Abstract
In the age of misinformation, conspiracy theories can have far-reaching consequences for individuals and society. Social and emotional experiences throughout the life course, such as loneliness, may be associated with a tendency to hold conspiracist worldviews. Here, we present results from a population-based sample of Norwegians followed for almost three decades, from adolescence into midlife (N = 2215). We examine participants’ life trajectories of loneliness using latent growth curve modeling. We show that people reporting high levels of loneliness in adolescence, and those who experience increasing loneliness over the life course, are more likely to endorse conspiracy worldviews in midlife.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
While conspiracy theories are not new1,2, recent events have shown how dangerous and polarizing they can be in a globalized, mediatized world. Conspiracy theories undermined global efforts to contain the COVID-19 virus during the pandemic3,4 and were used in the lead-up to the January 6, 2021, raid on the Capitol1. They lie at the core of political and social polarization5,6, fueling vaccine skepticism7, climate change skepticism8,9, and anti-science movements such as the flat earthers10,11. In the age of misinformation12, understanding what makes people endorse conspiracy theories is crucial. However, research on the psychology of conspiracy beliefs is rather recent, with more than half of the studies dating from 2020 or later1. Critically, very little longitudinal research on the antecedents of conspiracy beliefs is available to date. Existing studies capture only short periods of time13,14, complicating the identification of early antecedents. Developmental perspectives examining how people’s life trajectories are associated with conspiracy mindsets are therefore missing due to the lack of suitable data1. Here, we address this gap by investigating the link between conspiracy beliefs and loneliness trajectories over the course of three decades.
While several motives may be implicated in the development of conspiracist worldviews15,16,17,18, both theory and research suggest that frustration of social needs and the resulting feelings of loneliness may be particularly important. A recent meta-analysis found that the factor showing the strongest cross-sectional association with conspiracy beliefs (r = 0.37) is social alienation, of which loneliness can be seen as a facet16. Loneliness was also positively correlated with a conspiracy mindset (r = 0.19) in a representative German sample19. So far, however, research on the link between loneliness and conspiracy beliefs16, and in particular longitudinal research that could clarify how this link plays out over time, is scarce1. Whether loneliness experienced in critical periods (i.e., during adolescence) and over prolonged periods is associated with a conspiracist worldview later in life has not been examined.
There are, however, several theoretical reasons for such an association. First, conspiracy beliefs may help make sense of one’s loneliness20 in a way that protects the ego, following general processes of motivated reasoning21,22. Sense-making and ego defense seem to be among the main psychological functions of conspiracy beliefs23 and could be particularly relevant for lonely people who generally seem hypervigilant to social threats and may use blame to deal with their own negative emotions24. Conspiracy beliefs may preserve a positive self-image by shifting the blame for one’s loneliness to malicious others (e.g., I am not a failure but a victim of a conspiracy)17. These beliefs may even enhance people’s self-image by explaining their loneliness with their uniqueness (e.g., I am alone because I understand things others do not understand)17,25. Second, lonely people may lack the social feedback that could correct their developing conspiracist views, and once these views are formed, such people may purposefully seek reinforcing feedback from other like-minded conspiracy believers26,27.
Lastly, loneliness may motivate people to adopt conspiracy beliefs in an attempt to gain community and a sense of social identity1,28. Several theoretical models describe loneliness as a motivational force across development29,30,31. Some people who see themselves as lonely may experience a motivation to reconnect29,31, and seeking conspiracist communities might offer this opportunity. Online conspiracist groups in particular are easy to join, highly reinforcing and engaging, which may make them an accessible and suitable source of social nourishment and identity for socially isolated individuals1,27,32. Indeed, individuals high in conspiracy beliefs are those who feel most socially isolated after unplugging from the internet33. However, it should be noted that other lines of research suggest that loneliness is associated with social withdrawal rather than a motivation to reconnect34,35, which would make this a less plausible mechanism underpinning the link between loneliness and conspiracy beliefs than the previously described mechanisms of ego protection and lack of corrective feedback.
In this work, we show that people’s early experiences of loneliness and the increase of it throughout adulthood are positively associated with conspiracist worldviews in midlife. To do so, we use data collected from a population-based sample of 2215 Norwegians followed over 28 years.
Results
Participants were junior and senior high school students in grades 7-12 (Mage = 15.05, SDage = 1.98) at the first timepoint (in 1992), and in their early to mid-forties at the last timepoint (in 2020; Mage = 43.22, SDage = 2.00). They reported their levels of loneliness at five timepoints between 1992 and 2020 on a Norwegian short version of the UCLA Loneliness Scale36,37. At the last timepoint, in their mid-forties, participants also reported to what extent they endorsed a conspiracist worldview assessed by the Conspiracy Mentality Questionnaire38. Both measures showed satisfactory reliability (.76 ≤ αLoneliness ≤ .80, αCMQ = .83), and the loneliness measure was invariant over the five measurements, with the strong invariance model showing a close fit with the data, χ2(70) = 29.36, p < .001; CFI = .98; SRMR = .031; RMSEA = .038, pclose = 1.000, 90% CIRMSEA = [.033, .042]. Thus, we estimated a second-order latent growth curve model based on the strong invariance model (see Supplementary Table 3 for a technical description and detailed results).
Overall, consistent with previous research using the same scale39 and cohort studies in Norway40, participants’ loneliness showed an increasing trajectory between 1992 and 2020 (see Supplementary Information for details). This increase was linear in shape: loneliness grew steadily from adolescence until mid-adulthood. We then tested if the initial level of loneliness (i.e., the intercept) and its trajectory over 28 years (i.e., the slope) were associated with participants’ endorsement of a conspiracist worldview in midlife in a conditional growth model (Fig. 1, Table 1). Indeed, the lonelier participants were as adolescents in 1992, the greater their conspiracist worldview as adults in 2020, zintercept = 3.39, p = 0.001, βintercept = 0.11, 95% CI = [0.05, 0.18]. Moreover, the more loneliness increased over participants’ life course, the more likely they were to report a conspiracist worldview in 2020, zslope = 4.00, p < 0.001, βslope = 0.17, 95% CI = [0.08, 0.25]. These results proved robust after controlling for age, sex, parental education, and political orientation measured in 1994, zintercept = 4.14, p < 0.001, βintercept = .14, 95% CI = [0.08, 0.19], zslope = 3.61, p < 0.001, βslope = 0.15, 95% CI = [0.08, 0.22] (Supplementary Table 6).
Separate lines of research have linked conspiracy beliefs1,27 and loneliness41 to psychopathology. To rule out that the associations with loneliness were artifacts of underlying psychopathology, we included symptoms of depression and anxiety measured by a short version of the Hopkins Symptom Checklist42 (0.83 ≤ αSCL ≤ 0.91) as time-varying covariates in the model. Specifically, when estimating loneliness growth curves, we regressed loneliness within each timepoint on depression and anxiety scores43; χ2(324) = 2,134.06, p < 0.001; CFI = 0.907, SRMR = 0.038; RMSEA = 0.050, pclose = .424, 95% CIRMSEA = [.048, .052]. In this model, the intercept represents the baseline, and the slope represents the growth in loneliness that is not attributable to temporal changes in symptoms of depression and anxiety. Even after controlling for these symptoms, the initial level, zintercept = 3.34, p = 0.001, βintercept = .11, 95% CI = [0.06, 0.16], and the trajectory, zslope = 2.42, p = 0.015, βslope = 0.11, 95% CI = [0.04, 0.19] (Supplementary Table 6) of loneliness remained positively related to conspiracy worldviews.
Discussion
Our 28-year study shows that conspiracist worldviews held in midlife are associated with experiences of loneliness across adolescence and adulthood. Conspiracist worldviews were particularly appealing to participants who were relatively lonely as adolescents and experienced increasing loneliness through their lives. One possible explanation for this pattern, albeit tentative and requiring further research, is that the contrasting of one’s own increasing loneliness relative to peers might be potent in fostering feelings of social isolation44,45,46, motivating our participants to turn to conspiracy theorizing to protect their ego, or to seek social connection among like-minded conspiracist groups1,17,20,28.
As in any observational study, we cannot exclude the possibility that our results are confounded by third variables, despite our efforts to control for age, sex, parental education, political orientation, and depression and anxiety. Factors such as personality traits, paranoid tendencies, and lower cognitive abilities, or experiences such as economic deprivation and selective media exposure might predispose individuals to both loneliness47 and conspiracist worldviews1,48,49,50. Including all relevant variables in one observational study is infeasible, and only rigorous experimental research can further eliminate possible spuriousness. So far, experimental results show that manipulating ostracism increases participants’ conspiracy beliefs, which aligns with our findings51. Yet, it is essential to highlight that loneliness and ostracism, albeit related, are distinct concepts that may allude to the unfulfillment of different social needs. Whereas ostracism refers to the interpersonal or intergroup process of deliberate exclusion, loneliness captures a socio-affective state that can arise from ostracism. However, loneliness can have numerous other causes52,53,54, and not everyone who experiences ostracism or alienation necessarily feels lonely55. Therefore, future studies would benefit from exploring the nuanced impacts of ostracism, social alienation, loneliness, and their specific repercussions.
The associations observed in this study might also depend on contextual factors. Here, it is important to note that our data were collected in Norway—a technologically advanced society with high levels of institutional trust56—and future research is needed to test the generalizability of our findings in other contexts54. Norway can generally be described as a highly functioning welfare society with relatively low levels of loneliness57. On one hand, these low levels may mask, to some extent, the true size of associations between loneliness and conspiracist worldviews (i.e., due to floor effects). Thus, findings may be even more pronounced in societies where loneliness is more prevalent. On the other hand, given that loneliness seems generally uncommon in Norway, those who experience it might perceive themselves as outliers, prompting them to adopt ego defenses by embracing conspiracy theories. This may not be the case in societies where experiencing loneliness is more normative. Investigating the relationship between loneliness and conspiracist worldviews at the country level could bring further insights into the role of context, including cultural values (e.g., individualism-collectivism1,54,58, and other nation-level variables such as general political climate, corruption, autocracy, or economic dysfunction1,49.
Research might also explore how effects differ when examining beliefs in specific conspiracy theories, oftentimes deeply rooted in culture and sociopolitical divides, as opposed to the broad conspiracist worldview our study focused on. Our broad approach avoids catering exclusively to specific cultural contexts or groups, thus enhancing the generalizability of our findings. However, it is possible that loneliness has stronger associations with some conspiracy beliefs than with others. For instance, conspiracy theories rooted in a nation’s shared historical trauma are likely more commonly endorsed by its members50,59 and thus, might be more appealing to those seeking social connection.
Methodologically, our study has one key limitation: as conspiracist worldviews represent a relatively recent construct in psychological research, controlling for participants’ initial levels thereof in 1992 was impossible. Even so, we believe that our finding of a significant association between loneliness in adolescence and conspiracy mentality in midlife goes beyond earlier cross-sectional findings16,19. Showing that the estimated levels of loneliness in adolescence are associated with conspiracy mentality, almost three decades later and accounting for later developments of loneliness, suggests that loneliness is related to conspiracy mentality over substantially long timeframes. Combined with the significant association of the slope of loneliness to conspiracy mentality in midlife, this finding may suggest that loneliness and conspiracy mentality are systematically related to each other from adolescence to midlife in ways consistent with the theoretical notion that conspiracist worldviews reflect sense making and ego defenses adopted in response to loneliness20,21,23. The unavailability of earlier measurements of conspiracy worldviews is a major limitation, nevertheless. Namely, this study design prevents us from testing a reverse causal direction: that adopting a conspiracist worldview might further exacerbate loneliness. Indeed, people who express conspiracy theories early in life might be excluded from social groups1 which could lead to feelings of loneliness.
This considered, future research would be well advised to attempt a replication of our results in other contexts, and with experimental designs allowing for causal inferences. If successful, such replications would suggest that interventions targeting loneliness could be useful to reduce conspiracy beliefs and their societal repercussions. So far, the results of psychological interventions focusing primarily on cognitive processes (e.g., pre-bunking, debunking, cognitive inoculation) have been insufficient on their own to fully counter conspiracist worldviews1,12,60. There might, however, exist an alternative, complementary pathway to prevent conspiracy beliefs, one that leads via socio-affective processes. On one hand, previous research showed that the link between experimentally manipulated social exclusion (i.e., ostracism) and conspiracist thinking can be mitigated51, and the same may be the case for loneliness. On the other hand, targeting loneliness and fostering social connection is known to be effective: it helps prevent other adverse outcomes, including somatic and mental health problems or even mortality risks, for a myriad of different social groups61,62,63,64. Therefore, instead of concentrating solely on cognitive factors, research may test experimentally whether reducing people’s loneliness is a way to counter the onset of conspiracist worldviews and their societal repercussions.
Methods
This study complies with all relevant ethical regulations for research with human subjects and obtained ethical approval from the Norwegian Regional Committees for Medical Research Ethics (reference no.: 25462; project name: Young in Norway). The study is based on survey data from the Young in Norway Study65,66 collected at five timepoints: in 1992 (T1), 1994 (T2), 1999 (T3), 2005 (T4), and 2020 (T5). All items of the multi-item measures used here are reported in Supplementary Table 1.
Participants and procedure
At T1 (1992), a national sample of Norwegian junior and senior high school students, from 67 schools in grades 7–12 (age 12–20), was selected from stratified areas. Since the study did not include any experimental manipulations, no further randomization was applied. Each grade was equally represented, and cluster-sampling was applied with the school as the unit. See67 for more information about the sampling procedures.
Data68 were collected in the participating schools and the initial sample consisted of 11,985 participants, equally distributed according to gender and age. No statistical method was used to predetermine sample size. Written informed consent was obtained from participants or their parents whenever the participants were below the age of 15 at T1. Participants were followed up with questionnaires at school at T2 (1994), and a subset was then approached by postal means (using pen and pencil questionnaires) and digital means (using the Nettskjema online data collection tool) at the remaining time points: T3 (1999; N = 2924), T4 (2005; N = 2890), and T5 (2020; N = 2215). They were asked to renew their informed consent at T2 and at T4 in line with ethical stipulations.
Because the outcome measure of interest (i.e., conspiracy mentality) was included at T5, only data from participants who had completed this wave were used in the current study. Otherwise, no data were excluded from the analyses. In this sample, 57.4% of participants were women, and 42.6% were men. Most participants (93.6%) were ethnic Norwegians; 6.4% of participants had some immigrant background (i.e., were born abroad or had at least one parent who was born abroad). Less than half of participants (43.3%) had at least one parent who attended college or university.
Analyses
Descriptives and correlations
Descriptive statistics and correlations between study variables are presented in Supplementary Table 2.
Measurement invariance
All analyses were conducted in Mplus v.869 and used two-tailed significance tests. Since latent growth curve models assume that modeled constructs are psychometrically equivalent across time, we first tested for longitudinal measurement invariance of the loneliness measure to assess whether this assumption was met. We report the results in Supplementary Table 3. We fitted a measurement model including all observed indicators of the latent construct of loneliness (i.e., the five items of the Norwegian short version of the UCLA loneliness scale37) at each timepoint (configural invariance model). We then constrained factor loadings to equality across timepoints (weak invariance model), and then loadings and intercepts of the items (strong invariance model). This analysis revealed that two out of the five initially used items (i.e., the reversed items, for which higher scores were thought to indicate less loneliness: “I feel in tune with the people around me”, “I can find companionship when I want it”) yielded low loadings on the latent loneliness construct (0.29 ≤ β ≤ 0.54) and were not invariant across time, resulting in poor fit of the strong invariance model, χ2(251) = 2520.63, p < .001, CFI = 0.880, TLI = 0.856, SRMR = 0.067, RMSEA = 0.064, pclose < 0.001, 90% CIRMSEA = [0.062, 0.066]. Because latent growth modeling requires strong longitudinal invariance of the modeled construct70, we removed these two reversed items, achieving excellent fit with the data for both weak invariance and strong invariance models. We therefore retained the strong invariance model based on the resulting three-item loneliness measure. Thus, in all remaining analyses, factor loadings and intercepts were constrained to equality across the five time points. Moreover, all models included correlations between residuals of the same items of loneliness measured at different timepoints.
Latent growth curve analyses
We fitted a series of second-order latent growth curve models, that is, models consisting of both the strong invariance measurement model and a latent growth curve model71,72. Supplementary Table 4 presents the detailed results of these analyses. To account for uneven time intervals between measurements, time for the slope components in these analyses was coded proportionally to the lag from the first measurement to each timepoint: T1 as 0, T2 as 0.2, T3 as 0.7, T4 as 1.3, and T5 as 2.8 (please note that decimals were used to avoid inflated variance values). Since the variances of all latent variables were set to 1 for model specification, loneliness values had a mean of zero and SD of 1. Full information likelihood estimation was used to handle missing values (≤10.2%).
First, we tested two univariate models including only loneliness at five time points to determine the shape of the trajectory: a linear model including the intercept and linear slope of loneliness (Model 1) and a quadratic model including the intercept, the linear slope, and the quadratic slope of loneliness that fitted the data best (Model 2). Then, to test whether the different trajectories of loneliness from adolescence into mid-adulthood are associated with conspiracy worldviews in 2020, we added conspiracy worldview at T5 as an outcome measure (again, as a measurement model with a latent construct of conspiracy worldview consisting of scale items as observed indicators) to the retained quadratic model (Model 3). We regressed conspiracy worldview on the intercept and linear slope of loneliness (but not on the quadratic slope, due to its high correlation with the linear slope and the resulting multicollinearity). Moreover, we tested the robustness of this model by adding time-invariant covariates (sex as recorded in national registries, age at T5 as recorded in national registries, political orientation at T2, parental education at T1; Model 4) and symptoms of depression and anxiety as time varying covariate (Model 5).
Simple intercepts and slopes analysis
We conducted simple intercepts and slopes analyses73 to test how large the change in loneliness over three decades had been for participants who reported low, medium, and high levels of conspiracy worldview in midlife. Since the acceleration of the curve (quadratic slope) was not regressed on conspiracy worldview, we estimated its value at each level of conspiracy worldview based on the covariance of these two variables, using the formula:
where q is the value of the quadratic slope at low, medium or high level of the outcome, \(\bar{q}\) is the mean of the quadratic slope, cov(q,x) is the covariance between the quadratic slope and the outcome, var(q) is the variance of the quadratic slope, and x is the value of the outcome at the corresponding (low, M – 1 SD; medium, M; or high M + 1 SD) level of the outcome (here, conspiracy worldview in 2020). The results are presented in Table 2.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Data availability
The primary data generated in this study have been deposited in the Open Science Framework repository under accession code https://osf.io/yjzqe (see Supplementary Information, Supplementary Note 1, for variable names). The raw data related to sociodemographic characteristics of the participants (age, gender, parental education) are protected and are not available due to Norwegian data privacy laws.
Code availability
The analysis codes generated in this study have been deposited in the Open Science Framework repository under the accession code https://osf.io/yjzqe.
References
Hornsey, M. J., Bierwiaczonek, K., Sassenberg, K. & Douglas, K. M. Individual, intergroup and nation-level influences on belief in conspiracy theories. Nat. Rev. Psychol. 2, 85–97 (2023).
van Prooijen, J.-W. & van Vugt, M. Conspiracy theories: evolved functions and psychological mechanisms. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 13, 770–788 (2018).
Bierwiaczonek, K., Gundersen, A. B. & Kunst, J. R. The role of conspiracy beliefs for COVID-19 health responses: a meta-analysis. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 46, 101346 (2022).
Van Bavel, J. J. et al. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 460–471 (2020).
Imhoff, R. et al. Conspiracy mentality and political orientation across 26 countries. Nat. Hum. Behav. 6, 392–403 (2022).
Sutton, R. M. & Douglas, K. M. Conspiracy theories and the conspiracy mindset: implications for political ideology. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 34, 118–122 (2020).
Hornsey, M. J., Harris, E. A. & Fielding, K. S. The psychological roots of anti-vaccination attitudes: a 24-nation investigation. Health Psychol. 37, 307–315 (2018).
Hornsey, M. J. & Fielding, K. S. Understanding (and Reducing) Inaction on Climate Change. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 14, 3–35 (2020).
van der Linden, S. The conspiracy-effect: Exposure to conspiracy theories (about global warming) decreases pro-social behavior and science acceptance. Personal. Individ. Differences 87, 171–173 (2015).
Rutjens, B. T. & Većkalov, B. Conspiracy beliefs and science rejection. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 46, 101392 (2022).
van der Linden, S. Countering science denial. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3, 889–890 (2019).
Ecker, U. K. H. et al. The psychological drivers of misinformation belief and its resistance to correction. Nat. Rev. Psychol. 1, 13–29 (2022).
Liekefett, L., Christ, O. & Becker, J. C. Can Conspiracy Beliefs Be Beneficial? Longitudinal Linkages Between Conspiracy Beliefs, Anxiety, Uncertainty Aversion, and Existential Threat. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 49, 167–179 (2023).
Górska, P. et al. A vicious circle? Longitudinal relationships between different modes of in-group identity and COVID-19 conspiracy thinking. J. Soc. Psychol. 163, 877–894 (2023).
Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M. & Cichocka, A. The Psychology of Conspiracy Theories. Curr. Directions Psychological Sci. 26, 538–542 (2017).
Biddlestone, M., Green, R., Cichocka, A., Douglas, K. & Sutton, R. M. A systematic review and meta-analytic synthesis of the motives associated with conspiracy beliefs. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/rxjqc (2022).
Biddlestone, M., Green, R., Cichocka, A., Sutton, R. & Douglas, K. Conspiracy beliefs and the individual, relational, and collective selves. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 15, e12639 (2021).
Jetten, J., Peters, K. & Casara, B. G. S. Economic inequality and conspiracy theories. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 47, 101358 (2022).
Hettich, N. et al. Conspiracy endorsement and its associations with personality functioning, anxiety, loneliness, and sociodemographic characteristics during the COVID-19 pandemic in a representative sample of the German population. PLoS ONE 17, e0263301 (2022).
Graeupner, D. & Coman, A. The dark side of meaning-making: How social exclusion leads to superstitious thinking. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 69, 218–222 (2017).
Albarracín, D. in The Psychology of Fake News 196–219 (Routledge, 2020).
Baumeister, R. F. Identity, self-concept, and self-esteem: The self lost and found. in Handbook of Personality Psychol. 681–710 (Elsevier, 1997).
Prooijen, J.-W. V. Psychological benefits of believing conspiracy theories. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 47, 101352 (2022).
Preece, D. A. et al. Loneliness and emotion regulation. Personal. Individ. Diff. 180, 110974 (2021).
Lantian, A., Muller, D., Nurra, C. & Douglas, K. M. I know things they don’t know!”: the role of need for uniqueness in belief in conspiracy theories. Soc. Psychol. 48, 160–173 (2017).
Uscinski, J., Enders, A. M., Klofstad, C. & Stoler, J. Cause and effect: on the antecedents and consequences of conspiracy theory beliefs. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 47, 101364 (2022).
Phadke, S., Samory, M. & Mitra, T. What makes people join conspiracy communities?: Role of social factors in conspiracy engagement. Proc. ACM Hum.-Comput. Interact. 4, 1–30 (2020).
Ren, Z. B., Dimant, E. & Schweitzer, M. Social Motives for Sharing Conspiracy Theories. (2021).
Qualter, P. et al. Loneliness across the life span. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 250–264 (2015).
Cacioppo, J. T. et al. Loneliness within a nomological net: An evolutionary perspective. J. Res. Personal. 40, 1054–1085 (2006).
Heinrich, L. M. & Gullone, E. The clinical significance of loneliness: a literature review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 26, 695–718 (2006).
Nera, K., Jetten, J., Biddlestone, M. & Klein, O. Who wants to silence us’? Perceived discrimination of conspiracy theory believers increases ‘conspiracy theorist’ identification when it comes from powerholders – But not from the general public. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 61, 1263–1285 (2022).
Jetten, J., Zhao, C., Álvarez, B., Kaempf, S. & Mols, F. Trying to unplug for 24 hours: conspiracy mentality predicts social isolation and negative emotions when refraining from internet use. Adv. Psychol. 01, 1–19 (2023).
Smith, K. E. & Pollak, S. D. Approach motivation and loneliness: Individual differences and parasympathetic activity. Psychophysiology 59, e14036 (2022).
Layden, E. A., Cacioppo, J. T. & Cacioppo, S. Loneliness predicts a preference for larger interpersonal distance within intimate space. PLoS ONE 13, e0203491 (2018).
Russell, D., Peplau, L. A. & Ferguson, M. L. Developing a measure of loneliness. J. Personal. Assess. 42, 290–294 (1978).
von Soest, T., Luhmann, M. & Gerstorf, D. The development of loneliness through adolescence and young adulthood: its nature, correlates, and midlife outcomes. Dev. Psychol. 56, 1919–1934 (2020).
Bruder, M., Haffke, P., Neave, N., Nouripanah, N. & Imhoff, R. Measuring individual differences in generic beliefs in conspiracy theories across cultures: conspiracy mentality questionnaire. Front. Psychol. 4, 225 (2013).
Mund, M., Freuding, M. M., Möbius, K., Horn, N. & Neyer, F. J. The stability and change of loneliness across the life span: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 24, 24–52 (2019).
Nicolaisen, M. & Thorsen, K. Who are lonely? Loneliness in different age groups (18–81 Years Old), using two measures of loneliness. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 78, 229–257 (2014).
Achterbergh, L. et al. The experience of loneliness among young people with depression: a qualitative meta-synthesis of the literature. BMC Psychiatry 20, 415 (2020).
Derogatis, L. R., Lipman, R. S., Rickels, K., Uhlenhuth, E. H. & Covi, L. The Hopkins Symptom Checklist (HSCL): a self-report symptom inventory. Behav. Sci. 19, 1–15 (1974).
Grimm, K. J. Multivariate longitudinal methods for studying developmental relationships between depression and academic achievement. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 31, 328–339 (2007).
Morgan, D. & Burholt, V. Transitions in loneliness in later life: the role of social comparisons and coping strategies. Ageing Soc. 42, 1607–1628 (2022).
Peplau, L. A., Bikson, T. K., Rook, K. S. & Goodchilds, J. D. Being old and living alone. in Loneliness. A sourcebook of current theory, research and therapy (eds. Peplau, L. A. & Perlman, D.) (John Wiley & Sons, New York, 1982).
Yang, C.-C. Instagram use, loneliness, and social comparison orientation: interact and browse on social media, but don’t compare. Cyberpsychology, Behav., Soc. Netw. 19, 703–708 (2016).
Schermer, J. A. & Martin, N. G. A behavior genetic analysis of personality and loneliness. J. Res. Personal. 78, 133–137 (2019).
Alsuhibani, A., Shevlin, M., Freeman, D., Sheaves, B. & Bentall, R. P. Why conspiracy theorists are not always paranoid: Conspiracy theories and paranoia form separate factors with distinct psychological predictors. PLoS ONE 17, e0259053 (2022).
Hornsey, M. J. et al. Multinational data show that conspiracy beliefs are associated with the perception (and reality) of poor national economic performance. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 53, 78–89 (2023).
Bilewicz, M. Conspiracy beliefs as an adaptation to historical trauma. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 47, 101359 (2022).
Poon, K.-T., Chen, Z. & Wong, W.-Y. Beliefs in conspiracy theories following ostracism. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 46, 1234–1246 (2020).
Cohen-Mansfield, J., Hazan, H., Lerman, Y. & Shalom, V. Correlates and predictors of loneliness in older-adults: a review of quantitative results informed by qualitative insights. Int. Psychogeriatr. 28, 557–576 (2016).
Mahon, N. E., Yarcheski, A., Yarcheski, T. J., Cannella, B. L. & Hanks, M. M. A Meta-analytic Study of Predictors for Loneliness During Adolescence. Nurs. Res. 55, 308–315 (2006).
Luhmann, M., Buecker, S. & Rüsberg, M. Loneliness across time and space. Nat. Rev. Psychol. 2, 9–23 (2023).
Wesselmann, E. D., Wirth, J. H., Mroczek, D. K. & Williams, K. D. Dial a feeling: Detecting moderation of affect decline during ostracism. Personal. Individ. Differences 53, 580–586 (2012).
Svendsen, G. L. H. & Svendsen, G. T. The puzzle of the Scandinavian welfare state and social trust. Issues Soc. Sci. 3, 90–99 (2015).
Hansen, T. & Slagsvold, B. Late-life loneliness in 11 European countries: results from the generations and gender survey. Soc. Indic. Res. 129, 445–464 (2016).
Lykes, V. A. & Kemmelmeier, M. What predicts loneliness? Cultural difference between individualistic and collectivistic societies in Europe. J. Cross-Cultural Psychol. 45, 468–490 (2013).
Skrodzka, M., Kende, A., Faragó, L. & Bilewicz, M. Remember that we suffered!” The effects of historical trauma on anti-Semitic prejudice. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 52, 341–350 (2022).
Kreko, P. Countering conspiracy theories and misinformation. In: Routledge handbook of conspiracy theories, 1st edn (eds Butter, M. & Knight, P.) 242–256 (Imprint Routledge, London, 2020). https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429452734.
Hawkley, L. C. Loneliness and health. Nat. Rev. Dis. Prim. 8, 22 (2022).
Park, C. et al. The effect of loneliness on distinct health outcomes: a comprehensive review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 294, 113514 (2020).
Drinkwater, C., Wildman, J. & Moffatt, S. Social prescribing. BMJ 364, l1285 (2019).
Jetten, J., Haslam, C., Haslam, S. A. & Branscombe, N. R. The social cure. Sci. Am. Mind 20, 26–33, (2009).
Fluit, S., Kunst, J. R., Bierwiaczonek, K. & von Soest, T. Self-esteem trajectories over three decades predict opposition to social equality in midlife. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 120, e2212906120 (2023).
von Soest, T., Wichstrøm, L. & Kvalem, I. L. The development of global and domain-specific self-esteem from age 13 to 31. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 110, 592–608 (2016).
Wichstrøm, L. Alcohol intoxication and school dropout. Drug Alcohol Rev. 17, 413–421 (1998).
Bierwiaczonek, K., Fluit, S., von Soest, T., Hornsey, M. J. & Kunst, J. R. Data and analysis codes for the manuscript “Loneliness trajectories over three decades are associated with conspiracy worldviews in midlife” (Open Science Framework, https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/YJZQE (2024).
Muthén, L. K. & Muthén, B. O. Mplus User’s Guide. Eighth Edition, (Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles, CA, 2017).
Stoel, R. D., van den Wittenboer, G. & Hox, J. Methodological Issues in the Application of the Latent Growth Curve Model. in Recent Developments on Structural Equation Models: Theory and Applications (eds. van Montfort, K., Oud, J. & Satorra, A.) 241–261 (Springer Netherlands, Dordrecht, 2004).
Hancock, G. R., Kuo, W.-L. & Lawrence, F. R. An illustration of second-order latent growth models. Struct. Equ. Modeling: A Multidiscip. J. 8, 470–489 (2001).
Bishop, J., Geiser, C. & Cole, D. A. Modeling latent growth with multiple indicators: a comparison of three approaches. Psychological Methods 20, 43–62 (2015).
Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J. & Bauer, D. J. Computational tools for probing interactions in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. J. Educ. Behav. Stat. 31, 437–448 (2006).
Acknowledgements
This research was funded by the Research Council of Norway, grants #288083 and #301010 (T.v.S., S.F). This research was supported by an EEA Grant 2014–2021 (Nr. 2019/35/J/HS6/03498) in the IdeaLab call operated by the National Science Centre (NCN).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
K.B., T.v.S., J.R.K., S.F., and M.J.H. conceived the original idea for this research. K.B., S.F., and T.v.S. developed the methodological approach. K.B. conducted the analyses. K.B. and J.R.K. visualized the results. T.v.S. acquired the funding for this study. K.B. drafted the original manuscript. K.B., J.R.K., S.F., T.v.S., and M.J.H. revised the manuscript and approved the final version.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Communications thanks Roland Imhoff and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. A peer review file is available.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Bierwiaczonek, K., Fluit, S., von Soest, T. et al. Loneliness trajectories over three decades are associated with conspiracist worldviews in midlife. Nat Commun 15, 3629 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-024-47113-x
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-024-47113-x
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.