 (see following Figs.). CT trajectories for 148 Destrieux regions were derived from a normative model of CT development3 estimated from cross-sectional data of over 58,000 subjects (from here on referred to as “modeled CT”; Fig. 1B; Text S1.3.1; age distribution: Fig. S5; CT trajectories: Fig. S6A and Movie S1). First, we tested if modeled cross-sectional CT at each given time point in life was distributed across the cortex in patterns reflecting the distributions of specific neurobiological markers (Fig. 1C)32. To further understand the observed spatial associations, we then followed a hierarchical analysis framework based on regression models predicting the spatial patterns of pseudo-longitudinal “modeled CT change” from neurobiological markers. The outcome was quantified as the overall and marker-wise explained variance R2, interpretable as the percentage to which (modeled) CT change patterns can be explained from neurobiological markers25,26. In the first set of regression analyses, we assessed the combined and individual relevance of all 21 neurobiological markers for CT development and aging (D). In the second step, after identifying a subset of significantly associated markers, we evaluated their role in jointly explaining modeled CT changes while accounting for shared variance (E). In the final regress step, we repeated the analyses with those original neurobiological markers (i.e., before factor analysis) that loaded most strongly on the identified subset to demonstrate the validity of the factor-level markers. Next, we utilized developmental gene expression data (F) to validate and further specify our imaging-based findings (G). Last, we transferred our approach to longitudinal CT data from approximately 8,000 adolescents47,48 (H) to demonstrate that time period-specific association patterns identified using the normative model translate to the individual subject level (I).
Cross-sectional modeled CT shows diverse colocalization trajectories
Structural patterns resulting from the relative distribution of CT across cortical regions vary depending on the time point in life49. Temporal changes of these patterns might mirror the contribution of a certain cellular or molecular process to CT changes at a given time point. Using spatial Spearman correlations between each neurobiological marker and modeled CT at each time point, we revealed diverse colocalization trajectories with a general pattern of strongest changes from childhood to young adulthood (up to approximately 30 years) as well as in late adulthood (from 60 years onwards; Fig. 2 and S7). Colocalization strengths varied across the modeled CT percentiles extracted from the normative model, but temporal trajectories were consistent. On visual comparison, trajectories appeared similar across sexes but partly differed in overall colocalization strength (Fig. S8). The modeled nature of the colocalization estimates precluded statistical tests, which would need to be conducted in individual-level data.
Neurobiological markers explain CT change
Studying population-level and individual brain development and aging inevitably requires looking at respective changes over time, rather than focusing only on cross-sectional data50. We now asked to which extent different neurobiological markers explained the relative modeled change of CT across the lifespan (Figs. S6B, C) and which markers showed the strongest associations. Multivariate regression analyses predicting modeled CT change across 5-year periods throughout the lifespan (sliding window with 1-year-steps) showed that the combined, either molecular- or cellular-level, markers explained up to 54% of the spatial variance in modeled CT changes with peaks during young adulthood (molecular, 20–35 years) and adolescence (cellular, 15–20 years) [false discovery-rate (FDR)-corrected; Figs. 3 and 4, top]. Combining all 21 markers across biological levels explained up to 67% of modeled CT changes during the adolescence-to-adulthood transition (Fig. S9). Individually, 9 of the 21 neurobiological markers explained up to 15–38% of modeled CT change patterns, with most markers showing peaks up to young adulthood, i.e., between 5 and 30 years of age (FDR-corrected; Figs. 3 and 4, bottom). These 9 markers represented major neurotransmitters (dopaminergic, glutamatergic, cholinergic, noradrenergic), features of brain metabolism, neuron populations, and glial cells. All findings were robust against correction for baseline modeled CT as well as changes in sliding window step size, modeled sex, and modeled CT percentile (Figs. S9 and S10). Results were consistent after projecting all surface data into the more often used, but low-spatial-resolution, Desikan-Killiany parcellation51 (Text S1.3.2, Fig. S11). We did not find evidence for confounding effects of (i) the age distribution of the lifespan sample from which the modeled CT data was obtained or (ii) the approximated average ages of the adult samples from which neurobiological markers were derived (Text S1.3.3, Figs. S12, S13).
Specific neurobiological markers drive explained CT change
Next, we sought to understand in detail how the 9 significant neurobiological markers contributed to the overall explained modeled CT change while accounting for correlation and shared spatial variance patterns between molecular and cellular levels. Given that we found both the strongest modeled CT changes and CT associations during the period from childhood to young adulthood and given the particular clinical relevance of this timespan, we included modeled CT change from 5 to 30 years as an additional time window for further testing. Using dominance analyses25,26,52 to quantify the individual contribution of each univariately FDR-significant neurobiological marker in a multivariate setting, we found that the 9 molecular and cellular markers jointly explained 58% of modeled CT change patterns from 5 to 30 years, peaking at the transition from childhood to adolescence (10–15 years; Fig. 5, top). All 9 neurobiological markers contributed to the overall explained modeled CT change during different life periods (nominal p < 0.05) with 6 markers surviving FDR correction (Fig. 5, middle; Movie S2). During childhood and adolescence, 3 of these 6 markers explained most of the modeled CT change patterns, representing estimates of dopaminergic receptors (ni9; R2 = 16%; peek at 8–14 years), microglia and oligodendrocyte progenitor cells (ce3; R2 = 12%; 8–15 years), as well as of somatostatin-expressing interneurons (ce8; R2 = 12%; 5–14 years). Modeled CT change patterns during young and middle adulthood were explained by 2 neurobiological markers broadly associated with the major – i.e., dopaminergic, glutamatergic, cholinergic, and noradrenergic – neurotransmitter systems (ni3 and ni5; 29–56 years). Finally, late adulthood-modeled CT aging patterns were associated with a marker representing inhibitory neuron populations and astrocytes (ce4, 78–88 years). Except for microglia and oligodendrocyte progenitor cells, all identified associations were negative, i.e., indicating a stronger reduction of modeled CT in areas with higher density of the respective biological marker.
Specific cortical regions drive CT change associations
The spatial associations reported here are likely dominated by some cortical regions relative to others. By evaluating the impact of iteratively excluding each region from the multivariate models, we found that the most influential regions differed depending on the markers. For example, cellular markers associated to childhood and adolescence modeled CT development (ce9: somatostatin-expressing interneurons and ce3: microglia) were driven by premotor, cuneus, and frontopolar areas, whereas the association to dopaminergic receptors during this period (ni9) was more influenced by primary visual, mid-cingulate, and insular regions. While associations between modeled CT change during young and middle adulthood and cholinergic neurotransmission (ni5) exhibited similar patterns, adult colocalization to dopaminergic neurotransmission (ni3) was strongly influenced by sensorimotor areas (Fig. 6; Text S1.3.4; Fig. S14; Movie S2).
Factor-level markers reflect original brain atlases
Thus far, we focused on a lower-dimensional representation of neurobiological markers, which reduced predictor intercorrelation and increased statistical power, as compared to using the original 49 brain atlases. Nevertheless, we found that original atlases that were most closely related to each factor explained modeled CT change patterns to a similar extent as the factor-level models, aiding interpretation and supporting the validity of the factor-level approach (Text S1.3.5; Fig. S15 and S16). All univariate spatial associations between modeled CT change and the tested original atlases reached nominal significance (p < 0.05). Separate dominance analyses for each factor-level neurobiological marker with only strongly loading original atlases as predictors confirmed contributions of specific original atlases to the factor’s peak explained variance: somatostatin-expressing interneurons, dopaminergic D1 and D2 receptors, as well as glucose metabolism and aerobic glycolysis accounted for most of the associated markers’ peak effects during childhood and adolescence (ce9, ni9, ni4, and ni6). Peak effects during young and middle adulthood were mostly accounted for by α4β2 nicotinic receptors and the acetylcholine transporter (ni5) as well as the glutamatergic NMDA receptor (ni3; Fig. S15).
Developmental gene expression supports CT change associations
Next, we turned to developmental gene expression27 to confirm that the biological processes we found associated with cortical development were indeed upregulated in the cortex during the identified developmental period53. From a human postmortem dataset (n = 33, age range 0.33–82.05 years, see Kang et al.27 for details), we estimated gene expression trajectories across the neocortex corresponding to each original brain atlas relevant for the final 9 factor-level neurobiological markers (c.f., Fig. S15). For cell-type atlases, we averaged normalized gene expression values across the respective marker genes42,43. For molecular markers, we selected genes corresponding to each protein(-compound), in addition to two sets of genes associated with brain metabolism54 (Data S1). To pose as little assumptions on the sparse data as possible, we compared each gene/gene set during the age period with which the respective marker was associated with a control set of non-brain genes, testing (i) if the gene/gene set showed higher mean expression and (ii) if it showed a peak in its trajectory, quantified as a higher ratio of expression during versus outside the age period. As expected, most genes/gene sets showed higher mean expression and/or higher expression ratios during the respective neurobiological marker’s peak period, indicating that they were active in cortical tissue or had their individual peak expression in these timeframes (FDR-corrected; Fig. 7 and S17). Conversely, we observed relatively stable phases during mid-adulthood for most genes/gene sets, supporting our mechanistic rationale (see Introduction). Notably, for the two molecular markers explaining modeled CT aging patterns in adulthood (ni3: glutamatergic/ dopaminergic and ni5: cholinergic/ noradrenergic), we found evidence for associations only with dopaminergic D1 and glutamatergic NMDA receptors for ni3 as well as with the cholinergic α4β2 receptor for ni5.
Neurobiological markers explain individual CT trajectories
The above analyses successfully demonstrated that specific neurobiological markers account for a large proportion of variance arising from modeled CT change patterns. During the neurodevelopmental period from childhood to young adulthood, 6 markers accounted for about 50% of the total variance with D1/2 dopaminergic receptors, microglia, and somatostatin-expressing interneurons taking the largest share. Relevance of all these 6 markers during their respective associated neurodevelopmental periods could be confirmed in independent gene expression data. However, a sole focus on modeled population CT change, i.e., median predictions from the normative model3, does not allow for inferences about individual-level neurodevelopment, which is the mandatory prerequisite for exploring potential sources of interindividual variability. A successful validation in individual longitudinal data can also strengthen the potential mechanistic relevance of the identified neurobiological markers and support the use of normative models to non-invasively study neurodevelopmental mechanisms.
To demonstrate that our approach could be transferred to the individual level, we obtained 2-to-8-year longitudinal data from two large multi-site cohorts47,48, covering the neurodevelopmental period from late childhood to young adulthood (ABCD Study®: n = 6789; IMAGEN: n = 915–1142; Demographics and quality control: Text S1.4.1, Data S2, Fig. S18). Notably, only the ABCD baseline data, but not the ABCD follow-up data or any IMAGEN data were used in estimation of the Rutherford et al. CT model3. To further ensure independence from the CT model, we independently harmonized the ABCD and IMAGEN CT data across sites using ComBat(-GAM)55,56 (Tabs. S3 and S4). To nevertheless provide a reference for the extent to which CT changes could be explained in independent longitudinal data as compared to corresponding CT model predictions, we projected the ABCD and IMAGEN data into the CT model (Text S1.4.1) and extracted predicted CT for each individual subject and session (observed-vs.-predicted CT change patterns and correlations: Figs. S19 and S20). We first confirmed that the colocalization between cross-sectional single-subject CT and neurobiological markers mirrored the patterns observed for the modeled population-average (Figs. S7, S8, and S21). In line with these findings, the cohort-average relative change of CT across study timespans (appr. 10–12, 14–22, 14–19, and 19–22 years) was explained to extents comparable with predictions by the normative model (minimum/maximum observed R2 = 27/57%, model-prediction R2 = 47/56%; Fig. 8, upper and middle). These patterns translated to the individual-subject level, explaining on average between 9 and 18% in individual CT changes with considerable variability (range R2 = 0–61%; Fig. 8, lower; Fig. S22). Looking at individual marker-wise contributions, we again found the model-based patterns to be reflected on both cohort-average and individual-subject levels (Fig. 9; Figs. S22 and S23). While the neurobiological markers predicted to be most important (D1/2 and microglia) indeed explained significant amounts of CT change, two other markers, which primarily reflected aerobic glycolysis (ni4) and granule neurons (ce5), were equally dominant. Sensitivity analyses showed that CT change predictions (i) generalized from the normative data to individual subjects with above-chance performance but were a poor fit for many individuals, underscoring our focus on individual differences (Text S1.4.2; Fig S24), (ii) were not relevantly influenced by ComBat or CT model-based site harmonization (Figs. S22 and S23), (iii) increased with longer follow-up duration within each time period (Fig. S25), (iv) varied by sex and study site in some tested time periods despite site harmonization of the original cross-sectional CT data (Text S1.4.3; Fig. S26), and (v) varied with individual atypical CT development as well as data quality (Text S1.4.4; Fig. S27).
Discussion
Patterns of spatial colocalization between macroscale brain structure and the underlying neurobiology provide in vivo insight into healthy and pathological processes that are otherwise inaccessible to human studies. Our results suggest that the spatial alignment between modeled lifespan changes of CT and corresponding adult-derived neurotransmitter receptor, brain metabolism, and cell type profiles closely reflects neurodevelopmental processes across various neurobiological levels (see Fig. 10, S28, and Data S5 for a descriptive overview). While synaptogenesis and neuronal and glial proliferation continue into the first postnatal years, the second and third life decades are marked by a targeted reduction of neurons and cell components, likely reflecting functional specialization10,11,12,13,27,30,57,58,59. Indeed, our findings reveal dynamic patterns of associations between early CT development and neurobiological markers, in line with a diverse prior literature. Microglia have been implicated in synaptic remodeling7,32,35 and in myelination60,61, which has been shown to continue into young adulthood31,34,62,63,64. Brain metabolic demand was shown to peak close to the childhood-to-adolescence transition65,66,67, potentially connected to puberty-associated hormonal changes68. Somatostatin interneuron markers were shown to remarkedly decrease within the first decade of life59. Finally, dopamine D1 receptor activity was reported to peak in adolescence and young adulthood before declining steadily with age69,70,71. Notably, we identified the dopamine D1 and D2 receptors as the only neurotransmitter distributions that explained early CT development, in line with their initially discussed regulatory effects on cortical development17,18,19. Approaching adulthood, cortical development becomes less dynamic with most regions taking stable or steadily decreasing aging trajectories1,3. Only the cholinergic system consistently predicts CT changes throughout adulthood, potentially pointing to its role in healthy and pathological aging72. From a broader perspective, regional patterns of cortical development were often described in terms of a segregation in uni- and transmodal brain regions, showing distinct developmental trajectories and (micro-)structural profiles36,73. While we indeed observe regions typically implicated in these contexts to strongly contribute to child-to-adulthood developmental colocalizations (i.e., motor and medial occipital vs. lateral prefrontal and parietal cortices), we cannot report on a clear pattern, requiring further follow-up study.
While converging findings between our spatial association analyses and prior multi-disciplinary research can be taken as confirmation for our results, these convergences can neither prove that our results actually reflect cortex-developmental neurobiology, nor can they provide explanations for non-matching results. For instance, while one might intuitively bridge from our findings on a potential connection between CT aging patterns and cholinergic, glutamatergic, and dopaminergic neurotransmitter systems to neurodegeneration, it is much harder to find confirmation for a potential mechanism in the literature, considering that our findings are based on (modeled) typical CT aging patterns. Interpretation of our results is furthermore complicated by the constraints of the underlying data, with, on one side, the neurobiological brain atlases, which (i) were derived from independent adult populations of varying age and sex, (ii) were processed with different strategies, (iii) were in part – as was the developmental gene expression data – obtained from postmortem samples39, and (iv) exhibited shared spatial patterns, limiting specificity of single spatial colocalization estimates. On the other side, the normative CT model was based on (i) cross-sectional data from a (ii) largely White, Central European or North American74 population3,75 and (iii), while it was estimated in a sufficiently large20 and sex-matched cohort, may yet be biased by a non-uniform age distribution. Generally, although colocalization patterns were similar when evaluated using a coarser anatomical cortex parcellation, we recommend future studies to explore finer divisions based on cortical cytoarchitecture, which could potentially reveal more detailed associations. Nonetheless, we consider the high replicability of the observed associations, despite the noise introduced by the above limitations, to rather strengthen the robustness of our findings.
The spatial colocalization framework constitutes a powerful and flexible tool to study the biological underpinnings of both typical and atypical human brain processes in vivo. While its application in a wide range of neuroimaging contexts has brought valuable insights into the neurobiology of structural31,32,76 and functional77 brain development, as well as brain organization in health25 and disease26,40,41, inferences from spatial colocalization analyses are usually limited to an associative, non-causal level. Notably, this also is the case for our present study, in which all reported associations can only provide indirect evidence for involvement of specific neurobiological markers in CT development and aging, not providing actual mechanistic explanations. We identify two pathways to further evolve the spatial colocalization approach. First, spatial colocalizations, as readily available and efficiently applicable tools, can serve as potential guidance to targeted (causal) follow-up studies of specific processes. However, experimentally establishing causality (as in: manipulation of X causes changes in cortical morphology) is again likely hard to realize in humans, especially when brain morphology as compared to function is the outcome of interest. Second, from a neuroimaging perspective, the validity, meaningfulness, and – potentially – causality of spatial colocalizations can be tackled (i) by specifically testing on the individual level if the spatial distribution of a neurobiological process that is ethically measurable in vivo can predict the spatial distribution of neuroimaging outcome of interest, (ii) by providing supporting evidence from other levels of biological organization, which we assume to either influence or be influenced by spatially organized brain-processes, and (iii) by harnessing disorders with known pathobiology in a quasi-experimental lesion-mapping-like setting to explore influences of the pathological process on brain organization. Applying these strategies to the present work, follow-up studies could, for example, (i) test if cholinergic receptor distributions measured with PET at one adult timepoint predict CT change patterns in later adulthood, (ii) test if individual spatial colocalizations scale with genetic or epigenetic markers (as influencing factors), peripheral physiology or cognition (as influenced factors), or (iii) test if neurodevelopmental or -degenerative disorders such as attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, psychosis, Parkinson’s, or Alzheimer’s diseases lead to deviations in the expected developmental colocalization patterns (i.e., here, ni9, ni3, and ni5). Finally, strong quasi-experimental human evidence for the biological validity of spatial colocalizations between cortical morphology and neurobiological processes would be provided by the study of human subjects involuntarily exposed to development-affecting drugs (e.g., cocaine18) or rare disorders with specific targets such as genetic deletion/duplication syndromes or (pediatric) autoimmune encephalitides. Especially the latter could, due to their rapid onset, clinical course, and known targets (e.g., NMDA, GABAA, or GAD), lead to specific alteration patterns of expected developmental trajectories that might be captured with spatial colocalization approaches78,79.
Normative modeling of large-scale neuroimaging data has received considerable attention as a tool to translate basic research into clinical applications1,3,20,22,80. Our results indicate that if used as a reference for typical brain development, combining normative models of brain regional features with spatial colocalization approaches could facilitate discovery of physiological mechanisms underlying specific developmental patterns. Notably, here we focused exclusively on CT. Considering the diverging genetic influences and developmental trajectories of other brain-morphological features81,82, those might show different neurobiological associations. As evidenced by our comparative analyses between CT change predictions by the normative models and cohort-average CT changes as observed especially in the independent IMAGEN sample, normative models are capable of predicting population-level development, even when estimated only on cross-sectional data. Going beyond this group-level discovery approach, we demonstrate the feasibility of developmental spatial colocalization analyses in single subjects by mapping individual-level brain development to specific neurobiological markers. However, the strong variation in colocalization estimates observed on the individual level warrants further research into normative modeling of longitudinal data on the one side83 and potential sources of interindividual variability on the other. Taking a clinical perspective, in view of the ability of neurobiological markers to explain typical CT development patterns, studying how these findings translate to atypical neurodevelopment84 is a promising path for future research. Establishing developmental spatial colocalizations as potential diagnostic, prognostic, or therapeutic biomarkers will require demonstrating both their biological validity (see above) and sensitivity to the clinical outcome of interest. In the present case, their value as biomarkers may be limited by the large interindividual variation, requiring (i) further fine-tuning of individual-level cortex developmental change mapping including – if necessary – correction of study site confounds and (ii) optimization of the applied neurobiological markers in terms of source cohorts, potential dimensionality reduction, and spatial resolution.
In closing, it is important to reiterate that the central assumption of the spatial colocalization approach used here is that the spatial topology of adult neurobiology is reflected in neurodevelopmental changes in MRI-based CT estimates. Given the lack of non-invasive tools to study developmental neurobiology in vivo, our study provides evidence, albeit indirect, for such associations.
Methods
Ethics
No new human data were acquired for this study. Ethical approval for usage of publicly available and restricted-access databanks including human demographic, behavioral, and neuroimaging data has been granted by the Heinrich-Heine-University, Düsseldorf, Germany. Specific approval for collection and sharing of the used data (brain atlases, Braincharts model, Human Brain Transcriptome, ABCD, and IMAGEN) were provided by local ethics committees; detailed information is available in the cited sources. Informed consent was obtained from each participant and their parents in the case of underage subjects. Use of the ABCD data is registered at the NDA database at https://doi.org/10.15154/1528657. The responsible IMAGEN investigator is T. Banaschewski.
Software
Spatial colocalizations between CT (changes) and cortical atlases were conducted using JuSpyce 0.0.285 (https://github.com/LeonDLotter/JuSpyce) in a Python 3.9.11 environment. Other used software23,24,85,86,87,88,89,90,91,92,93,94 is listed in detail in Supplementary Text S1.5.
Data sources and processing
Atlases of molecular and cellular neurobiological markers
Neurobiological atlases (Fig. S1) were separated into two broad categories according to their source modality. Sample characteristics and data sources are provided in Data S1.
The neuroimaging (“ni-”) dataset consisted of group-average nuclear imaging atlases (neurotransmitter receptors, brain metabolism and immunity, synaptic density, and transcriptomic activity) and an MRI-based marker of cortical microstructure (T1w/T2w ratio; Text S1.2.1)23,24,25,44,45,95,96,97. Maps were (i) transformed from fsLR (metabolism and T1w/T2w) or Montreal Neurological Institute space (all others) to fsaverage5 space using registration fusion24,98, (ii) parcellated in 74 cortical regions per hemisphere (Destrieux46), and (iii) Z-standardized across parcels within each atlas.
Cell type (“ce-”) atlases were built by (i) retrieving genetic cell type markers identified by Lake et al.42 and Darmanis et al.43 via single-nucleus RNA sequencing in human brain tissue from the PsychENCODE dataset99, (ii) extracting Allen Human Brain Atlas mRNA expression values38 for each Destrieux parcel and each marker gene using abagen86 (default settings, data mirrored across hemispheres, Text S1.2.2), (iii) Z-standardizing the data across parcels within each gene, and (iv) taking the uniform average of the data across genes within each cell type.
We reduced the dimensionality of the atlas datasets to decrease multicollinearity in multivariate regression analyses. As the nuclear imaging and mRNA expression data likely differed strongly in terms of confounds and signal-to-noise ratio, and to study molecular- and cellular-level effects separately, data sources were not mixed during dimensionality reduction. To retain interpretability, we used factor analysis for dimensionality reduction (minimum residuals, promax rotation). All unrotated factors that explained ≥ 1% of the variance of each dataset were retained. We chose the oblique rotation method as the resulting factor intercorrelation would be expected from non-independent biological processes or cell populations. Resulting predictors were named by assigning each original atlas to the factor it loaded on the most (nuclear imaging: ni1–n; mRNA expression: ce1–n; MRI: only microstructural marker, no dimensionality reduction: mr1). In an additional analysis, we ensured that the factor solution estimated on the original brain atlases explained more variance in the original dataset than factor analyses estimated on permuted brain maps (see Text S1.2.4).
Braincharts CT model
The Braincharts reference model was estimated on 58,836 subjects from 82 sites (50% training/testing split; 51% female based on self-reported sex; age range 2.1–100 years; age distribution: Fig. S5). Detailed information on the included samples, CT estimation, and modeling procedure was provided by Rutherford et al.3. Notably, while ABCD baseline data were included in the model estimation, ABCD follow-up and IMAGEN data were not. Briefly, T1-weighted MRI data were obtained from the original cohorts and FreeSurfer 6.0100 was used to extract parcel-wise CT data. Image quality was ensured based on FreeSurfer’s Euler characteristic101 and manual quality control of 24,354 images3,47. CT development was modeled separately for each Destrieux parcel using warped Bayesian linear regression models predicting CT from age, sex, and site as a fixed effect. The applied methodology was developed for use in large datasets, can model nonlinear and non-Gaussian effects, accurately accounts for confounds in multisite datasets, and allows for estimation of site batch effects in previously unseen data3,94,102,103,104.
We extracted Braincharts “modeled” CT data separately for females and males for each of 148 cortical parcels for 171 timepoints (5–90 years with 0.5-year steps) and 7 percentiles (1st, 5th, 25th, 50th, 75th, 95th, and 99th). We focused on the age range of 5 years onwards as the used FreeSurfer pipeline was not adjusted for very young ages3. For colocalization analyses, the extracted modeled CT data were used as is. For model-based (pseudo-)longitudinal analyses, we calculated the relative modeled CT change ∆CT from year i to year j based on the median (50th percentile) sex-average modeled CT data as \({\Delta {{{\rm{CT}}}}}_{(i,j)}=\frac{{{{{\rm{CT}}}}}_{j}-{{{{\rm{CT}}}}}_{i}}{{{{{\rm{CT}}}}}_{i}}\). Lifespan CT change was then calculated using a sliding window with 1-year steps and 5-year length from 5 to 90 years as \({\Delta {{{\rm{CT}}}}}_{\left(i,j\right)},{i}\in \left[5..85\right],j=i+5\).
ABCD and IMAGEN CT data
Processed and parcellated CT data from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development℠ (ABCD Study®) cohort47 was taken directly from the ABCD 4.0 release. Baseline (T0, ~10 years) and 2-year follow-up (T2) structural MRI data were processed using FreeSurfer 7.1.1 by the ABCD study team. Details were provided by Casey et al.47 and in the release manual (https://doi.org/10.15154/1523041). For the IMAGEN cohort48, T1-weighted MRI data at baseline (T0, ~14 years) and at one or two follow-up scans (T5, ~19, and T8, ~22 years) were retrieved and processed with FreeSurfer’s standard pipeline (7.1.1). Following Rutherford et al.3, we relied on the total number of surface defects as provided by FreeSurfer for quality control. We excluded subjects that exceeded a threshold of Q3+IQR×1.5 calculated in each sample across timepoints101,105 or failed the manual quality ratings provided in the ABCD dataset. One ABCD study site (MSSM) stopped data collection during baseline assessment and was excluded. For each cohort (ABCD: n = 20; IMAGEN: n = 8 sites), we applied site harmonization to cross-sectional CT data of all subjects in one step across sessions using ComBat56, modeling age as a non-linear covariate (ComBAT-GAM55) in addition to covariate effects of sex and session. Facilitating comparison between observed and Braincharts-predicted CT data, we additionally projected the ABCD and IMAGEN data into the Braincharts model to derive predictions and individual deviation scores (for sensitivity analyses, Text S1.4.1).
Colocalization analyses were calculated on the site-adjusted and original CT values at each timepoint. For longitudinal analyses, the relative CT change between each time point within each cohort was calculated as above (ABCD: T0–T2; IMAGEN: T0–T8, T0–T5, and T5–T8).
Null map-based significance testing
Spatial associations between brain maps can be assessed in correlative analyses in the sense of testing for cortex- or brain-wide alignment of the distributions of two variables A (e.g., CT) and B (e.g., a neurotransmitter receptor)23,25,31,106. Effect sizes (e.g., correlation coefficients) resulting from correlating A and B convey interpretable meaning. However, parametric p values do not, as they are influenced by the rather arbitrary number of observations (between thousands of voxels/vertices and a few parcels) and spatial autocorrelation in the brain data107. Null model-based inference approaches circumvent this problem by comparing the observed effect size to a null distribution of effect sizes obtained by correlating the original brain map A with a set of permuted brain maps generated from B to derive empirical p values. From several approaches proposed to preserve or reintroduce spatial autocorrelation patterns in null maps107, we relied on the variogram-based method by Burt et al.87 as implemented in JuSpyce via BrainSMASH24,85,87.
Discovery analyses based on the Braincharts model
Lifespan colocalization trajectories
To characterize lifespan trajectories of colocalization between cross-sectional modeled CT and neurobiological markers, we calculated Spearman correlations between each brain atlas and modeled CT data at each extracted time point and percentile. Smoothed regression lines (locally estimated scatterplot smoothing) were estimated on data from all percentiles combined to highlight developmental trajectories. As the resulting developmental patterns were largely similar across sexes, we performed the main analyses on female-male-averaged modeled CT data and reported sex-wise results in the supplementary materials.
Prediction of modeled CT change
The main objective of this study was to determine the extent to, and the temporal patterns in which, neurobiological marker could explain modeled CT development and aging patterns. To achieve this goal, we designed a framework in which we predicted stepwise relative (modeled) CT change from one or more brain atlases in multivariate or univariate regression analyses. The amount of (modeled) CT variance explained R2 was used as the main outcome measure (adjusted in multivariate analyses). Exact one-sided p values were calculated by generating a constant set of 10,000 null maps for each brain atlas and comparing observed R2 values to R2 null distributions obtained from 10,000 regression analyses using the null maps as predictors.
To determine the general extent to which modeled CT changes could be explained, we performed one multilinear regression per lifespan timestep (81 models) using (i) all neuroimaging and (ii) all mRNA expression-based atlases. In an additional analysis, we assessed the result combining all atlases irrespective of modality. The resulting p values were FDR-corrected across all models and atlas source modalities. To quantify individual atlas-wise effects and identify specific neurobiological markers of potential relevance to CT development, we performed univariate regression analyses per timestep and atlas (21 × 81 models), correcting for multiple comparisons using FDR correction within each modality. In sensitivity analyses, we assessed effects of correcting for baseline modeled CT (regression of modeled cross-sectional CT at year x from CT change between year x and year y), adjusting modeled CT percentile (1st and 99th), sex (female and male separately), and window length (1-year, 2-year). As above, the results were consistent across sexes, thus all main analyses were reported for sex-average modeled CT data and the following model-based analyses were performed only on sex-average data. As our analyses are based on the 148-parcel Destrieux parcellation46, while the low-resolution Desikan-Killiany parcellation51 (68 parcels) is more prevalent in the literature, we evaluated if spatial association patterns remained stable using Desikan-Killiany-transformed data (Text S1.3.2). Finally, we tested if explained modeled CT change across time windows was correlated to (i) the number of subjects that went into model estimation per time window and (ii) the distance in years to the approximate age of the neurobiological marker sources (Text S1.3.3).
Marker-wise contributions to explained modeled CT change
Aiming to identify when and how neurobiological markers contributed to explaining modeled CT change, we retained only those brain atlases that significantly explained modeled CT development individually (FDR correction) and conducted dominance analyses predicting modeled CT change from this joint set of atlases. Dominance analysis aims to quantify the relative importance of each predictor in a multivariate regression. The total dominance statistic is calculated as the average contribution of a predictor x to the total R2 across all possible subset models of multivariate regression and can here be interpreted as the extent to which modeled CT development during a certain time period is explained by x in presence of the whole set of predictors X and as a fraction of the extent to which modeled CT development is explained by set X25,26,52. Following from this, in our models, the sum of the atlas-level R2 at a given timespan equals the total R2 at this time point. The significance of dominance analyses was determined as described above by generating null distributions and estimating empirical p values for both, the “full model” multivariate R2 and the predictor-wise total dominance R2. Finally, Spearman correlations between modeled CT change and neurobiological markers were conducted to indicate the directionality of associations.
Dominance analyses were conducted at each timestep and, to highlight the main postnatal developmental period between child and adulthood, on the modeled CT development across this entire period defined as \({\Delta {CT}}_{({{\mathrm{5,30}}})}\) (82 models). Resulting p values were corrected across the whole analysis (full model and atlas-wise: 82 + 82 × 9 p values).
Brain-regional influences on modeled CT change association patterns
To estimate the importance of individual brain regions for the associations between CT change and brain atlases, we relied on the atlas-wise residual differences across brain-regions as unitless measures of the influence of individual cortex regions on the dominance analysis results. The residual difference of prediction errors ∆PE for each predictor x out of the predictor set X was calculated as \(\Delta {{{\rm{PE}}}}=\left|{{{{\rm{PE}}}}}_{X\setminus \{x\}}\right|-\left|{{{{\rm{PE}}}}}_{X}\right|\). The results were visualized on surface maps for descriptive interpretation.
Relationships between dimensionality-reduced and original neurobiological markers
To assess whether the factor-level markers represented the original neurobiological atlases according to the applied atlas-factor-association scheme, we performed (i) dominance analyses and (ii) univariate regressions per factor-level atlas using only the strongest associated original atlases as predictors. The latter were defined as the five atlases that loaded the most on each factor if the absolute loading exceeded 0.3. FDR correction was performed independently for dominance analyses and univariate regressions across all tests.
Validation analyses based on developmental gene expression data
Data sources and (null) gene set construction
Normalized developmental gene expression data for n = 17,565 genes was downloaded from the Human Brain Transcriptome database (https://hbatlas.org/pages/data); the original dataset was published by Kang et al.27. As of the postnatal focus of our study, we included only subjects after birth, resulting in n = 33, aged between 0.33 and 82.05 years. The original data was sampled across multiple cortical regions and, in some cases, both hemispheres per subject. However, because a maximum of only 11 cortex regions was sampled, we decided to average the data per subject across hemispheres and neocortical areas (c.f. Kang et al.).
We identified the original brain atlases that loaded most strongly on each factor-level neurobiological marker (c.f. section 4.5.5). Each of these original atlases was represented in the genetic data through a single gene or a set of genes (Tab. 1); in the case of gene sets, gene expression data was averaged across genes. For most nuclear imaging maps, we selected the genes or gene sets that coded for, or were associated with, the respective tracer target. For brain metabolism maps, we took two gene sets associated with aerobic and anaerobic glycolysis from Goyal et al.54. We did not have a gene set for the CBF (cerebral blood flow) map. For cell type maps, we took the original gene sets from which the maps were generated42,43.
For permutation-based significance testing (see below), we created n = 10,000 null gene expression datasets by randomly selecting genes or same-sized gene sets from n = 2154 non-brain genes (https://www.proteinatlas.org/humanproteome/brain/human+brain, “Not detected in brain”).
Associations to temporal patterns of explained CT development
The following process was used to test for associations between modeled CT change explained by neurobiological markers and developmental gene expression trajectories: (i) We fitted a smoothed regression line (locally estimated scatterplot smoothing) to the gene expression data associated with each gene/gene set as well as to the respective null gene expression datasets. (ii) For each dimensionality-reduced neurobiological marker, we identified the time period in which it explained modeled CT change significantly (FDR-corrected). (iii) For each of the (null) gene expression trajectories associated with the current neurobiological marker, we calculated the average gene expressions during and outside of the significant time period. (iv) We separately compared the mean and the ratio of gene expression during vs. outside the significant time period between the observed and null gene expression data to derive empirical one-sided p values for the association between each neurobiological marker and each associated gene/gene set. (v) FDR-correction was applied across all tests at once. A significantly higher mean expression would indicate increased cortical expression of a marker during the tested time period as compared to non-brain genes. An increased ratio would broadly indicate that the marker’s gene expressions peaks during the tested time period.
Validation analyses based on ABCD and IMAGEN single-subject data
Developmental colocalization trajectories
First, we tested whether colocalization patterns between neurobiological markers and single-subject cross-sectional CT followed the predictions of the Braincharts model. Spearman correlations were calculated between each subject’s CT values and each atlas at all available timepoints, for CT data (i) as extracted from FreeSurfer, (ii) after ComBat-harmonization, (iii) after projection into the Braincharts model, and (iv) as predicted by the model.
Explained CT development patterns on cohort- and individual-subject levels
Following, we assessed how neurobiological markers that significantly explained modeled CT development during the period covered by ABCD and IMAGEN data (9–25 years) performed in individual-subject longitudinal data. Dominance analyses were performed in two steps. First, for each of the four investigated time spans (ABCD: ~10–12; IMAGEN: ~14–22, ~14–19, 19–22 years), one dominance analysis was calculated to predict the cohort-average CT change pattern from neurobiological markers. Second, dominance analyses were calculated in the same fashion, but for every subject. For comparison, analyses were repeated on CT change patterns as predicted by the Braincharts model from each subject’s age and sex. For cohort-average dominance analyses, exact p values were estimated as described for the stepwise model-based analyses. For individual-level analyses, instead of estimating p values for each subject, we tested whether the mean R2 values of the full models and each predictor observed in each cohort and time span were significantly higher than was estimated in 1000 null-analyses with permuted atlas data. Finally, we repeated the subject-level analyses on the original CT change data prior to site-harmonization, under correction for intracranial volume, and on the longitudinal change of deviation Z scores as returned by the Braincharts model3.
We then estimated how the subject-level regression models generalized from the subject-level normative CT change patterns to the actual observed CT change by applying the regression models estimated on each subject’s normative CT change patterns to each subject’s observed CT change (Text S1.4.2).
Further sensitivity analyses were conducted to estimate how CT change predictions were affected by follow-up duration, sex, study site, the prediction accuracy of CT and CT change patterns [correlation between predicted and observed CT (change), average Braincharts CT deviation (change), count of extreme deviation (change)], and data quality (number of surface defects). Subject-level full model R2 values were compared by sex and study site using analyses of covariances corrected for follow-up duration (and sex or site). All other variables were correlated with full model R2 values using Spearman correlations.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Data availability
The neurobiological marker source data, the Braincharts models, data extracted from these models, developmental gene expression data, and colocalization results generated in this study have been deposited in an associated GitHub repository (https://github.com/LeonDLotter/CTdev/; https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7902901)108. The Braincharts models are furthermore available at https://github.com/predictive-clinical-neuroscience/braincharts. The individual-level raw and processed ABCD and IMAGEN data are available under restricted access for data privacy reasons and by regulations of the original investigators. Access to ABCD can be obtained by application to the NIHM Data Archive (https://nda.nih.gov/study.html?id=2313). Further information on how to gain access is provided on the ABCD study website (https://wiki.abcdstudy.org/faq/faq.html). Access to IMAGEN can be obtained via a project-specific application coordinated by an IMAGEN principal investigator. The responsible researcher for the present study is author TB (tobias.banaschewski@zi-mannheim.de). Review of data access proposals is expected within weeks after submission. Protected data generated by this study can be shared with any researcher with approved data access. Group-level processed ABCD and IMAGEN data are available in the linked repository. Source Data files corresponding to each main and Supplementary Fig. are provided with this paper. As subject-level ABCD and IMAGEN data cannot be shared openly, respective Source Data files contain group-level summary metrics.
Code availability
All code supporting the analyses and conclusions of this publication is available via the publication repository (https://github.com/LeonDLotter/CTdev/; https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7901282)108. The code is organized in annotated Jupyter notebooks, which also produce all main and Supplementary Figs.
References
Bethlehem, R. A. I. et al. Brain charts for the human lifespan. Nature 604, 525–533 (2022).
Gogtay, N. et al. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 101, 8174–8179 (2004).
Rutherford, S. et al. Charting brain growth and aging at high spatial precision. eLife 11, e72904 (2022).
Vanderhaeghen, P. & Polleux, F. Developmental mechanisms underlying the evolution of human cortical circuits. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 213–232 (2023).
Erus, G. et al. Imaging patterns of brain development and their relationship to cognition. Cereb. Cortex 25, 1676–1684 (2015).
Norbom, L. B. et al. New insights into the dynamic development of the cerebral cortex in childhood and adolescence: Integrating macro- and microstructural MRI findings. Prog. Neurobiol. 204, 102109 (2021).
Paolicelli, R. C. et al. Synaptic pruning by microglia is necessary for normal brain development. Science 333, 1456–1458 (2011).
Sowell, E. R. et al. Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. J. Neurosci. 24, 8223–8231 (2004).
Walhovd, K. B., Fjell, A. M., Giedd, J., Dale, A. M. & Brown, T. T. Through Thick and Thin: a Need to Reconcile Contradictory Results on Trajectories in Human Cortical Development. Cereb. Cortex 27, bhv301 (2017).
Huttenlocher, P. R. Synaptic density in human frontal cortex - developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Res 163, 195–205 (1979).
Huttenlocher, P. R. Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development. Neuropsychologia 28, 517–527 (1990).
Petanjek, Z., Judaš, M., Kostović, I. & Uylings, H. B. M. Lifespan alterations of basal dendritic trees of pyramidal neurons in the human prefrontal cortex: a layer-specific pattern. Cereb. Cortex 18, 915–929 (2008).
Petanjek, Z. et al. Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 108, 13281–13286 (2011).
Paus, T., Keshavan, M. & Giedd, J. N. Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 947–957 (2008).
Li, H. et al. Laminar and columnar development of Barrel Cortex Relies on Thalamocortical neurotransmission. Neuron 79, 970–986 (2013).
Altamura, C. et al. Altered neocortical cell density and layer thickness in Serotonin transporter knockout mice: a quantitation study. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1394–1401 (2007).
Kalsbeek, A., Matthijssen, M. A. H. & Uylings, H. B. M. Morphometric analysis of prefrontal cortical development following neonatal lesioning of the dopaminergic mesocortical projection. Exp. Brain Res. 78, 279–289 (1989).
Grewen, K. et al. Prenatal cocaine effects on brain structure in early infancy. NeuroImage 101, 114–123 (2014).
Bhide, P. G. Dopamine, cocaine and the development of cerebral cortical cytoarchitecture: A review of current concepts. Semin. Cell Dev. Biol. 20, 395–402 (2009).
Ge, R. et al. Normative modelling of brain morphometry across the lifespan with CentileBrain: algorithm benchmarking and model optimisation. Lancet Digit. Health 6, e211–e221 (2024).
Marquand, A. F., Rezek, I., Buitelaar, J. & Beckmann, C. F. Understanding heterogeneity in clinical cohorts using normative models: beyond case-control studies. Biol. Psychiatry 80, 552–561 (2016).
Bedford, S. A., Seidlitz, J. & Bethlehem, R. A. I. Translational potential of human brain charts. Clin. Transl. Med 12, e960 (2022).
Dukart, J. et al. JuSpace: A tool for spatial correlation analyses of magnetic resonance imaging data with nuclear imaging derived neurotransmitter maps. Hum. Brain Mapp. 42, 555–566 (2021).
Markello, R. D. et al. neuromaps: structural and functional interpretation of brain maps. Nat. Methods 1–8 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41592-022-01625-w (2022).
Hansen, J. Y. et al. Mapping neurotransmitter systems to the structural and functional organization of the human neocortex. Nat. Neurosci. 25, 1569–1581 (2022).
Hansen, J. Y. et al. Local molecular and global connectomic contributions to cross-disorder cortical abnormalities. Nat. Commun. 13, 4682 (2022).
Kang, H. J. et al. Spatio-temporal transcriptome of the human brain. Nature 478, 483–489 (2011).
Zhu, Y. et al. Spatiotemporal transcriptomic divergence across human and macaque brain development. Science 362, eaat8077 (2018).
Siletti, K. et al. Transcriptomic diversity of cell types across the adult human brain. Science 382, eadd7046 (2023).
Huttenlocher, P. R. & Dabholkar, A. S. Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 387, 167–178 (1997).
Parker, N. et al. Assessment of neurobiological mechanisms of cortical thinning during childhood and adolescence and their implications for psychiatric disorders. JAMA Psychiatry 77, 1127 (2020).
Vidal-Pineiro, D. et al. Cellular correlates of cortical thinning throughout the lifespan. Sci. Rep. 10, 21803 (2020).
Ball, G., Seidlitz, J., Beare, R. & Seal, M. L. Cortical remodelling in childhood is associated with genes enriched for neurodevelopmental disorders. NeuroImage 215, 116803 (2020).
Patel, Y. et al. Maturation of the human cerebral cortex during adolescence: myelin or dendritic arbor? Cereb. Cortex 29, 3351–3362 (2019).
Shin, J. et al. Cell-specific gene-expression profiles and cortical thickness in the human brain. Cereb. Cortex 28, 3267–3277 (2018).
Whitaker, K. J. et al. Adolescence is associated with genomically patterned consolidation of the hubs of the human brain connectome. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 9105–9110 (2016).
Paus, T. Imaging microstructure in the living human brain: A viewpoint. NeuroImage 182, 3–7 (2018).
Hawrylycz, M. J. et al. An anatomically comprehensive atlas of the adult human brain transcriptome. Nature 489, 391–399 (2012).
Liharska, L. E. et al. A study of gene expression in the living human brain. 2023.04.21.23288916 Preprint at https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.04.21.23288916 (2023).
Kasper, J. et al. Local synchronicity in dopamine-rich caudate nucleus influences Huntington’s disease motor phenotype. Brain 146, 3319–3330 (2023).
Premi, E. et al. Unravelling neurotransmitters impairment in primary progressive aphasias. Hum. Brain Mapp. 44, 2245–2253 (2023).
Lake, B. B. et al. Neuronal subtypes and diversity revealed by single-nucleus RNA sequencing of the human brain. Science 352, 1586–1590 (2016).
Darmanis, S. et al. A survey of human brain transcriptome diversity at the single cell level. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 7285–7290 (2015).
Kim, M.-J. et al. First-in-human evaluation of [11C]PS13, a novel PET radioligand, to quantify cyclooxygenase-1 in the brain. Eur. J. Nucl. Med Mol. Imaging 47, 3143–3151 (2020).
Lois, C. et al. Neuroinflammation in Huntington’s disease: new insights with 11 C-PBR28 PET/MRI. ACS Chem. Neurosci. 9, 2563–2571 (2018).
Destrieux, C., Fischl, B., Dale, A. & Halgren, E. Automatic parcellation of human cortical gyri and sulci using standard anatomical nomenclature. NeuroImage 53, 1–15 (2010).
Casey, B. J. et al. The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 43–54 (2018).
Schumann, G. et al. The IMAGEN study: reinforcement-related behaviour in normal brain function and psychopathology. Mol. Psychiatry 15, 1128–1139 (2010).
Douaud, G. et al. A common brain network links development, aging, and vulnerability to disease. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 111, 17648–17653 (2014).
Becht, A. I. & Mills, K. L. Modeling individual differences in brain development. Biol. Psychiatry 88, 63–69 (2020).
Desikan, R. S. et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage 31, 968–980 (2006).
Azen, R. & Budescu, D. V. The dominance analysis approach for comparing predictors in multiple regression. Psychol. Methods 8, 129–148 (2003).
Warrier, V. et al. Genetic insights into human cortical organization and development through genome-wide analyses of 2,347 neuroimaging phenotypes. Nat. Genet 55, 1483–1493 (2023).
Goyal, M. S., Hawrylycz, M., Miller, J. A., Snyder, A. Z. & Raichle, M. E. Aerobic glycolysis in the human brain is associated with development and neotenous gene expression. Cell Metab. 19, 49–57 (2014).
Pomponio, R. et al. Harmonization of large MRI datasets for the analysis of brain imaging patterns throughout the lifespan. NeuroImage 208, 116450 (2020).
Fortin, J.-P. et al. Harmonization of cortical thickness measurements across scanners and sites. NeuroImage 167, 104–120 (2018).
Yeung, M. S. Y. et al. Dynamics of Oligodendrocyte generation and myelination in the human brain. Cell 159, 766–774 (2014).
Glantz, L. A., Gilmore, J. H., Hamer, R. M., Lieberman, J. A. & Jarskog, L. F. Synaptophysin and PSD-95 in the human prefrontal cortex from mid-gestation into early adulthood. Neuroscience 149, 582–591 (2007).
Fung, S. J. et al. Expression of interneuron markers in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the developing human and in schizophrenia. AJP 167, 1479–1488 (2010).
McNamara, N. B. et al. Microglia regulate central nervous system myelin growth and integrity. Nature 613, 120–129 (2023).
Santos, E. N. & Fields, R. D. Regulation of myelination by microglia. Sci. Adv. 7, eabk1131 (2021).
Miller, D. J. et al. Prolonged myelination in human neocortical evolution. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 16480–16485 (2012).
Grydeland, H., Walhovd, K. B., Tamnes, C. K., Westlye, L. T. & Fjell, A. M. Intracortical myelin links with performance variability across the human lifespan: results from T1- and T2-weighted MRI myelin mapping and diffusion tensor imaging. J. Neurosci. 33, 18618–18630 (2013).
Grydeland, H. et al. Waves of maturation and senescence in micro-structural MRI markers of human cortical myelination over the lifespan. Cereb. Cortex 29, 1369–1381 (2019).
Chugani, H. T., Phelps, M. E. & Mazziotta, J. C. Positron emission tomography study of human brain functional development. Ann. Neurol. 22, 487–497 (1987).
Takahashi, T., Shirane, R., Sato, S. & Yoshimoto, T. Developmental changes of cerebral blood flow and oxygen metabolism in children. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 20, 917–922 (1999).
Shan, Z. Y. et al. Cerebral glucose metabolism on positron emission tomography of children. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 2297–2309 (2013).
Satterthwaite, T. D. et al. Impact of puberty on the evolution of cerebral perfusion during adolescence. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 111, 8643–8648 (2014).
Weickert, C. S. et al. Postnatal alterations in dopaminergic markers in the human prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience 144, 1109–1119 (2007).
Jucaite, A., Forssberg, H., Karlsson, P., Halldin, C. & Farde, L. Age-related reduction in dopamine D1 receptors in the human brain: from late childhood to adulthood, a positron emission tomography study. Neuroscience 167, 104–110 (2010).
Johansson, J. et al. Biphasic patterns of age-related differences in dopamine D1 receptors across the adult lifespan. Cell Rep. 42, 113107 (2023).
Schliebs, R. & Arendt, T. The cholinergic system in aging and neuronal degeneration. Behav. Brain Res. 221, 555–563 (2011).
Liang, X. et al. Structural connectome architecture shapes the maturation of cortical morphology from childhood to adolescence. Nat. Commun. 15, 784 (2024).
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. & Norenzayan, A. The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 61–83 (2010).
Wei, Y., Zhang, H. & Liu, Y. Charting normative brain variability across the human lifespan. Neurosci. Bull. 39, 362–364 (2023).
Wong, A. P.-Y. et al. Inter-regional variations in gene expression and age-related cortical thinning in the adolescent brain. Cereb. Cortex 28, 1272–1281 (2018).
Kasper, J. et al. Resting-State Changes in Aging and Parkinson’s Disease Are Shaped by Underlying Neurotransmission: A Normative Modeling Study. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging, in press https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2024.04.010 (2024).
Harms, A. et al. Mesiotemporal volumetry, cortical thickness, and neuropsychological deficits in the long-term course of Limbic Encephalitis. Neurol. Neuroimmunol. Neuroinflamm. 10, e200125 (2023).
Bartels, F. et al. Clinical and magnetic resonance imaging outcome predictors in pediatric Anti–N-Methyl-D-Aspartate receptor Encephalitis. Ann. Neurol. 88, 148–159 (2020).
Marquand, A. F. et al. Conceptualizing mental disorders as deviations from normative functioning. Mol. Psychiatry 24, 1415–1424 (2019).
Panizzon, M. S. et al. Distinct genetic influences on cortical surface area and cortical thickness. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2728–2735 (2009).
Wierenga, L. M., Langen, M., Oranje, B. & Durston, S. Unique developmental trajectories of cortical thickness and surface area. NeuroImage 87, 120–126 (2014).
Di Biase, M. A. et al. Mapping human brain charts cross-sectionally and longitudinally. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 120, e2216798120 (2023).
Collins, M. A. et al. Accelerated cortical thinning precedes and predicts conversion to psychosis: The NAPLS3 longitudinal study of youth at clinical high-risk. Mol. Psychiatry 28, 1182–1189 (2023).
Lotter, L. D. & Dukart, J. JuSpyce - a toolbox for flexible assessment of spatial associations between brain maps. Zenodo https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.6884932 (2022).
Markello, R. D. et al. Standardizing workflows in imaging transcriptomics with the abagen toolbox. eLife 10, e72129 (2021).
Burt, J. B., Helmer, M., Shinn, M., Anticevic, A. & Murray, J. D. Generative modeling of brain maps with spatial autocorrelation. NeuroImage 220, 117038 (2020).
Virtanen, P. et al. SciPy 1.0: fundamental algorithms for scientific computing in Python. Nat. Methods 17, 261–272 (2020).
Salo, T. et al. neurostuff/NiMARE: 0.0.9rc2. Zenodo https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4895600 (2021).
Hunter, J. D. Matplotlib: A 2D Graphics environment. Comput. Sci. Eng. 9, 90–95 (2007).
Waskom, M. L. seaborn: statistical data visualization. J. Open Source Softw. 6, 3021 (2021).
Gale, D. J., Vos, de Wael, R., Benkarim, O. & Bernhardt, B. Surfplot: Publication-ready brain surface figures. Zenodo https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.5567926 (2021).
Marquand, A. F. et al. PCNToolkit. Zenodo https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.5207839 (2021).
Rutherford, S. et al. The Normative Modeling Framework for Computational Psychiatry. https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2021.08.08.455583v1 (2021).
Wey, H.-Y. et al. Insights into neuroepigenetics through human histone deacetylase PET imaging. Sci. Transl. Med. 8, 351ra106 (2016).
Kaulen, N. et al. mGluR5 and GABAA receptor-specific parametric PET atlas construction—PET/MR data processing pipeline, validation, and application. Hum. Brain Mapp. 43, 2148–2163 (2022).
Glasser, M. F. et al. A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature 536, 171–178 (2016).
Wu, J. et al. Accurate nonlinear mapping between MNI volumetric and FreeSurfer surface coordinate systems. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 3793–3808 (2018).
Wang, D. et al. Comprehensive functional genomic resource and integrative model for the human brain. Science 362, eaat8464 (2018).
Fischl, B. FreeSurfer. Neuroimage 62, 774–781 (2012).
Monereo-Sánchez, J. et al. Quality control strategies for brain MRI segmentation and parcellation: Practical approaches and recommendations - insights from the Maastricht study. NeuroImage 237, 118174 (2021).
Fraza, C. J., Dinga, R., Beckmann, C. F. & Marquand, A. F. Warped Bayesian linear regression for normative modelling of big data. NeuroImage 245, 118715 (2021).
Bayer, J. M. M. et al. Accommodating Site Variation in Neuroimaging Data Using Normative and Hierarchical Bayesian Models. https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2021.02.09.430363v2 (2021).
Kia, S. M. et al. Federated Multi-Site Normative Modeling Using Hierarchical Bayesian Regression. 2021.05.28.446120 https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2021.05.28.446120v1 (2021).
Rosen, A. F. G. et al. Quantitative assessment of structural image quality. NeuroImage 169, 407–418 (2018).
Dukart, J. et al. Cerebral blood flow predicts differential neurotransmitter activity. Sci. Rep. 8, 4074 (2018).
Markello, R. D. & Misic, B. Comparing spatial null models for brain maps. NeuroImage 236, 118052 (2021).
Lotter, L. D. Repository: Regional patterns of human cortex development correlate with underlying neurobiology. Zenodo https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7901282 (2024).
Pantano, P. et al. Regional cerebral blood flow and oxygen consumption in human aging. Stroke 15, 635–641 (1984).
Yamaguchi, T. et al. Reduction in regional cerebral metabolic rate of oxygen during human aging. Stroke 17, 1220–1228 (1986).
Nordberg, A., Alafuzoff, I. & Winblad, B. Nicotinic and muscarinic subtypes in the human brain: Changes with aging and dementia. J. Neurosci. Res. 31, 103–111 (1992).
Mesulam, M.-M. & Geula, C. Acetylcholinesterase-rich pyramidal neurons in the human neocortex and hippocampus: Absence at birth, development during the life span, and dissolution in Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 24, 765–773 (1988).
Acknowledgements
LDL was supported by the Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF) and the Max Planck Society (MPG), Germany. AS was funded by the Max Planck Society (Otto Hahn award) and Helmholtz Association’s Initiative and Networking Fund under the Helmholtz International Lab grant agreement InterLabs-0015, and the Canada First Research Excellence Fund (CFREF Competition 2, 2015–2016) awarded to the Healthy Brains, Healthy Lives initiative at McGill University, through the Helmholtz International BigBrain Analytics and Learning Laboratory (HIBALL). Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive DevelopmentSM (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10,000 children age 9-10 and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study® is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089, U24DA041123, U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/consortium_members/. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in the analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators. The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. The ABCD data used in this report came from https://doi.org/10.15154/1523041. This work received support from the following sources: the European Union-funded FP6 Integrated Project IMAGEN (Reinforcement-related behaviour in normal brain function and psychopathology) (LSHM-CT- 2007-037286), the Horizon 2020 funded ERC Advanced Grant “STRATIFY” (Brain network based stratification of reinforcement-related disorders) (695313), Human Brain Project (HBP SGA 2, 785907, and HBP SGA 3, 945539), the Medical Research Council Grant “c-VEDA” (Consortium on Vulnerability to Externalizing Disorders and Addictions) (MR/N000390/1), the National Institute of Health (NIH) (R01DA049238, A decentralized macro and micro gene-by-environment interaction analysis of substance use behavior and its brain biomarkers), the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London, the Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung (BMBF grants 01GS08152; 01EV0711; Forschungsnetz AERIAL 01EE1406A, 01EE1406B; Forschungsnetz IMAC-Mind 01GL1745B), the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG grants SM 80/7-2, SFB 940, TRR 265, NE 1383/14-1), the Medical Research Foundation and Medical Research Council (grants MR/R00465X/1 and MR/S020306/1), the National Institutes of Health (NIH) funded ENIGMA (grants 5U54EB020403-05 and 1R56AG058854-01), NSFC grant 82150710554 and European Union funded project “environMENTAL”, grant no: 101057429. Further support was provided by grants from: the ANR (ANR-12-SAMA-0004, AAPG2019 - GeBra), the Eranet Neuron (AF12-NEUR0008-01 - WM2NA; and ANR-18-NEUR00002-01 - ADORe), the Fondation de France (00081242), the Fondation pour la Recherche Médicale (DPA20140629802), the Mission Interministérielle de Lutte-contre-les-Drogues-et-les-Conduites-Addictives (MILDECA), the Assistance-Publique-Hôpitaux-de-Paris and INSERM (interface grant), Paris Sud University IDEX 2012, the Fondation de l’Avenir (grant AP-RM-17-013), the Fédération pour la Recherche sur le Cerveau; the National Institutes of Health, Science Foundation Ireland (16/ERCD/3797), U.S.A. (Axon, Testosterone and Mental Health during Adolescence; RO1 MH085772-01A1) and by NIH Consortium grant U54 EB020403, supported by a cross-NIH alliance that funds Big Data to Knowledge Centres of Excellence.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Consortia
Contributions
Conception and design: L.D.L., J.D., S.B.E. Data acquisition and provision: J.Y.H., B.M., C.P., T.B., F.N., G.J.B., A.L.W.B., S.D., H.F., A.G., H.G., P.G., A. Heinz, R.B., J.-L.M., M.-L. P., E.A., D.P.O., T.P., L.P., S.H., J. H.F., M.N.S., N.V., H.W., R.W., G.S., F. Nees, T., IMAGEN Consortium. Analysis and interpretation: L.D.L., A.S., C.P., J.D. Manuscript drafting: L.D.L., J.D. Manuscript revision: All authors. Supervision: J.D., S.B.E.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
Dr. Banaschewski served in an advisory or consultancy role for Lundbeck, Medice, Neurim Pharmaceuticals, Oberberg GmbH, and Shire. He received conference support or speaker’s fees from Lilly, Medice, Novartis, and Shire. He has been involved in clinical trials conducted by Shire and Viforpharma. He received royalties from Hogrefe, Kohlhammer, CIP Medien, and Oxford University Press. The present work is unrelated to the above grants and relationships. Dr. Barker has received honoraria from General Electric Healthcare for teaching on scanner programming courses. Dr. Poustka served in an advisory or consultancy role for Roche and Viforpharm and received speaker’s fees from Shire. She received royalties from Hogrefe, Kohlhammer, and Schattauer. The present work is unrelated to the above grants and relationships. All other authors report no biomedical financial interest or other potential conflicts of interest.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Communications thanks Yong He, Linn Norbom and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. A peer review file is available.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Lotter, L.D., Saberi, A., Hansen, J.Y. et al. Regional patterns of human cortex development correlate with underlying neurobiology. Nat Commun 15, 7987 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-024-52366-7
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-024-52366-7
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.