Abstract
Brain evolution has primarily been studied at the macroscopic level by comparing the relative size of homologous brain centers between species. How neuronal circuits change at the cellular level over evolutionary time remains largely unanswered. Here, using a phylogenetically informed framework, we compare the olfactory circuits of three closely related Drosophila species that differ in their chemical ecology: the generalists Drosophila melanogaster and Drosophila simulans and Drosophila sechellia that specializes on ripe noni fruit. We examine a central part of the olfactory circuit that, to our knowledge, has not been investigated in these species—the connections between projection neurons and the Kenyon cells of the mushroom body—and identify species-specific connectivity patterns. We found that neurons encoding food odors connect more frequently with Kenyon cells, giving rise to species-specific biases in connectivity. These species-specific connectivity differences reflect two distinct neuronal phenotypes: in the number of projection neurons or in the number of presynaptic boutons formed by individual projection neurons. Finally, behavioral analyses suggest that such increased connectivity enhances learning performance in an associative task. Our study shows how fine-grained aspects of connectivity architecture in an associative brain center can change during evolution to reflect the chemical ecology of a species.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Brain evolution has been primarily studied at the macroscopic level by comparing gross neuroanatomical features in homologous brain centers across distantly related species1,2,3. This pioneering work revealed that, over intermediate evolutionary timescales, the number of specialized brain centers does not change considerably, but the types and numbers of neurons forming these centers can vary greatly. Recent advances in comparative transcriptomics have provided new insights into the evolution and diversification of neurons, revealing how subtle variations in highly conserved regulatory gene networks can give rise to drastic changes in the rate at which neuronal progenitors proliferate or the types of neuron they give rise to4. To what degree such changes reflect selection pressures remains unclear. Moreover, how such changes manifest themselves at the level of neuronal circuits is not yet understood, as it remains technically challenging to delineate neuronal circuits in non-traditional model systems and to compare them across species with different evolutionary trajectories5.
Flies in the genus Drosophila have evolved to exploit a remarkable diversity of ecological niches6. The phylogeny of most Drosophila species has been resolved, revealing that even closely related Drosophila species can inhabit drastically different environments7. For instance, Drosophila sechellia, a species endemic to the Seychelles islands, is a specialist for noni—a toxic fruit that produces a distinctive bouquet of pungent acids—whereas its closest relative, Drosophila simulans, is a generalist and a human commensal that can be found in most cosmopolitan areas8,9,10,11 (Fig. 1a). A mere 0.1-0.25 million years of evolution separate these two species from their common ancestor, and only 3 million years separate their common ancestor from their well-studied relative, Drosophila melanogaster7. D. melanogaster is also a human commensal whose ecology largely overlaps with that of D. simulans12. These inverse relationships—phylogenetically, D. sechellia is closer to D. simulans while ecologically D. melanogaster and D. simulans are more alike—make this group of Drosophila highly suitable for investigating how neuronal circuits evolve over comparatively short timescales.
Olfactory driven responses used to locate food sources are likely one of the most important ways whereby a species can adapt behaviors to its ecological niche. In D. melanogaster, the majority of olfactory sensory neurons express only one receptor gene, and the axons of neurons expressing the same receptor gene(s) converge on a specific glomerulus in the antennal lobe, forming a stereotypical map13,14 (Fig. 1b). The genomes of D. melanogaster, D. simulans and D. sechellia harbor a comparable number of functional olfactory receptor genes, and homologous glomeruli can be identified in the antennal lobe in these species15,16,17. In D. sechellia, however, a few olfactory sensory neurons show species-specific responses to noni odors: Or22a-expressing neurons are preferentially activated by methyl esters whereas Ir75b-expressing neurons are most sensitive to hexanoic acid16,18,19,20,21. By contrast, in D. melanogaster, Or22a-expressing neurons are broadly tuned to different ethyl esters whereas Ir75b-expressing neurons are broadly tuned to shorter-chain acids16,18,19,20,21. These species-specific tuning properties are due to specific amino acid differences in the presumed ligand-binding domain of Or22a and Ir75b receptors16,21. In addition, there are two- to three-fold more Or22a- and Ir75b-expressing neurons in D. sechellia, and the glomeruli innervated by these neurons, DM2 and DL2d, respectively, are larger16,18,19. Other glomeruli—such as VM5d, which is innervated by Or85c/b-expressing neurons—are also larger in D. sechellia compared to D. melanogaster due to increased sensory neuron numbers16,19,20.
Whether and how the changes that occurred at the levels of receptor proteins and sensory neurons are reflected downstream, at the level of higher processing centers, is largely unknown.
Projection neurons innervating individual antennal lobe glomeruli transmit olfactory information to the mushroom body, an associative brain center, and the lateral horn, a center that mediates innate responses to odors22 (Fig. 1b). The projection neurons of the antennal lobe and the Kenyon cells of the mushroom body have been studied in detail in D. melanogaster. Each glomerulus type is innervated by a distinct, but largely stereotyped number of projection neurons, from one up to eight23. The mushroom body consists of about 2000 neurons, called Kenyon cells, that can be divided into three major types (α/β, α‘/β‘ and γ Kenyon cells); each Kenyon cell receives input from a small number of projection neurons, on average seven24,25. The connectivity architecture between projection neurons and Kenyon cells has been resolved, revealing two important principles: first, these connections are unstructured, in that individual Kenyon cells integrate inputs from a random set of projection neurons; second, some types of projection neuron connect more frequently to Kenyon cells than others, leading to a biased representation of glomeruli in the mushroom body25,26,27,28. These two connectivity patterns—randomization of input and biased connectivity—are genetically hardwired, suggesting that they might be shaped by selection pressures29.
Here, we show that randomization of sensory input and biased connectivity are conserved features of the mushroom body architecture, observed not only in D. melanogaster but also in related species such as D. simulans and D. sechellia. However, we found that the identity of the most biased projection neurons, which are either underrepresented or overrepresented, varies across species that occupy different ecological niches. Specifically, projection neurons detecting fermenting food odors are prioritized in the human commensals D. melanogaster and D. simulans, whereas projection neurons detecting noni are prioritized in D. sechellia. Additionally, our findings reveal that these differences in connectivity are attributed to morphological differences of the projection neurons connecting individual glomeruli to the mushroom body, either through changes in the number of projection neurons or through changes in the number of presynaptic boutons formed by individual neurons. Furthermore, our study suggests that increased connectivity to the mushroom body enhance learning performance in associative tasks.
Results
Randomization of input is conserved across Drosophila species
To compare features of the connectivity architecture between projection neurons and Kenyon cells in D. melanogaster, D. simulans and D. sechellia, we first resolved gross anatomical features of the antennal lobes of these species. Using confocal images of brains immuno-stained with a neuropil marker, we reconstructed entire antennal lobes by manually tracing the borders of individual glomeruli on single planes and projecting their volumes in three-dimensional space (Supplementary Fig. 1, Supplementary Data 1). The resulting projections preserved the shape, volume and location of all the glomeruli forming an antennal lobe. Each glomerulus was annotated using the well-characterized D. melanogaster antennal lobe map, which was generated using similar methods, as a reference13,14,30. Glomerular volumes were compared across species (Supplementary Data 1). We found that the antennal lobe map is largely conserved in the three species and contains a total of 51 glomeruli that can be recognized based on their shape and location. The volumes of most glomeruli are comparable across species with a few exceptions, including the noni-responsive DL2d, DM2 and VM5d glomeruli, which are larger in D. sechellia, as previously reported (DL2d: D. melanogaster: 1565 ± 334 μm3 (n = 3); D. simulans: 2007 ± 185 μm3 (n = 3); D. sechellia: 3066 ± 296 μm3 (n = 3); DM2: D. melanogaster: 3139 ± 227 μm3 (n = 3); D. simulans: 3169 ± 227 μm3 (n = 3); D. sechellia: 4594 ± 127 μm3 (n = 3); VM5d: D. melanogaster: 905 ± 115 μm3 (n = 3); D. simulans: 1773 ± 127 μm3 (n = 3); D. sechellia: 3793 ± 153 μm3 (n = 3))16,17,19,21. Despite these volume differences, the antennal lobes of the three species investigated are macroscopically nearly identical, which is in line with the notion that gross anatomy is conserved over the fairly short evolutionary distances separating the three species.
We next set out to compare the global connectivity architecture of the mushroom body across the three species by adapting the technique we previously developed to map projection neuron–Kenyon cell connections in D. melanogaster26 (Fig. 1c). For each species, individual Kenyon cells were photo-labeled in flies carrying the broad neuronal driver nSynaptobrevin-GAL4 and a UAS-photoactivatable-GFP effector transgene. In D. melanogaster, α/β, α‘/β‘ and γ Kenyon form a variable number of claw-shaped dendritic terminals. α/β, α‘/β‘ and γ Kenyon cells were found in all three species, appear morphologically indistinguishable from one another and form on average a comparable number of claw-shaped dendritic terminals24 (Supplementary Fig. 2). To identify the projection neurons connected to a photo-labeled Kenyon cell, a red-dye was electroporated sequentially in most of the claw-shaped dendritic terminals formed by that cell (Fig. 1c, Supplementary Fig. 3). Using this technique, the inputs of hundreds of Kenyon cells were identified in terms of the glomeruli from which they originate. We reported these results in a connectivity matrix that summarizes the glomerular inputs to 200 Kenyon cells. Statistical analyses of the resulting matrix can be used to reveal structured patterns of connectivity, such as whether groups of glomeruli are preferentially connected to the same Kenyon cells or whether projection neuron–Kenyon cell connections are random and biased26,29. We generated a total of four such connectivity matrices (Fig. 1d): two using D. melanogaster males and females (a total of 687 and 704 connections, respectively), one using D. simulans females (717 connections) and one using D. sechellia females (692 connections).
We performed an unbiased search for potential structural features in these connectivity matrices using principal component analysis. The variance associated with individual principal component projections provides a sensitive measure of structure as we have previously shown26,29. We extracted correlations within a given experimental connectivity matrix and compared them to correlations extracted from matrices in which connections were randomly shuffled. In these shuffled matrices, the total number of connections between projection neurons and Kenyon cells is the same as the number of connections reported in each experimental matrix, but the connections were randomly assigned (1000 uniform shuffle matrices). We also generated a second set of shuffle matrices in which the connections were scrambled but the frequencies at which projection neurons from each glomerulus connect to Kenyon cells were fixed to reflect the frequencies measured experimentally (1000 biased shuffle matrices). We found that the observed spectrum of variances is not significantly different to that of the biased shuffle matrices, but does significantly deviate from that of the uniform shuffle matrices (Fig. 2a, b). These results suggest that there are no detectable structural features in the experimental matrices other than the structure generated by the biases in connectivity frequencies.
Biases in connectivity correlate with the chemical ecology of a species
If the connections between projection neurons and Kenyon cells were completely random, we would expect Kenyon cells to integrate input uniformly across glomeruli, and each glomerulus to have a connectivity frequency of about 2%. Yet, we found that the connections between projection neurons and Kenyon cells are biased: some glomeruli are overrepresented and have a connectivity frequency significantly higher than 2%, whereas some glomeruli are underrepresented and have a connectivity frequency significantly lower than 2% (Supplementary Fig. 4). The result is a non-uniform distribution of connectivity frequencies. We found that there are between nine and 12 overrepresented glomeruli (glomeruli with connectivity frequencies higher than 2%, p-value < 0.05) and between 10 and 13 underrepresented glomeruli (glomeruli with connectivity frequencies lower than 2%, p-value < 0.05) within each connectivity matrix. This result shows that the non-uniform distribution of connectivity frequencies is a feature present across species.
To compare the overall extent of bias in the distributions of connectivity frequencies derived from the connectivity matrices, we inferred their Jensen-Shannon distances. This statistical method measures the similarity of two probability distributions, with a distance of zero indicating identical distributions. To gauge the extent to which the Jensen-Shannon distance indicates overall similarity of the observed distributions in connectivity frequencies, we compared the distributions of connectivity frequencies measured in the experimental matrices to those measured using the corresponding uniform shuffle matrices and obtained distances ranging from 0.23 to 0.27; when we compared the distributions of connectivity frequencies measured in the experimental matrices to those measured using the biased shuffle matrices, we obtained distances ranging from 0.08 to 0.09 (Supplementary Fig. 5). When we compared the distributions of connectivity frequencies measured using two D. melanogaster female matrices—one matrix was generated in this study and the other was generated in a previous study29—we obtained a relatively short distance of 0.17. Likewise, we obtained a relatively short distance of 0.15 when we compared the distributions measured in D. melanogaster females and males (Fig. 2c). This result suggests that the biases in connectivity are largely similar in both sexes. The distances measured when comparing D. melanogaster and D. simulans range from 0.16 to 0.17, indicating that the overall extent of bias in connectivity is similar in these species. By contrast, the distances between D. melanogaster and D. sechellia range from 0.20 to 0.22, whereas the distance between D. simulans and D. sechellia is 0.24, showing that the overall extent of biases in connectivity is higher in D. sechellia than in its sibling species. As D. sechellia and D. simulans are phylogenetically more closely related to each other than to D. melanogaster, the observed pattern suggests that the overall similarity in biases is not a function of evolutionary relatedness.
We next investigated whether the larger Jensen-Shannon distances measured for D. sechellia result from small differences in connectivity frequencies distributed across glomeruli or whether they result from large differences restricted to a few glomeruli. We first performed pairwise comparisons between D. sechellia and the generalists by using the ratio of connectivity frequencies obtained for a given glomerulus in the biased shuffle data sets (connectivity frequency measured in the D. sechellia matrix divided by the connectivity frequency measured in the biased shuffle data sets of all generalists) (Fig. 2d, e). We found that most glomeruli are connected to Kenyon cells at similar frequencies across all data sets but that a few glomeruli stand out when comparing D. sechellia to the generalists: the DL2d and DP1l glomeruli are significantly more connected in D. sechellia than in the generalists whereas the DC3 and VM5v glomeruli are significantly less connected in D. sechellia than in the generalists. To confirm these findings, we then performed pairwise comparisons and measured the ratio of connectivity frequencies obtained for a given glomerulus using the experimental data sets (connectivity frequency measured in matrix 1 divided by the connectivity frequency measured in matrix 2) (Fig. 2f). We could confirm that the DL2d, DP1l, DC3 and VM5v glomeruli show significant differences in their connectivity rates when comparing D. sechellia to one or both of the generalists.
The DL2d and DP1l glomeruli—which receive input from the acid-sensing Ir75b- and Ir75a-expressing neurons, respectively—were found to be connected at higher frequencies in D. sechellia than in one of the other two species (DL2d connectivity frequencies: D. sechellia: 3.19%, D. simulans: 0.68%, p-value < 0.01, D. melanogaster: 1.02%, p-value < 0.05; DP1l: D. sechellia: 3.19%, D. melanogaster: 0.44%, p-value < 0.001, D. simulans: 0.95%, p-value < 0.05). The DC3 and VM5v glomeruli were found to be connected at lower frequencies in D. sechellia than in one or both species. These glomeruli receive input from olfactory sensory neurons tuned to various fruit volatiles: the Or83c-expressing neurons associated with the DC3 glomerulus are narrowly tuned to farnesol, an odor made by yeast and citrus fruits, whereas the neurons associated with the VM5v (Or98a-expressing neurons) glomeruli are broadly tuned to alcohols and esters produced by fermenting fruits13,14,31,32,33 (connectivity frequencies: DC3: D. sechellia: 1.37%, D. melanogaster: 4.50%, p-value < 0.001, D. simulans: 4.91%, p-value < 0.001; VM5v: D. sechellia: 1.82%, D. simulans: 5.05%, p-value < 0.001, D. melanogaster: 3.36%, p-value < 0.05).
Overall, these results show that differences in connectivity frequencies are species-specific, not sexually dimorphic, and restricted to a fraction of olfactory channels. The channels that differ the most relay information about food odors to the mushroom body, suggesting that the representation ethologically relevant information in the mushroom body might change as species diverged. Most notably, the connectivity frequency measured for a given glomerulus changes in the D. sechellia lineage, being more similar in D. melanogaster and D. simulans than when comparing any of these species to D. sechellia. As D. sechellia is more closely related to D. simulans than to D. melanogaster, this pattern appears not to be a function of phylogenetic relatedness, but instead of ecological relatedness. To better visualize this notion, we generated a hierarchical clustering dendrogram based on the connectivity frequencies measured in the experimental matrices (Fig. 2g). In this dendrogram, each line represents a glomerulus, and the connectivity frequency measured in a given species for that glomerulus is depicted as a color gradation. Glomeruli were linked into clusters based on the similarity of their connectivity frequencies across species. In the tree constructed based on this analysis, D. melanogaster and D. simulans cluster and D. sechellia stands out as an outlier. This observation shows that changes in glomerular representation in the mushroom body are highly correlated with the ecology of a species, not its phylogeny, and, therefore, could have evolved in D. sechellia as this species diverged to exploit noni fruit.
Two morphological features of projection neurons underlie shifts in biases
From our analyses of the above data sets, we observed two types of change in the mushroom body connectivity architecture of D. sechellia when compared to that of D. melanogaster and D. simulans: the representation of the DL2d and DP1l glomeruli increases and the representation of the DC3 and VM5v glomerulus decreases. In D. melanogaster, it is known that biases in connectivity are a function of the overall number of presynaptic boutons formed in the mushroom body by the projection neurons associated with a given glomerulus25,26,28. Thus, changes in glomerular representation across species could result from changes in the number of projection neurons associated with a given glomerulus, changes in the number of presynaptic boutons individual projection neurons form or both. To determine whether any of these cases prevail, we photo-labeled (Fig. 3a) and dye-labeled (Fig. 4a) the projection neurons innervating a given glomerulus to quantify the number of neurons and measure the volume of the presynaptic boutons the labeled neurons occupy in the mushroom body.
The DL2d and DP1l glomeruli are more frequently connected to Kenyon cells in D. sechellia (Fig. 2d, Supplementary Data 2). We identified eight DL2d projection neurons in D. sechellia (five neurons located in the anterior-dorsal cluster, or adDL2d neurons, and three neurons located in the ventral cluster, or vDL2d neurons) but only six DL2d projection neurons in D. melanogaster and D. simulans (five adDL2d neurons and one vDL2d neuron in both species) (Table 1, Fig. 3b, c). The number of DL2d projection neurons identified in D. melanogaster is consistent with the number of projection neurons reported in the available connectomes of the D. melanogaster brain23 (Supplementary Fig. 6). In all species, the vDL2d neurons bypass the mushroom body and project only to the lateral horn. Therefore, the number of neurons connecting the DL2d glomerulus to the mushroom body is the same in all species. However, collectively, the adDL2d neurons show larger bouton volume in D. sechellia than they do in the other species (bouton volume of all adDL2d neurons: D. melanogaster: 112.18 ± 12.41; D. simulans: 104.44 ± 25.93; D. sechellia: 348.29 ± 186.63; Table 1, Fig. 3d). We found that the increased number of connections between adDL2d neurons and Kenyon cells in D. sechellia is due to more presynaptic boutons being formed per neuron (Fig. 4b, c, Supplementary Table 1). We also found that individual adDL2d neurons show more complex branching patterns based on several quantifiable parameters (Fig. 4c, Supplementary Table 1). We identified a similar phenotype for the projection neurons associated with the DP1l glomerulus: there are three DP1l projection neurons in D. sechellia (one neuron located in the lateral cluster, or lDP1l neuron, and two vDP1l neurons) but only two DP1l projection neurons in D. melanogaster (one lDP1l neuron and one vDP1l neuron) as it has been reported in the connectomes23 (Table 1, Supplementary Fig. 7). As with the vDL2d neurons, the vDP1l neurons bypass the mushroom body and only the lDP1l neurons connect to Kenyon cells. We found that for lDP1l neurons the bouton volume is larger in D. sechellia than in D. melanogaster and D. simulans (bouton volume of the lDP1l neuron: D. melanogaster: 244.39 ± 69.00; D. simulans: 232.41 ± 100.73; D. sechellia: 388.91 ± 75.88).
The DC3 and VM5v glomeruli are less frequently connected to Kenyon cells in D. sechellia (Fig. 2d, Supplementary Data 2). We identified two DC3 projection neurons in D. sechellia (two adDC3 neurons) but as many as three in D. melanogaster (three adDC3 neurons as reported in the connectomes23) and six in D. simulans (three adDC3 neurons and three vDC3 neurons) (Table 1, Fig. 3b, c, Supplementary Fig. 8). In D. simulans, the vDC3 neurons bypass the mushroom body and project only to the lateral horn. The volume of presynaptic boutons collectively formed by the adDC3 neurons in the mushroom body is significantly smaller in D. sechellia than in D. melanogaster and D. simulans (bouton volume of all adDC3 neurons: D. melanogaster: 370.15 ± 195.68; D. simulans: 364.06 ± 146.60; D. sechellia: 233.02 ± 83.80; Table 1, Fig. 3d). However, individual adDC3 projection neurons are morphologically similar across species with no significant differences in branch length, number of forks or bouton number (bouton volume of individual adDC3 neuron: D. melanogaster: 184.15 ± 34.60; D. simulans: 169.01 ± 43.23; D. sechellia: 165.36 ± 25.56; Fig. 4b, c, Supplementary Table 1). Thus, the decrease in the number of connections between adDC3 neurons and Kenyon cells in D. sechellia is due to a decrease in number of projection neurons innervating the DC3 glomerulus in that species. We identified a similar phenotype for the projection neurons associated with the VM5v glomerulus: there are two adVM5v projection neurons in D. sechellia but as many as three adVM5v projection neurons in D. melanogaster, as reported in the connectomes23, and three adVM5v projection neurons in D. simulans (Table 1, Fig. 3b, c, Supplementary Fig. 6). We found that, as for the adDC3 projection neurons, the adVM5v neurons form collectively a smaller bouton volume in D. sechellia but, individually, the VM5v neurons are similar across species (bouton volume of all VM5v neurons: D. melanogaster: 256.78 ± 104.09; D. simulans: 268.43 ± 132.67; D. sechellia: 140.14 ± 77.76; Table 1; bouton volume of individual VM5v neurons: D. melanogaster: 116.45 ± 18.68; D. simulans: 85.38 ± 17.48; D. sechellia: 169.73 ± 39.67; Fig. 4b, c, Supplementary Table 1). Thus, as with the adDC3 neurons, there are fewer VM5v neurons in D. sechellia, and, therefore, fewer connections between the VM5v glomerulus and Kenyon cells. Combined with the observations made for the DL2d and DP1l projection neurons, these results suggest that increases in glomerular representation in D. sechellia occur through increases in bouton number, whereas decreases in glomerular representation occur through decreases in the number of projection neurons associated with different glomeruli. These observations further suggest that selection pressures may influence shifts in glomerular representation through at least two different types of molecular mechanism — those regulating synaptogenesis and those regulating neurogenesis — and that these mechanisms could be glomerular specific.
We extended our anatomical analyses beyond the DL2d, DP1l, DC3 and VM5v projection neurons. We did not detect significant interspecific differences for the VA1d, VC3 and VL2a projection neurons although we identified these glomeruli as differently represented in at least one of the species investigated (Table 1, Supplementary Fig. 7). We have also performed anatomical analyses on neurons innervating glomeruli that we identified as identically represented in all three species, namely the DA2, DM2 and DP1m glomeruli. We could not detect any noticeable differences in the axonal termini that these projection neurons extend in the mushroom body (Table 1, Supplementary Fig. 7). Although our photo-labeling and dye-labeling techniques might not fully capture the extent of morphological differences that exist between the species investigated, our results suggest that our approach is sufficient to identify the most significant morphological features that underlie shifts in connectivity across species. Altogether, our results suggest that interspecific differences in the mushroom body connectivity architecture are specific and restricted to a small number of projection neurons.
Connectivity frequencies and learning performance
We tested whether the changes in glomerular representation we observed in the mushroom body of the species correlates with differences in learning performance by using a well-established behavioral paradigm34. In this paradigm, flies were conditioned to associate a stimulus—either air perfumed with an odor diluted in mineral oil or pure mineral oil—with electric shocks, and their preference for the conditioned stimulus was subsequently tested in a T-maze (Supplementary Fig. 8). In short, we measured the number of flies seeking out the conditioned stimulus over the number of flies seeking out the unconditioned stimulus to derive a Performance Index. We used either a protocol that included a single regimen of electric shocks (single training) or a protocol that included six spaced regimens of electric shocks (spaced training). In a first series of experiments, we tested odors known to activate the glomeruli whose representation shifts most drastically across species: the DC3 and DL2d glomeruli. Farnesol, strongly and selectively activates the OR83c-expressing neurons in D. melanogaster32 (Supplementary Table 2). The OR83c-expressing neurons project to the DC3 glomerulus, and the adDC3 projection neurons connect to Kenyon cells at a much higher frequency in D. melanogaster and D. simulans than in D. sechellia (Fig. 2e, f, Supplementary Table 2). We found that farnesol could not trigger strong learned responses in any of the species in either the single or spaced training protocol (Fig. 5a, b, Supplementary Fig. 9). Hexanoic acid, a noni odor, weakly activates multiple types of olfactory sensory neuron in D. melanogaster but strongly activates the IR75b-expressing neurons in D. sechellia21,33 (Supplementary Table 2). The IR75b-expressing neurons project to the DL2d glomerulus, and the adDL2d projection neurons connect with Kenyon cells at a high frequency in D. sechellia; the projection neurons associated with the glomeruli activated by hexanoic acid collectively connect at a similarly high frequency in D. melanogaster and D. simulans (Supplementary Table 2). We found that hexanoic acid can trigger learned responses in D. melanogaster and D. simulans when using either the single or spaced training protocol but not in D. sechellia where weak learned responses of much smaller amplitudes are only observed when using the spaced training protocol (Fig. 5a, b, Supplementary Fig. 9). These results suggest that activation of a single glomerulus is insufficient to elicit robust learned responses in this species, and that the activation of multiple glomeruli that collectively connect to Kenyon cells at high frequencies might be required for learning.
We thus set out to test odors known to activate multiple types of olfactory sensory neuron in D. melanogaster, and therefore multiple glomeruli: 4-methylcyclohexanol and 3-octanol, which are detected by at least three and nine different types of olfactory sensory neuron, respectively31,33 (Supplementary Table 2). We found that both odors triggered strong learned responses in D. melanogaster using either the single or spaced training protocol (Fig. 5a, b, Supplementary Fig. 9). We noticed that the magnitude of the Performance Index varies significantly across odors: 3-octanol, which activates the largest number of glomeruli, gave rise to larger Performance Indices than 4-methycyclohexanol. To determine whether differences in connectivity frequencies could underlie the amplitude of the Performance Indices, we plotted the cumulative connectivity frequencies of the glomeruli known to be activated by each odor in D. melanogaster against the Performance Indices measured with a given protocol (Fig. 5c, d). We observed a strong positive correlation, suggesting that odors activating a large number of highly connected glomeruli are more learnable than odors activating one or a few glomeruli. We performed similar analyses on D. simulans and D. sechellia and observed similar correlations. It is important to note that the cumulative connectivity frequencies were calculated using the odor responses measured in D. melanogaster, with the exception of hexanoic acid for which recordings collected in D. simulans and D. sechellia are available21. Interestingly, the Performance Indices also vary across species although all species display equally strong avoidance to electric shocks (Supplementary Fig. 10): D. melanogaster performed better than D. simulans and remarkably better than D. sechellia regardless of the protocol used, whereas D. sechellia was only able to learn when we used the spaced training protocol (Fig. 5c, d). These differences could reflect the fact that this training paradigm was originally developed and optimized for D. melanogaster; our data set indeed represents one of the first comparative study investigating differences in learning performance across Drosophila species. These differences could also reflect the fact that D. sechellia is impaired in its ability to synthesize dopamine, a neurotransmitter essential for learning35. The poor ability of D. sechellia to learn may reveal a relaxed requirement for this species to form associations with a wide range of stimuli as, unlike generalists, its survival depends on a single resource, noni. To test whether D. sechellia can learn any odor, we trained flies with an odor predicted to activate a large number of glomeruli that are connected to Kenyon cells at high frequencies in D. sechellia, namely 2,3-butanedione and isopentyl acetate using the single training protocol (Supplementary Fig. 11, Supplementary Table 2). These odors elicited robust learning in all species suggesting that D. sechellia are capable of learning when a large number of projection neurons are recruited. Altogether, these results suggest that increased connectivity between projection neurons and Kenyon cells enhances learning performance.
Discussion
In this study, we harness a phylogenetically and ecologically informed approach to pinpoint differences in neuronal connectivity architecture and learning performance between three species of Drosophila. The species differ in the frequency with which inputs from a small set of olfactory channels—mainly those relaying information about food odors—are represented among the overall input to a higher-order processing center, the mushroom body. Notably, most of these differences are found in the specialist species D. sechellia, suggesting they are due to an ecological niche shift, rather than merely a function of phylogenetic distance. Evolutionary differences in sensory representation are caused by morphological alterations of the projection neurons connecting individual glomeruli to the mushroom body, either through changes in the number of projection neurons or through changes in the number of presynaptic boutons per neuron. Our study also suggests that increased connectivity might enhance learning performance in an associative task.
While we have identified clear species-specific connectivity patterns, it is likely that other, more nuanced features of connectivity architecture underlie the evolution of the mushroom body in Drosophila species. A recent electron microscopy-based study of the D. melanogaster mushroom body has revealed a subtle but significant connectivity pattern between a subgroup of projection neurons and the α/β and α’/β’ Kenyon cells, but our mapping technique is not sensitive enough to capture this particular feature of connectivity architecture28,29. Likewise, our study has limitations regarding the conclusions that can be drawn about the forces driving the evolution of connectivity architecture in the mushroom body. For instance, it remains unclear whether the neuronal connectivity traits identified are variable across individuals in natural populations, especially those inhabiting different environments. It remains equally unclear whether the anatomical and behavioral changes that we observed confer a fitness advantage and could be adaptive. The fact that we were able to correlate a visible neuronal phenotype with these connectivity changes and correlate connectivity frequencies with learning performance, makes such investigations theoretically possible in the future. Another important limitation of our study is the assumption that olfactory sensory neurons have the same detection capabilities across all species. However, a recent study indicates that the activity patterns evoked by odors in the antennal lobe of the species we investigated, while not identical, are largely conserved17.
Despite these limitations, our study contributes new insights into the emergence of behavioral adaptations. Behavioral adaptations arise from modifications in the way neuronal circuits process information, and it is known that different cellular mechanisms can give rise to such modifications5,36,37. For instance, changes in the expression levels of receptors or ion channels can alter the electrophysiological properties of neurons and, consequently, neuronal output. There are many documented examples of such evolutionary changes at the periphery, including in the Drosophila olfactory systems, showing that sensory systems can adapt by finely tuning their detection capabilities to features of the environment peculiar to a species5,36. Changes in neuronal connectivity, whether through changes in synaptic weights between existing partners or through the formation of new synaptic partners, can also alter the way information flows in a circuit. In principle, such modifications can occur at any level within a circuit. However—perhaps because neurons at the periphery are more accessible than neurons embedded in the higher brain centers—there is a predominance of documented functional changes in sensory neurons over connectivity changes in central nervous systems. Our study provides the first evidence at the cellular level for such evolutionary changes in the connectivity architecture of higher olfactory brain centers and how these changes might enhance cognitive functions.
While it is understood that behaviors and their underlying neuronal circuits evolve through various mechanisms, our limited examples of these mechanisms hinder our capacity to determine if some are more common than others. Namely, how higher brain centers evolve remains, for the most part, completely unknown. Higher brain centers often feature more integrated circuits with many connections, and therefore it is conceivable that they might be less liable to change during evolution due to the disruptive impacts such changes might have across circuits. The anatomical changes we observed in this study are comparatively subtle, involving changes in the quantity of neuronal connections formed while leaving the overall random circuit architecture intact. Theoretical studies have demonstrated that randomization of input enables mushroom body-like networks to generate a representation space of high dimensionality, where many odors can be represented by non-overlapping neuronal ensembles38,39,40. We propose that biases in connectivity enable the mushroom body to better represent ecologically meaningful odors by increasing the coding space allocated to these odors, potentially increasing the fitness of an animal. Shifts in connectivity biases emphasize information streams that are most relevant to an animal without completely inactivating or adding de novo streams. This evolutionary pattern may be widespread across brain centers and species.
Methods
Fly stocks and husbandry
Flies were reared under standard conditions (25 °C, 60% humidity) in incubators that maintain a 12 h light/12 h dark cycle (Percival Scientific Inc, Cat#DR36VL); D. melanogaster and D. simulans flies were reared on standard cornmeal agar medium, whereas D. sechellia flies were reared on standard cornmeal agar medium that was supplemented with noni juice (Healing Noni). The stocks used and their sources were as follows: D. melanogaster: w1118;;; (Bloomington Stock Center, 5905), yw;[N-Synaptobrevin-GAL4]2.1;; (J. Simpson, University of California, Santa Barbara)24 and y1,w1118;[10xUAS-IVS-Syn21-mC3PA-GFP-p10]attP40;; (Axel laboratory, Columbia University)24; D. sechellia: w[N-Synaptobrevin-GAL4, w*]3P3-RFP-DEL;;; and w[UAS-C3PA-GFP, w*]3P3-RFP-DELA;;; (Benton laboratory, University of Lausanne)16; D. simulans: attp2176 and attp2178 (Stern laboratory, Janelia Farm Research Campus)41.
Transgenesis
A N-synaptobrevin-GAL4 (D. simulans) plasmid was generated by cloning a 1.9 kb sequence upstream of N-synaptobrevin using PCR and genomic DNA extracted from D. simulans attp2176 flies (forward oligonucleotide: GATCGGTACCGAACTCGTCCTCAAAGATGGAAACAGAG; reverse oligonucleotide: GATCGCGGCCGCGAATTCGGCTGGCGATGATTAGGATG); the amplified sequence was inserted into the pGal4attB plasmid using the NotI and KpnI restriction sites; this N-synaptobrevin-GAL4 (D. simulans) plasmid was injected into the D. simulans attp2176 strain with the φC31 integrase following a standard protocol (BestGene) resulting in the y1w1; pBac{3XP3::EYFP, N-synaptobrevin-GAL4}attp2176;; transgenic line42. The y1w1;;pBac{3XP3::EYFP, UAS-C3PA-GFP}attp2178 transgenic line was generated by injecting a UAS-C3PA-GFP plasmid into the D. simulans attp2178 strain using similar protocols43.
Reconstructing antennal lobes
Antennal lobes were reconstructed from confocal images of immuno-stained brains. The brains of flies were dissected at room temperature in a phosphate buffered saline solution or PBS (Sigma-Aldrich, P5493), fixed in 2% paraformaldehyde (Electron Microscopy Sciences, 15710) for either 45 min (D. melanogaster) or 35 min (D. simulans and D. sechellia) at room temperature, washed five times in PBST (PBS with 1x Triton, Sigma-Aldrich, T8787) at room temperature, blocked with 5% goat Serum (Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories) in PBST for 30 min at room temperature, and incubated in a solution that contained the primary antibody (1:20 in 5% Goat Serum/PBST, Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, nc82, AB 2314866) at 4°C overnight. On the following day, brains were washed four times in PBST and incubated in a solution that contained the secondary antibody (1:500 in 5% Goat Serum/PBST, Thermal Fisher, goat anti-mouse Alexa Fluor 488, AB 2576217) at 4°C overnight. On the following day, brains were washed four times in PBST and mounted on a slide (Fisher Scientific, 12-550-143) using the mounting media VECTASHIELD (Vector Laboratories Inc., H-1000). Immuno-stained brains were imaged using an LSM 880 confocal microscope (Zeiss). Each antennal lobe was reconstructed from a confocal image using the segmentation software Amira (FEI Visualization Sciences Group, version 2020.3.1). Individual glomeruli were reconstructed via manual segmentation: boundaries were demarcated by hand and interpolated. Glomeruli were assigned identities according to their position based on the available anatomical maps and the D. melanogaster hemibrain connectome v1.2.113,14,30,44. Glomerular volumes were calculated from the reconstructed voxel size, and the sum of those volumes were used to calculate whole antennal lobe volumes. We identified a total of 51 glomeruli in the antennal lobe reconstructions but only 49 in the mapping experiments used to generate the connectivity matrices. This is because VC3 is split into two glomeruli — VC3m and VC3l — in the reconstructions but when scoring matrices, we could not distinguish VC3m from VC3l.
Photo-labeling projection neurons and Kenyon cells
Neurons were photo-labeled based on a previously published protocol45. In short, brains were dissected in saline (108 mM NaCl, 5 mM KCl, 5 mM HEPES, 5 mM Trehalose, 10 mM Sucrose, 1 mM NaH2PO4, 4 mM NaHCO3, 2 mM CaCl2, 4 mM MgCl2, pH≈7.3), treated for 1 min with 2 mg/ml collagenase (Sigma-Aldrich) and mounted on a piece of SYLGARD placed at the bottom of a Petri dish. Each brain was either mounted with its anterior side facing upward (for photo-labeling projection neurons) or with its posterior side facing upward (for photo-labeling Kenyon cells). The photo-labeling and image acquisition steps were performed using a two-photon laser scanning microscope (Bruker, Ultima) with an ultrafast Chameleon Ti:Sapphire laser (Coherent) modulated by Pockels Cells (Conotopics). During the photo-labeling step, the laser was tuned to 710 nm and about 5 to 30 mW of laser power was used; during the image acquisition step, the laser was tuned to 925 nm and about 1 to 14 mW of laser power was used. Both power values were measured behind the objective lens. A 60X water-immersion objective lens (Olympus) was used for both photo-labeling and image acquisition. A GaAsP detector (Hamamatsu Photonics) was used for measuring green fluorescence. Photo-labeling was performed by drawing a region of interest—on average 1.0 ×1.0 µm—either in the center of the targeted glomerulus (for labeling projection neurons) or in the center of the soma (for labeling Kenyon cells). Photo-labeling projection neurons: Photoactivation was achieved through two to four cycles of exposure to 710-nm laser light, during which each pixel was scanned four times, with 25 repetitions per cycle, and 15 min rest period between each cycle. Image acquisition was performed with the laser tuned to 925 nm at a resolution of 512 by 512 pixels with a pixel size of 0.39 μm and a pixel dwell time of 4 μs; each pixel was scanned twice. A minimum of five samples per species were analyzed for each type of projection neuron. Photo-labeling Kenyon cells: Photoactivation was achieved through three to five single scans with the laser tuned to 710 nm, during which each pixel was scanned eight times. Before image acquisition, a 10 min rest period was implemented to allow diffusion of the photoactivated fluorophore within the neuron. Image acquisition was performed at a resolution of 512 by 512 pixels with a pixel size of 0.39 μm and a pixel dwell time of 4 μs; each pixel was scanned 2 times.
Mapping Kenyon cell input connections using dye electroporation
The projection neurons connecting to a photo-labeled Kenyon cell were identified based on previously published protocols26,29. See Supplementary Fig. 3 for a schematic depicting the procedure. In short, electrodes were made by pulling borosilicate glass pipette with filament (Sutter Instruments, BF100-50-10) to a resistance of 9-11 MΩ, fire-polished using a micro-forge (Narishige) to narrow their opening and backfilled with 100 mg/ml 3000-Da Texas-dextran dye (Thermo-Fisher, D3328). Under the guidance of a two-photon microscope (Bruker, Ultima), an electrode was centered into the post-synaptic terminal—or claw—of a photo-labeled Kenyon cell using a motorized micromanipulator (Sutter Instruments). Short current pulses (each 10-50 V in amplitude and 0.5 millisecond long) were applied until the projection neuron connecting to the targeted Kenyon cell claw was visible. Not all the projection neurons connecting to a given Kenyon cells were dye-filled but on average 4 ± 1 of the claws formed by a given Kenyon cell were dye-filled. An image of the antennal lobe was acquired at the end of the procedure. Dye-labeled glomeruli were identified based on their shape, position and the location of their soma as defined in the available anatomical maps and the Drosophila melanogaster hemibrain connectome v1.2.113,14,30,44.
The selection of D. melanogaster male connectivity matrices for this study was driven by specific methodological considerations. The transgenes used for the mapping technique, nSynaptobrevin-GAL4 and UAS-photoactivatable-GFP, are both located on the X chromosome in D. sechellia, limiting the analysis to female flies. Initially, the reference matrix was the male D. melanogaster connectivity matrix from Caron et al. (2013) Nature. To ensure consistency and comparability, a matrix for male D. melanogaster was first generated. To assess any potential sexual dimorphism in connectivity biases, a female D. melanogaster was generated and compared to the male matrices. No meaningful differences were found and therefore species-comparisons were performed using female matrices only.
Dye-labeling individual projection neurons
Individual projection neurons were dye-labeled using a previously published protocol16,43. The cell body of the projection neuron of interest was first identified by lightly photo-labeling all the projection neurons innervating a given glomerulus by performing a single cycle of exposure to 710 nm light. After a rest period of 10 min, an unpolished electrode filled with Texas Red dextran dye was attached to the cell body of one of the photo-labeled projection neurons, and the dye was electroporated into the neuron using short current pulses; each pulse was 10 to 30 V in amplitude and 0.5 millisecond long. A resting period of about 30 min allowed the dye to diffuse throughout the neuron. Image acquisition was performed at a resolution of 512 by 512 pixels with a pixel size of 0.39 μm and a pixel dwell time of 4 μs; each pixel was scanned two times. A minimum of five samples per species were analyzed for each type of projection neuron.
Quantifying morphological features of projection neurons
Representative images of projection neurons were projected at maximal intensity using the ImageJ/Fiji software46 (National Institutes of Health). Projection neurons were counted based on the number of photo-labeled cell bodies observed in the anterior or lateral or ventral clusters of the antennal lobe. Primary branches were defined as processes that emerge from the main axonal projection that traverses the calyx of the mushroom body. The length of the branches formed by a projection neuron and the number of forks were quantified using the Simple Neurite Tracer plugin for ImageJ/Fiji software, while the surface area of the axonal arbors in the mushroom body calyx and lateral horn was calculated using the ROI Manager and Measure features of this software47. Total projection neuron bouton volume for a given sample was measured using Fluorender (University of Utah Scientific Computing and Imaging Institute; version 2.26.247,48): boutons were traced using the Paint Brush function. To distinguish boutons from the background, the Edge Detect parameter was kept on and the Edge STR was fixed at 0.505, while the selection threshold was adjusted to different values depending on signal intensity48. The Physical Size value of the traced boutons was reported as total bouton volume.
Hierarchical clustering dendrogram
The hierarchical clustering dendrogram was generated using RStudio and the gplots package. The connectivity frequencies measured for each glomerulus in a given species (based on the female connectivity matrices) were used as data. The Euclidean distance between connectivity frequencies was measured as a proxy for similarity. A tree representing overall similarity was generated by clustering glomeruli using the Complete Linkage function.
Aversive learning paradigm
The aversive learning paradigm was designed based on a previously published protocol49. See Supplementary Fig. 8 for a schematic depicting the procedure. Flies were collected a few hours before performing the protocol and housed in regular food vials before being tested. Groups of flies—containing between 60 and 100 individuals—were introduced in a T-maze training apparatus (CelExplorer Labs Co., TMK-501) that was attached to a flowmeter that kept a constant stream of 0.7 L/min (Dwyer Instruments, 116011-01). During the training phase, flies were exposed to a first stimulus, henceforth referred to as the Conditioned Stimulus + (CS + ), at the same time as they were subjected to a regimen of electric shocks delivered by a stimulator (Grass Instruments Co, S48) for a period of one min (12 pulses of 90 V at a frequency of 0.2 Hz); shortly after, flies were allowed to rest for 45 s while exposed to ambient air before being exposed to a second stimulus, henceforth referred to as the Conditioned Stimulus - (CS-), for one min without experiencing any electric shocks; flies were then allowed to rest for 45 s. The conditioned stimuli were either an odor dissolved in mineral oil or mineral oil alone (Sigma-Aldrich, M5904); the odors used were farnesol (1:1000 in mineral oil; Sigma-Aldrich, 43348), hexanoic acid (1:1000 in mineral oil; Sigma-Aldrich, 21529), 3-octanol (1:1000 in mineral oil; Sigma-Aldrich, 218405) or 4-methylcyclohexanol (1:1000 in mineral oil; Sigma-Aldrich, 153095). The training phase was performed either once (single training) or repeated six times with a 15-minute-long inter-training interval (spaced training). Between training and testing, there was a resting phase during which flies were housed in regular food vials and kept in the dark. During the testing phase, flies were given the choice to enter an arm of the T-maze perfumed with CS+ or the other arm perfumed with CS-. The performance index (PI) was calculated as follows: PI = (CS+-CS−)/(CS++CS−). Each n reported in the data sets represents the average values obtained in a pair of reciprocal experiments; in reciprocal experiments, the stimuli used as CS+ and CS− were switched. Innate odor acuity was measured by allowing flies to choose between either a chamber perfumed with mineral oil or a chamber perfumed with an odor over the course of two minutes. Shock acuity was measured by allowing flies to choose between a chamber lined with a copper grid onto which 90 V electric shocks were delivered every five seconds over the course of one minute. All experiments were performed at 23 °C and 55–65% relative humidity under dim red light.
Statistical analyses
For the statistical analyses of the data shown in Table 1, Figs. 3, 4, Supplementary Data 1, Supplementary Table 1 and Supplementary Fig. 3, 4 and 9, p-values were computed using the Mann–Whitney U test; statistical significance is indicated as p < 0.05 (*), p < 0.01 (**) and p < 0.001 (***). For the statistical analyses of the data shown in Fig. 2 and Supplementary Data 2, p-values were computed using the Fisher’s exact test; to control for false positives, p-values were adjusted with a false discovery rate of 10% using a Benjamini-Hochberg procedure. For the statistical analyses of the data shown in Fig. 5, p-values were computed using the sample t test.
To compute the p-values shown in Fig. 2d, e, the connection frequencies measured in the D. sechellia experimental matrix were compared to the connectivity frequencies measured in a data set that contains biased shuffle matrices built using the generalist experimental matrices (D. melanogaster male, D. melanogaster female or D. simulans). These biased shuffle matrices preserved biases in input connection frequencies and the distribution of input counts to Kenyon cells; 1000 biased shuffle matrices were generated for each comparison. Across comparisons, the probability that the observed D. sechellia glomerulus connection frequency was lower (Fig. 2d) or higher (Fig. 2e) than that observed in the data set containing the biased shuffle matrices was computed.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Data availability
The data generated in this study are provided in the Supplementary Information and Source Data file. The unprocessed data—namely, raw two-photon and confocal images—is very large (~2.5 TB). Rather than housing these data on a public repository, the corresponding author will make them available upon request. Source data are provided with this paper.
Code availability
The codes used to analyze the data in this study are available on GitHub: https://github.com/evavigato/Ellis-et-al-2024.git.
References
Striedter, G. F. & Northcutt, R. G. Brains Through Time (Oxford University Press, 2019). https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780195125689.001.0001https://www.hup.harvard.edu/books/9780674046337.
Striedter, G. F. Principles of Brain Evolution. Principles of brain evolution (Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, 2005).
Strausfeld. Arthropod Brains (2011).
Tosches, M. A. From cell types to an integrated understanding of brain evolution: the case of the cerebral cortex. Annu.Rev. Cell Dev. Biol. 37, 495–517 (2021).
Roberts, R. J. V., Pop, S. & Prieto-Godino, L. L. Evolution of central neural circuits: state of the art and perspectives. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 725–743 (2022).
Markow, T. A. The secret lives of Drosophila flies. Elife 4, e06793 (2015).
Drosophila 12 Genomes Consortium. et al. Evolution of genes and genomes on the Drosophila phylogeny. Nature 450, 203–218 (2007).
Sturtevant, A. H. A new species closely resembling Drosophila melanogaster. Psyche (Camb. Mass) 26, 153–155 (1919).
Tsacas, L. & Bächli, G. Drosophila sechellia. n. sp., huitieme espece du sous-groupe melanogaster des iles Seychelles (Diptera, Drosophilidae). Rev. Fr. Entomol. 3, 146–150 (1981).
R’Kha, S., Capy, P. & David, J. R. Host-plant specialization in the Drosophila melanogaster species complex: a physiological, behavioral, and genetical analysis. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 88, 1835–1839 (1991).
Auer, T. O., Shahandeh, M. P. & Benton, R. Drosophila sechellia: a genetic model for behavioral evolution and neuroecology. Annu Rev. Genet 55, 527–554 (2021).
Lachaise, D. & Silvain, J.-F. How two afrotropical endemics made two cosmopolitan human commensals: the Drosophila Melanogaster–D. Simulans palaeogeographic riddle. Genetica 120, 17–39 (2004).
Couto, A., Alenius, M. & Dickson, B. J. Molecular, anatomical, and functional organization of the Drosophila olfactory system. Curr. Biol. 15, 1535–1547 (2005).
Fishilevich, E. & Vosshall, L. B. Genetic and functional subdivision of the Drosophila antennal lobe. Curr. Biol. 15, 1548–1553 (2005).
McBride, C. S. & Arguello, J. R. Five Drosophila genomes reveal nonneutral evolution and the signature of host specialization in the chemoreceptor superfamily. Genetics 177, 1395–1416 (2007).
Auer, T. O. et al. Olfactory receptor and circuit evolution promote host specialization. Nature 579, 402–408 (2020).
Depetris-Chauvin, A. et al. Evolution at multiple processing levels underlies odor-guided behavior in the genus Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 33, 4771–4785.e7 (2023).
Stensmyr, M. C., Dekker, T. & Hansson, B. S. Evolution of the olfactory code in the Drosophila melanogaster subgroup. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 270, 2333–2340 (2003).
Dekker, T., Ibba, I., Siju, K. P., Stensmyr, M. C. & Hansson, B. S. Olfactory shifts parallel superspecialism for toxic fruit in Drosophila melanogaster sibling, D. sechellia. Curr. Biol. 16, 101–109 (2006).
Ibba, I., Angioy, A. M., Hansson, B. S. & Dekker, T. Macroglomeruli for fruit odors change blend preference in Drosophila. Naturwissenschaften 97, 1059–1066 (2010).
Prieto-Godino, L. L. et al. Evolution of acid-sensing olfactory circuits in Drosophilids. Neuron 93, 661–676.e6 (2017).
Jefferis, G. S. X. E. et al. Comprehensive maps of drosophila higher olfactory centers: spatially segregated fruit and pheromone representation. Cell 128, 1187–1203 (2007).
Schlegel, P. et al. Information flow, cell types and stereotypy in a full olfactory connectome. Elife 10, e66018 (2021).
Aso, Y. et al. The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learning. Elife 3, e04577 (2014).
Li, F. et al. The connectome of the adult Drosophila mushroom body provides insights into function. Elife 9, e62576 (2020).
Caron, S. J. C., Ruta, V., Abbott, L. F. & Axel, R. Random convergence of olfactory inputs in the Drosophila mushroom body. Nature 497, 113–117 (2013).
Gruntman, E. & Turner, G. C. Integration of the olfactory code across dendritic claws of single mushroom body neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1821–1829 (2013).
Zheng, Z. et al. Structured sampling of olfactory input by the fly mushroom body. Curr. Biol. 32, 3334–3349.e6 (2022).
Hayashi, T. T. et al. Mushroom body input connections form independently of sensory activity in Drosophila melanogaster. Curr. Biol. 32, 4000–4012.e5 (2022).
Grabe, V. et al. Elucidating the neuronal architecture of olfactory glomeruli in the Drosophila antennal lobe resource elucidating the neuronal architecture of olfactory glomeruli in the Drosophila antennal lobe. 3401–3413 (2016).
Hallem, E. A. & Carlson, J. R. Coding of odors by a receptor repertoire. Cell 125, 143–160 (2006).
Ronderos, D. S., Lin, C.-C., Potter, C. J. & Smith, D. P. Farnesol-detecting olfactory neurons in Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 34, 3959–3968 (2014).
Münch, D. & Galizia, C. G. DoOR 2.0—comprehensive mapping of Drosophila melanogaster odorant responses. Sci. Rep. 6, 21841 (2016).
Krashes, M. J. & Waddell, S. Drosophila aversive olfactory conditioning. Cold Spring Harb. Protoc. 2011, pdb.prot5608 (2011).
Lavista-Llanos, S. et al. Dopamine drives Drosophila sechellia adaptation to its toxic host. Elife 3, e03785 (2014).
Zhao, Z. & McBride, C. S. Evolution of olfactory circuits in insects. J. Comp. Physiol. A 206, 353–367 (2020).
Pan, J. W. et al. Patterns of transcriptional parallelism and variation in the developing olfactory system of Drosophila species. Sci. Rep. 7, 8804 (2017).
Luo, S. X., Axel, R. & Abbott, L. F. Generating sparse and selective third-order responses in the olfactory system of the fly. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 107, 10713–10718 (2010).
Stevens, C. F. What the fly’s nose tells the fly’s brain. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 112, 9460–9465 (2015).
Litwin-Kumar, A., Harris, K. D., Axel, R., Sompolinsky, H. & Abbott, L. F. Optimal degrees of synaptic connectivity. Neuron 93, 1153–1164.e7 (2017).
Stern, D. L. et al. Genetic and transgenic reagents for Drosophila simulans, D. mauritiana, D. yakuba, D. santomea, and D. virilis. G3 Genes|Genomes|Genet. 7, 1339–1347 (2017).
Croset, V. et al. Ancient protostome origin of chemosensory ionotropic glutamate receptors and the evolution of insect taste and olfaction. PLoS Genet 6, e1001064 (2010).
Ruta, V. et al. A dimorphic pheromone circuit in Drosophila from sensory input to descending output. Nature 468, 686–690 (2010).
Scheffer, L. K. et al. A connectome and analysis of the adult Drosophila central brain. Elife 9, e57443 (2020).
Li, J., Ellis, K. E. & Caron, S. J. C. Photo-labeling neurons in the Drosophila brain. STAR Protoc. 2, 100381 (2021).
Schindelin, J. et al. Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nat. Methods 9, 676–682 (2012).
Longair, M. H., Baker, D. A. & Armstrong, J. D. Simple Neurite Tracer: open source software for reconstruction, visualization and analysis of neuronal processes. Bioinformatics 27, 2453–2454 (2011).
Wan, Y. et al. FluoRender: joint freehand segmentation and visualization for many-channel fluorescence data analysis. BMC Bioinform. 18, 280 (2017).
Jacob, P. F. & Waddell, S. Spaced training forms complementary long-term memories of opposite valence in Drosophila. Neuron 106, 977–991.e4 (2020).
Acknowledgements
The authors thank members of the Caron laboratory for comments on the manuscript; David Stern for sharing the attp2176 and attp2178 D. simulans strains and φC31 integrase mRNA; Vanessa Ruta for sharing the UAS-C3PA-GFP plasmid; Adam Lin for preparation of the standard cornmeal agar medium; the Cell Imaging Core at the University of Utah for use of the Zeiss LSM 880 microscope; Nathan Clark for general guidance generating the hierarchical clustering dendrogram and similarity trees; Ashley Platt and Miles Jacob for assistance with general laboratory concerns. This work has been funded by grants from the National Institute for Neurological Disorders and Stroke (R01 EB 029858 (A.L.K.), R01 NS 106018 (S.J.C.C.) and R01 NS 1079790 (S.J.C.C.)), the National Science Foundation (DBI 1707398 (A.L.K.) and IOS 2042397 (S.J.C.C.)), the Gatsby Charitable Foundation (A.L.K.), the European Research Council (European Research Council Advanced Grant 833548) (R.B.) and the Swiss National Science Foundation (Ambizione Grant PZ00P3 185743) (T.O.A.). Further financial support was provided by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (T32-HD-007491) (K.E.E.), the Department of Energy Computational Science Graduate Fellowship (DE-SC0022158) (I.G.), the Burroughs Wellcome Foundation (A.L.K.), the McKnight Endowment Fund (A.L.K.), the Simons Collaboration on the Global Brain (A.L.K.), the Human Frontier Science Program (LT000461/2015-L) (T.O.A.) and the Georges S. and Dolores Eccles Foundation (S.J.C.C.).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
K.E.E., S.B., and S.J.C.C. conceived the project. K.E.E. photo-labeled projection neurons and Kenyon cells, dye-labeled individual projection neurons, and analyzed their morphology; K.E.E. generated the connectivity matrices; S.B. performed and analyzed all behavioral experiments; H.M.S. generated and analyzed the antennal lobe reconstructions; I.G. and A.L.K. performed the statistical analyses of the connectivity matrices; E.V. generated the similarity trees; T.O.A. and R.B. provided the D. sechellia transgenic lines and plasmids used to generate the D. simulans transgenes. K.E.E and S.J.C.C. wrote the manuscript with input from all other authors.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Communications thanks the anonymous reviewers for their contribution to the peer review of this work. A peer review file is available.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Source data
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Ellis, K.E., Bervoets, S., Smihula, H. et al. Evolution of connectivity architecture in the Drosophila mushroom body. Nat Commun 15, 4872 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-024-48839-4
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-024-48839-4
This article is cited by
-
Diversity of visual inputs to Kenyon cells of the Drosophila mushroom body
Nature Communications (2024)
-
Olfactory sensory neuron population expansions influence projection neuron adaptation and enhance odour tracking
Nature Communications (2024)