Abstract
Dopamine has a significant role in motor and cognitive function. The dopaminergic pathways originating from the midbrain have received the most attention; however, the relevance of the cerebellar dopaminergic system is largely undiscovered. Here, we show that the major cerebellar astrocyte type Bergmann glial cells express D1 receptors. Dopamine can be synthesized in Purkinje cells by cytochrome P450 and released in an activity-dependent fashion. We demonstrate that activation of D1 receptors induces membrane depolarization and Ca2+ release from the internal store. These astrocytic activities in turn modify Purkinje cell output by altering its excitatory and inhibitory synaptic input. Lastly, we show that conditional knockout of D1 receptors in Bergmann glial cells results in decreased locomotor activity and impaired social activity. These results contribute to the understanding of the molecular, cellular, and circuit mechanisms underlying dopamine function in the cerebellum, revealing a critical role for the cerebellar dopaminergic system in motor and social behavior.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Dopamine (DA) is a neuromodulator that has a profound role in motor and cognitive function1. A great deal of knowledge has been gained by exploring DA action in the DAergic pathways that originate from the midbrain. DA dysfunction is implicated in the neuropathology of a myriad of neurological and psychiatric diseases, including Parkinson’s disease (PD), Huntington’s disease (HD), schizophrenia, drug addiction, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and autism spectrum disorder (ASD)2,3,4,5,6,7. In addition to the midbrain, many of these disorders are associated with an impairment in the cerebellum8,9,10,11. This role could be ascribed to cerebellar influence on those DAergic pathways via their anatomical connection12. The cerebellum has direct projections to the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and modulates its DA release, contributing to reward and social behavior13,14. The cerebellum also affects food consumption by modulating DA release in the striatum15. Nevertheless, it is also plausible that an innate DAergic system in the cerebellum is involved in the etiology of these disorders16. All in all, the cerebellar DAergic system was often underappreciated, but accumulating evidence suggests it may have a significant role.
DAergic fibers were shown to be spread throughout the molecular layer (ML) in early studies using immunostaining and autoradiographic mapping in the rodent cerebellum17,18. In vivo autoradiography and positron emission tomography (PET) in primates demonstrated DA binding in the cerebellar dentate nucleus19. DA release was also detected in the rat cerebellum using microdialysis20. Quantitative analysis by high-performance liquid chromatography (HPLC) demonstrated cerebellar DA21, consistent with a more recent finding that revealed cerebellar DA using proton nuclear magnetic resonance (1H-NMR)22. Physiological experiments further showed that DA signaling is responsible for Ca2+-mediated depolarization-induced slow currents (DISCs) in Purkinje cells (PCs)23. DA receptor subunits D1R-D5R have also been observed in the various lobules of the cerebellar cortex16,24. These findings indicate that an independent DAergic system exists in the cerebellum. However, the exact source of DA and the precise distribution of DA receptors are undefined.
The cerebellum is a unique brain structure, where PCs integrate excitatory synaptic inputs from climbing fibers (CFs) and parallel fibers (PFs) and send solely inhibitory output25. Intercalated among PC soma and dendrites are molecular layer interneurons (MLIs) and Bergmann glial cells (BGs). MLIs provide a major synaptic inhibition and BGs have a critical role in fine control of cerebellar neurotransmission and network activity. BGs are the primary astrocyte type and outnumber PCs by approximately eightfold26. Each BG has up to five polarized processes extending through the entire ML. These radial BG fibers sprout small, convoluted side branches that generate lamellate appendages, forming neuron-glial interaction sites. By ensheathing excitatory and inhibitory synapses on PCs, BGs are crucial for neuroprotection, synaptic stability, and synaptic plasticity27,28,29, and thus contribute to neuropathological mechanisms in many diseases30,31,32. DA receptor subtypes are known to be distributed in neuronal types, while our finding reveals that D1Rs are also highly expressed in BGs. Our experiments were designed to assess DAergic modulation of BGs, its effect on PCs, and its role in motor and non-motor behaviors.
Results
DAergic system in the cerebellum
Drd1a-tdTomato and Drd2-EGFP transgenic mice have been widely utilized to characterize the role of D1Rs and D2Rs in the basal ganglia33,34. Using these mice, we found that the expression level of D2R reporter GFP was minimal in the cerebellum; however, the D1R reporter tdTomato was highly enriched (Fig. 1a, Supplementary Fig. 1a). The D1R expression was observed in cells with multiple radial branches, suggestive of cerebellar BGs (Fig. 1a, right). Indeed, immunostained cerebellar sections demonstrated colocalization of tdTomato with the astrocyte marker glial fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP) or S100β (Fig. 1b and Supplementary Fig. 1b), but not with the PC marker calbindin D28K (CB) (Supplementary Fig. 1c), confirming the expression of D1Rs in BGs. As the astrocyte markers only label the soma and the main processes, to fully demonstrate the distribution of D1Rs in BGs, we filled BGs with biocytin followed by tdTomato immunostaining (Fig. 1c). We found that the fine processes of BGs were also enriched with D1Rs. We further performed immunostaining with an antibody selective for D1Rs, which had been demonstrated in the striatum by their overlap with the D1R reporter but not the D2R reporter (Supplementary Fig. 1d). Co-labeling with tdTomato showed that D1Rs were expressed in BG soma and processes (Fig. 1d). Similarly, immunolabelled D1Rs were observed in BGs filled with biocytin (Fig. 1e). Such distribution was also supported by single-molecule fluorescence in situ hybridization (smFISH), showing Drd1a mRNA expression at the border of the Purkinje cell layer (PCL) and molecular layer (ML) (Fig. 1f). We further revealed that the DA transporter (DAT) was also expressed in BGs and PCs (Fig. 1g), by using an antibody selective for DAT1 (Supplementary Fig. 1e). In the cerebellar granule cell layer (GCL), tdTomato was not found in NeuN-positive GCs or GFAP-positive astrocytes (Supplementary Fig. 2a). In the deep cerebellar nuclei, tdTomato was identified in some cells but not in GFAP-positive astrocytes (Supplementary Fig. 2b). Other than the cerebellum, co-immunostaining of tdTomato with GFAP or S100β demonstrated that D1R-expressing cells were generally not astrocytes, as shown in several major brain regions including the cerebral cortex, hippocampus, and striatum (Supplementary Fig. 2c-h).
DA source in the cerebellum
We next sought to identify the source of DA within the cerebellum. DA synthesis is generally thought to involve the rate-limiting enzyme tyrosine hydroxylase (TH)35. Using TH as the marker, we found that DAergic neurons were largely localized in the substantial nigra pars compacta (SNc), VTA, as well as the locus coeruleus (LC) (Supplementary Fig. 3a, b), which is consistent with the reported DA sources in the brain36. To determine if their axonal terminals enter the cerebellum, we injected AAV-DIO-YFP vectors into the SNc/VTA and LC of Slc6a3-Cre mice to express YFP in DAergic neurons (Supplementary Fig. 3c–f). The axonal bundles emanating from the SNc/VTA were clearly seen entering the cerebellum through the superior cerebellar peduncle (SCP) but did not appear to extend into the cerebellar cortex. (Supplementary Fig. 3c–e). YFP-expressing axons from the LC also did not project into the cerebellum (Supplementary Fig. 3f). To further explore these potential pathways, we injected an anterograde fluorescent tracer Dextran-Alexa Fluor 488 into the SNc/VTA and allowed 4 days for its transport (Supplementary Fig. 3g). As shown by YFP expression, the axonal afferents were identified in the SCP but not in the cerebellar cortex (Supplementary Fig. 3h–j). We also injected Retrobeads into crus 1 and 2 of the cerebellum; however, we did not observe apparent retrogradely labeled DAergic neurons in the SNc/VTA (Supplementary Fig. 3k, l). These results indicate that the SNc, VTA, or LC may not be the major source of DA supplied to the cerebellum.
We then explored a member of the cytochrome P450 (CYP) superfamily CYP2Ds that can metabolize tyramine into DA37,38,39,40, and are found in the cerebellum41. Immunostaining using antibodies against N or C terminal CYP2D6 and CB demonstrated that CYP2D was intensely expressed in CB-positive PC dendrites and soma (Fig. 2a and Supplementary Fig. 4a), suggesting that cerebellar DA may be synthesized in this alternative pathway. To detect the DA release following its synthesis, we injected AAV-CAG-dLight1.1 into the cerebellar cortex to express DA biosensor dLight1.1 (Fig. 2b)42. dLight1.1 was highly expressed in S100β-positive BGs but to a lesser degree in CB-positive PCs (Supplementary Fig. 4b). To confirm its sensitivity, we applied exogenous DA to cerebellar slices at different concentrations (0.5, 2, and 8 µM). dLight1.1 signals were robustly evoked in a dose-dependent manner, which was blocked by pretreatment with the D1 antagonist SCH23390 (Fig. 2c). We further assessed activity-dependent DA release by delivering electrical stimuli to dLight1.1-expressing cerebellar slices. Strong dLight1.1 signals were evoked and then abolished following SCH23390 treatment (Fig. 2d). To pharmacologically verify the role of CYP2D in DA synthesis, we performed in vitro and in vivo treatments with a potent CYP2D inhibitor fluoxetine43,44. We expressed dLight1.1 in the cerebellum and later prepared slices for treatment with fluoxetine for 2 h. In a different set of experiments, during dLight1.1 expression, we also administered fluoxetine to mice for 2 wks prior to slice preparation. Intriguingly, in both groups evoked dLight1.1 signals were largely reduced compared with the control (Fig. 2e). A similar effect was seen in slices following in vitro treatments of another CYP2D inhibitor quinidine (Supplementary Fig. 4c)45,46. To exclude the possibility that fluoxetine treatment degraded dLight1.1 itself, we delivered stimuli followed by DA perfusion to the same slice. In the groups that had undergone in vitro and in vivo treatments, stimuli did not trigger dLight1.1 signals, but the following exogenous DA still induced signals (Supplementary Fig. 4d). These data suggest that DA in the cerebellum is produced in PCs through a CYP2D-dependent non-canonical pathway and released in an activity-dependent manner.
Field electrical stimulation in slices inevitably evoked the activity of a population of heterogeneous cells. To examine the immediate role of PC activity in DA release, we first attempted to record dLight1.1 signals during PC firing triggered by injections of sequential positive currents into a cell (Supplementary Fig. 4e). As action potentials from a single PC might be insufficient to induce detectable DA release, no dLight1.1 signals were seen in slices. We then employed chemogenetic stimulation to depolarize a population of PCs by expressing an excitatory designer receptor exclusively activated by a designer drug (DREADD), hM3D(Gq), in the cerebellum of Pcp2-Cre mice. PCs expressing hM3D(Gq) were located in close proximity to dLight1.1-expressing BGs; as expected, bath-application of the DREADD activator clozapine N-oxide (CNO) led to PC depolarization and firing (Fig. 2f). Examination of dLight1.1 signals revealed a significant elevation following CNO perfusion (Fig. 2g), suggesting that DA is released specifically from PCs. To further examine if DA release is related to the action of vesicular monoamine transporter (VMAT)−2, we applied the VMAT-2 inhibitor reserpine to dLight1.1-expressing slices47. dLight1.1 signals were significantly decreased (Fig. 2h), indicating the important role of VMAT-2 in DA release.
Lastly, we compared DA release in the cerebellum with the striatum using in vivo fiber photometry. Examination of dLight1.1 signals in freely moving mice showed that the spontaneous signals in the cerebellum were less frequent and smaller in fluorescence z-score than those in the striatum (Fig. 2i). Altogether, these data show tonic DA release in these two brain regions is differentially regulated, which is consistent with our results showing that DA in the cerebellum is mainly originated from PCs but not from the SNc, VTA, or LC.
DA-mediated membrane depolarization and Ca2+ signaling in BGs
DA is known to promote cell depolarization and excitability in basal ganglia48. To study the effect of D1R activation on membrane potential, we performed current clamp in BGs at their resting membrane potential. Bath-application of the D1 agonist SKF83822 resulted in the depolarization of BG membrane potential (Fig. 3a). To explore the possible contribution of BG depolarization to Ca2+ transients, we infused BGs with the organic Ca2+ indicator Fluo 5F and then monitored Ca2+ signals in response to membrane depolarization induced by short sequential current injections (Supplementary Fig. 5a). Compared with the control, membrane depolarization significantly increased the frequency and peak amplitude of Ca2+ transients in BGs. We subsequently attempted to test if membrane hyperpolarization thwarts SKF83822-induced Ca2+ signals. Interestingly, the hyperpolarization itself also increased Ca2+ activity (Supplementary Fig. 5a)49,50.
We further assessed the direct effect of D1R activation on Ca2+ signals by using the genetically encoded Ca2+ indicator gfaABC1D-GCaMP6f. It was specifically expressed in BGs, as indicated by its colocalization with S100β in immunostaining (Supplementary Fig. 5b); it reliably reflected intense Ca2+ dynamics in BGs (Supplementary Fig. 5c). We found that SKF83822 treatment enhanced the frequency and amplitude of spontaneous Ca2+ signals (Fig. 3b and Supplementary Movie 1). We next tested if Ca2+ is released from the internal store. Pretreatment with the IP3R antagonist 2-APB prevented the effect (Supplementary Movie 2), suggesting Ca2+ rise is at least partially due to intracellular Ca2+ mobilization. Furthermore, inhibition of D1Rs by SCH23390 attenuated the Ca2+ frequency increase and slightly decreased the amplitude. To test if the activity of D2Rs has any effect on Ca2+ rises in BGs, we applied the D2R agonist quinpirole to GCaMP6f-expressing slices. Imaging of Ca2+ signals in BGs did not reveal any alterations in frequency or amplitude (Supplementary Fig. 5d), which agrees with the expression of D2R in PCs but not BGs24. These data suggest that D1Rs activated by DA have a significant role in eliciting Ca2+ rises in BGs.
To test if these results can be replicated by chemogenetically depolarizing BGs, we expressed GFAP-hM3D(Gq) in BGs (Supplementary Fig. 5e). CNO treatment led to depolarization of membrane potential in BGs in a dose-dependent fashion (Fig. 3c). We then co-expressed gfaABC1D-GCaMP6f and GFAP-hM3D(Gq) in BGs (Fig. 3d). Imaging of GCaMP6f in these slices showed that CNO treatment resulted in a robust Ca2+ oscillation in BGs (Fig. 3e). Consistent with the role of IP3 signaling in hM3D(Gq)-induced Ca2+ fluctuation51, 2-APB pretreatment eliminated the oscillation. These data indicate that the D1R activation is involved in membrane depolarization and IP3R-mediated Ca2+ signaling in BGs.
DAergic modulation of AMPARs in BGs
In addition to the fast action, D1Rs may have a role in AMPAR membrane insertion, considering previous reports on this regulation in neurons52,53,54. Therefore, we first characterized the cellular distribution of the major AMPAR subunits in the cerebellum. Dual immunostaining of AMPAR subunits and GFAP or CB showed that GluA1 and GluA4 subunits were localized to BGs, whereas GluA2 was present in PCs (Fig. 4a and Supplementary Fig. 6a), which is consistent with previous reports28,55,56. Immunostaining of these subunits in Drd1a-tdTomato mice also uncovered similar results (Supplementary Fig. 6b). We further loaded BGs with biocytin during whole-cell recordings, followed by immunostaining for GluA1, GluA2, and GluA4, and performed Airyscan confocal microscopy (Fig. 4b). Examination of these subunits on biocytin-filled processes in x-y, y-z, and x-z projections verified the localization of GluA1 and GluA4 in BGs and GluA2 in PCs. As AMPARs lacking GluA2 subunits are inward rectifying and permeable to Ca2+57, the immunostaining result suggests that AMPARs in BGs are mainly GluA2-lacking Ca2+-permeable (CP)-AMPARs. The current-voltage (I-V) relationship of membrane currents evoked in BGs by direct AMPA application, indeed, exhibited the typical inward rectification (Fig. 4c). In BGs filled with the Ca2+ indicator Oregon Green 488 BAPTA-1 (OGB-1), AMPA-induced currents in BGs were also accompanied by intracellular Ca2+ signals (Supplementary Fig. 6c). AMPA currents and their associated Ca2+ signals were sensitive to a selective CP-AMPAR blocker NASPM, and completely blocked by the AMPAR antagonist CNQX. These electrophysiological and Ca2+ imaging data demonstrate that cerebellar BGs express GluA2-lacking CP-AMPARs.
We then determined whether pharmacological activation of D1Rs modulates AMPAR subunits in BGs. We treated cerebellar slices with SKF83822 for 1 h and prepared homogenates for Western immunoblotting. SKF83822 treatment did not alter the total protein levels of GluA1 and GluA4 subunits (Fig. 4d). However, surface biotinylation assay on GluA1 and GluA4 in slices showed that SKF83822 increased surface levels of GluA1 and to a lesser degree GluA4 (Fig. 4e). This effect on GluA1 in BGs was blocked by the protein kinase A (PKA) inhibitor Rp-cAMPs, consistent with the role of PKA in D1R-mediated GluA1 trafficking in neurons58. It is known that GluA1 Ser831 is phosphorylated by protein kinase C (PKC) and Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent kinase II (CaMKII), whereas Ser845 is phosphorylated by PKA59. Congruent with the involvement of PKA in D1R activation, SKF83822 treatment resulted in phosphorylation of GluA1 at Ser845 but not at Ser831 (Fig. 4f). To further investigate the role of D1Rs in regulating GluA1 and GluA4 subunits, we generated Drd1 conditional knockout (cKO) in BGs by crossing Slc1a3-Cre mice to Drd1tm2.1 mice followed by administration of tamoxifen to their offspring28,60. Immunostaining in cerebellar sections from these mice showed the lack of D1R immunoreactivity in BGs (Supplementary Fig. 6d). Surface biotinylation assay on AMPAR subunits in the cerebellum of these mice showed that surface levels of GluA1 and GluA4 but not GluA2 were downregulated compared with controls (Fig. 4g). These data demonstrate that D1Rs have a critical role in surface insertion and homeostasis of AMPAR subunits in BGs.
BG modulation of synaptic input to PCs through D1R activation
BGs can profoundly impact the activity of neighboring PCs by actively releasing neurotransmitters or passively altering their reuptake26. To study the modulation of PC activity by D1R activation, we delivered alternating stimuli to electrophysiologically-identified PFs and CFs and recorded postsynaptic currents (PSCs) in PCs during SKF83822 perfusion (Supplementary Fig. 7a, b). In particular, to examine CF input to PCs, we ensured that the afferent path was stimulated and the PSCs were not unclamped (Supplementary Fig. 7c, d). Surprisingly, without blockade of GABAergic activity, SKF83822 treatment resulted in a large reduction in the amplitude of PSCs at both PF-PC and CF-PC synapses (Supplementary Fig. 7e). It should be noted that in a few cases especially in CF-PC synapses, there existed a transient increase in PSC amplitude lasting about 1–2 min (Supplementary Fig. 7f).
We first investigated if MLIs contribute to the reduction of PSC amplitude because MLIs provide dense inhibitory innervations on PCs. Indeed, dual immunostaining of CB and parvalbumin (PV) demonstrated that PCs were innervated by a large portion of puncta derived from PV-positive MLIs (Supplementary Fig. 7g). In biocytin-filled MLIs, numerous axonal terminals were opposed to the soma and dendrites of PCs (Supplementary Fig. 7h). Considering the space clamp from distal dendrites in our recordings, the currents generated by MLI activity could thwart inward currents, leading to a decrease in PSC amplitude. Indeed, when PCs were clamped to a more depolarized state, GABAAR-mediated outward currents evoked at −40 mV were evident (Supplementary Fig. 7i). Notably, when PCs were held at a similar depolarizing potential, SKF83822 increased GABAAR-mediated outward currents (Supplementary Fig. 7j), suggesting that an increase in MLI activity is one factor contributing to the reduced PC excitation.
To further dissect cellular mechanisms, we recorded from PCs spontaneous inhibitory PSCs (sIPSCs) in the presence of the AMPAR antagonist NBQX, the NMDAR antagonist D-AP5, and high intracellular Cl- (140 mM) and recorded spontaneous excitatory PSCs (sEPSCs) in the presence of picrotoxin and low intracellular Cl- (17.5 mM) (Fig. 5a and Supplementary Fig. 7k). SKF83822 was shown to increase the frequency of sIPSCs although it had no effect on their amplitude. By also examining sEPSCs during SKF83822 treatment, we found that their frequency underwent a marked reduction preceded by a transient enhancement while their amplitude remained unaltered. Based on this evidence, we infer that the increase in sIPSC frequency and the decrease in sEPSC could explain the reduction of evoked PSCs during SKF83822 application. To provide chemogenetic evidence for D1R-mediated modulation of PCs, we applied CNO to hM3D(Gq)-expressing BGs and recorded PSCs following PF stimuli. CNO that exerted similarly to SKF83822 on BG Ca2+ signals (Fig. 3c-e), also reduced the amplitude of evoked PSCs (Supplementary Fig. 7l).
Lastly, to examine the effect of BG modulation on PC output, we evaluated spontaneous PC spiking by using cell-attached recordings on PCs (Fig. 5b). We found that SKF83822 significantly reduced the frequency of spikes in PCs. In addition, the firing regularity was also decreased, as indicated by an increase in the coefficient of variation 2 (CV2) (Fig. 5c). Collectively, these data suggest that modulation of BGs by D1R activation has an overall inhibitory effect on PC activity.
Decreased locomotor activity but intact eyeblink conditioning in Drd1 cKO mice
To test if the DAergic system is implicated in behaviors, we conducted a battery of behavioral tests in Drd1 cKO mice. First, we performed an open field test in Drd1 cKO mice to evaluate locomotor and exploratory activity. Compared with control mice, Drd1 cKO mice exhibited a lower level of locomotor activity as evaluated by the total distance traveled in the arena (Fig. 6a). Further analyses showed no difference in the percent time spent and the percent distance traveled in the central area (Fig. 6a1–a3), suggesting normal anxiety levels in Drd1 cKO mice. To test if D1R activation has an opposite effect on motility, we performed the test in mice that had received bilateral intracranial microinfusion of SKF83822 in the cerebellum for 5 days. SKF83822 microinfusion significantly enhanced the distance traveled, compared with the distance traveled prior to the treatment (Fig. 6b). Furthermore, we used the rotarod and the ladder rung walking tests to evaluate motor coordination in Drd1 cKO mice. The latency to fall from the rotarod was significantly shorter in these mice (Fig. 6c). There was also an increasing trend for the number of foot slip errors in the ladder rung walking test (Supplementary Fig. 8a). Automated gait analysis did not reveal a significant difference in the average stride length of right forepaw (RF), right hindpaw (RH), left forepaw (LF), and left hindpaw (LH) (Supplementary Fig. 8b). We further performed grip strength test to evaluate the neuromuscular function in Drd1 cKO. Front grip strength was significantly decreased compared with control mice (Fig. 6d). In addition, we used the marble burying test to examine repetitive digging behaviors and the nestlet building to evaluate general well-being (Supplementary Fig. 8c, d). No differences between control and Dr1d cKO mice were found in the number of marbles buried and the score of nestlet building.
We next determined if the deletion of Drd1 from BGs impacts delay and trace eyeblink conditioning, two types of motor learning that involve the cerebellum and the hippocampus61. In the delay eyeblink conditioning, the conditioned stimulus (CS) was paired with the unconditioned stimulus (US), with the CS preceding the US and ending concomitantly with the US. In contrast, in the trace eyeblink conditioning the CS and US are separated by a stimulus-free interval. In both conditioning, control and Drd1 cKO mice were subjected to a protocol consisting of acquisition, extinction, reacquisition, and retention (Fig. 6e, f). After 12 sessions of pairings mice showed a significant percentage of conditioned responses (CRs). Following the acquisition mice underwent 3 sessions of extinction, where only the CS was presented. The percentage of CRs displayed a rapid decline. In the reacquisition phase, mice quickly reached an asymptotic CR rate in 3 sessions. To test the long-term retention, mice received CS-US pairings again after a 2-wk break. Mice fast recalled the task, showing high percent CRs within 2 sessions. When percent CRs were compared between control and Drd1 cKO mice, no difference in either delay or trace eyeblink conditioning was observed in any phase. Moreover, CR amplitude measured by fraction eye closure (FEC) from the CS-alone presentation was also indistinguishable between two groups (Fig. 6e, f and Supplementary Fig. 9a, b). To ask why the eyeblink conditioning is intact in Drd1 cKO mice, we evaluated cellular activity by performing immunostaining of the immediate early gene c-fos, as used previously in eyeblink conditioning62. The number of c-fos-positive cells or the intensity of c-fos immunofluorescence serves as an estimate of neuronal activity63. Indeed, as the hippocampus is required for conditioning, there was an increased number of c-fos-positive cells in control and Drd1 cKO mice, compared with naïve mice (Supplementary Fig. 10a, b). As no discrete c-fos-positive cells were identified in the cerebellum, the intensity of c-fos was measured as an alternative metric. In contrast to the hippocampus, we did not find any difference between the control, Drd1 cKO, and naïve mice after either delay or trace eyeblink conditioning (Supplementary Fig. 10c, d). These data indicate that eyeblink conditioning does not involve strong neuronal activity in the cerebellum and that D1Rs may not have a critical role.
DAergic modulation of social activity
Apart from motor activity, the cerebellum is known to be involved in social activity and cognitive function61,64. To determine whether Drd1 cKO affects social behaviors, we used a three-chamber interaction arena to test sociability and social memory sequentially. For the sociability test, mice were allowed to freely explore either a chamber containing a novel mouse (S1) restrained under an inverted cup or a chamber containing an empty inverted cup (E). Control mice spent significantly more time interacting with S1 than with E (Fig. 7a, b). However, Drd1 cKO mice showed no significant preference between the S1 and E. As locomotor activity may affect the number of contacts, we calculated the preference index by normalizing the time difference to the total time (Supplementary Fig. 11). The preference index of interaction time in control was statistically different than chance (one-sample t test against chance p < 0.05), and higher than Drd1 cKO mice. Immediately following the sociability test, we placed a second novel mouse (S2) under the previously empty cup and allowed the test mice to explore both chambers. Indicative of social memory for S1 and their preference for S2, control mice spent more time interacting with S2 (Fig. 7a, b and Supplementary Fig. 11). However, Drd1 cKO did not show a preference between S1 and S2. These data suggest that Drd1 cKO mice demonstrate impaired social interaction and social memory.
Levels of mammalian target of rapamycin (mTOR) signaling are known to be positively correlated with impaired social interaction, and treatment with the mTOR inhibitor rapamycin improves social interaction deficits65. Consistently, in Drd1 cKO mice with impaired social activity, phosphorylation levels of mTOR were significantly higher (Fig. 7c). We also evaluated levels of phosphorylated mTOR in slices treated with SKF83822 (Fig. 7d). SKF83822 treatment resulted in a decrease in p-mTOR levels, which were prevented by pretreatment with SCH23390. These data suggest that DAergic activity in the cerebellum is critically engaged in social behavior by acting through mTOR signaling.
Discussion
Our study demonstrates the expression of DA, D1R, and DAT in the cerebellum, suggesting an intrinsic cerebellar DAergic system. Previous studies reported that D1Rs are expressed in cerebellar ML and/or PCL23,66,67,68. Here, we further demonstrate that D1Rs are expressed in BGs while DAT is in both BGs and PCs. Astrocytes were previously found to express several DA receptor types69,70,71. Pharmacological activation of D1Rs with their agonists activates PKA and ERK1/2 in cultured striatal astrocytes to promote astrocyte migration69,72. Stimulation of D1Rs also triggers Ca2+ transients in astrocytes depending on the NAD + /NADH redox state73,74. By exploiting the pharmacological approach and DA sensor imaging, we provide solid evidence that DA can be synthesized in PCs by the enzyme CYP2D. Several studies demonstrate that human CYP2D6 or rodent CYP2Ds can metabolize m-tyramine and p-tyramine into DA37,38,39,40. More notably, CYP2D6 activity is associated with the incidence and prevalence of brain disorders. In PD patients, CYP2D6 protein levels are largely reduced in the cerebellum of PD patients;75 CYP2D6 polymorphism leads to a higher susceptibility to PD76. Individuals with CYP2D6 variants also present personality, cognition, and psychopathological vulnerability77. Our identification of CYP2D for DA synthesis does not entirely exclude the possibility of the production of DA by TH. Modest TH expression was observed in PCs in the vermal lobules V and VI78. Axonal fibers derived from the VTA were sparsely found in the deep cerebellar nuclei or in crus 1, and 2;79 fibers labeled with anti-TH were shown unevenly in the ML, PCL, and GL of a few lobules, such as V and VI80,81. All these findings indicate low TH expression in a limited number of lobules, which is consistent with our result from TH immunolabeling. Conversely, CYP2D is expressed in all lobules throughout the cerebellum, commensurate with D1R expression. We further demonstrate that DA detected by its sensor was abolished by in vitro or in vivo treatments of the CYP2D inhibitor. These data strongly suggest that CYP2D in PCs is the major enzyme required for DA synthesis in the cerebellum. This finding is also congruent with a previous report showing that strong depolarization in PCs can trigger vesicular DA from PCs23. Nevertheless, these immunostaining and pharmacological results must be further substantiated in mice lacking CYP2D specifically in the cerebellum. Furthermore, to confirm the direct action of released DA on BGs, for our future experiments it is necessary to use viral vectors that express DA sensors specifically in BGs.
Our data show that D1R activation depolarizes membrane potential and induces intracellular Ca2+ signals. BGs express high a density of inwardly rectifying K+ channels, which are critical for maintaining glial membrane potentials82. BG depolarization could be caused by D1R-mediated inhibition of inwardly rectifying K+ channels (Kir), as seen in striatal neurons83. Depolarization of membrane potential can be a contributing factor to the fast rises of Ca2+ signals. In addition to these rapid responses, D1R treatment also yields long-term cellular modification by assisting the insertion of GluA1 into the BG membrane. This GluA1 membrane trafficking involves PKA activity and phosphorylation of GluA1 at Ser845, similar to that observed in neurons58,59. The increase of GluA2-lacking CP-AMPARs, resulting from GluA1 insertion, would prolong membrane depolarization and Ca2+ influx. Fast Ca2+ rises from the internal store and slow rises mediated by CP-AMPARs are expected to affect PC activity and behavior significantly. The D1R-mediated signaling is generally thought to be associated with Gs-coupled receptor stimulation84. Our data indeed show that D1R activation promotes GluA1 insertion, which can be blocked by inhibiting protein kinase A (PKA), a downstream signal of Gs. Furthermore, our pharmacological experiments also reveal that D1Rs could induce Ca2+ transients by signaling through Gq-coupled receptors and their downstream effector IP3Rs. This effect can be mimicked by the chemogenetic stimulation of excitatory hM3DGq DREADD receptors. Consistently, one study showed that 2-APB treatment could block D1R-induced IP3R signaling85. More notably, evidence demonstrates that D1R also stimulates Gq in astrocytes74,86. Together with these and other previous reports87,88,89,90,91, we speculate that D1R-mediated signaling can act through both Gs and Gq, which work separately or together in triggering Ca2+ rises from different sources in BGs.
BG activity is long known to impact PC synaptic transmission and plasticity26. Depolarization of a single BG induces a significant increase in the frequency of sEPSCs recorded in an adjacent PC, which was believed to be caused by modulation of presynaptic glutamate release92. Increasing Ca2+ signals in BGs results in a decrease in extracellular K+, thereby altering PC membrane potential and transiently increasing spike activity93.
In agreement with these previous reports, the D1R agonist that can depolarize BGs induces an initial increase in sEPSC frequency in PCs. This enhancement, however, is transient; a persistent reduction ensues after extended exposure. We are uncertain how depolarized membrane potential and increased Ca2+ signals in BGs, mechanistically, lead to these alterations in PCs. Considering the rapid action and the existence of presynaptic glutamate receptor (GluR) distribution, we postulate that the increased sEPSC frequency may be caused by activation of presynaptic ionotropic GluRs94,95 and that the subsequent decrease may be due to a strong delayed inhibition of presynaptic metabotropic GluR-mediated transmitter release96,97. Our data further demonstrate that the inhibitory input is upregulated in PCs, suggesting that neurotransmitters (e.g., glutamate) released from BGs activate both GABAergic MLIs and PC presynaptic sites and result in reduced overall neurotransmission at both PF-PC and CF-PC synapses. Toning down synaptic transmission leads to decreased PC excitability, which may serve as a common mechanism underlying cerebellum-related behaviors98. In the hippocampus, a similar circuit inhibition is mediated by DA’s action on the cortical input99,100.
Ca2+ rise in BGs is associated with locomotion101,102. Consistently, our data show that microinfusion of the D1 agonist in the cerebellum augments locomotor activity, while Drd1 cKO mice exhibit decreased locomotor activity. We did not see an impairment in cerebellum-related delay or trace eyeblink conditioning. Immunostaining for c-fos did not show a significant increase in intensity after eyeblink conditioning in either control or Drd1 cKO mice, suggesting BGs or PCs are not strongly activated. Whether the deep cerebellar nuclei or cerebellar cortex is a hub for eyeblink conditioning is still under debate103. Our findings suggest that the cerebellar cortex might have a less important role in eyeblink conditioning. However, it is also possible that c-fos immunostaining may not be an ideal tool to detect accumulating cell activity if these cells like PCs and BGs are intrinsically active. In comparison with the importance of BGs in social interaction, we speculate that in BGs, the subcellular and molecular signaling required for eyeblink conditioning and other cerebellar functions may be profoundly different. As the cerebellar cortex is critical for the timing of the conditioning104, we also cannot exclude the possibility that delay or trace eyeblink conditioning with other intervals may display the deficit. Collectively, these findings suggest that the DA pathway in the cerebellum is involved in social activity but likely not in classical conditioning.
In summary, our results reveal the molecular, cellular, and circuit mechanisms underlying the DA system in the cerebellum (Fig. 8). We show that PCs synthesize and release DA in an activity-dependent manner. The released DA binds D1Rs in BGs and then promotes membrane depolarization, Ca2+ signaling, and membrane insertion of AMPAR GluA1 subunits. These actions likely trigger glutamate release from BGs, which then enhances interneuron activity and reduces PF- and CF-PC synaptic transmission. The altered synaptic input to PCs modulates their firing frequency and pattern, ultimately impacting locomotor and social behaviors. These findings indicate that the cerebellar DAergic system has a critical pathophysiological role in many disorders associated with motor and social dysfunction.
Methods
Animal models
Animals were handled and housed according to the Committee on Laboratory Animal Resources of the National Institutes of Health. Both male and female mice at age of 2–3 months were used in our study. Mice were housed at the room temperature (25 °C) with a 12-h light/dark cycle and humidity between 40 and 60%. Mice were provided ad libitum access to food and water. All experimental protocols were reviewed and approved annually by the Institutional Animals Care and Use Committee of the University of Alabama at Birmingham (IACUC-22247). Previous reports have described the generation and genotyping of the following mouse lines: Drd1-tdTomato33, Drd2-EGFP34, Slc6a3-Cre105, Pcp2-Cre106, Slc1a3-Cre107, and Drd1tm2.1108. Drd1a-tdTomato mice express the fluorescent tdTomato in D1R-expressing cells, while Drd2-EGFP mice express the fluorescent EGFP in D2R-exprssing cells. Slc6a3-Cre, Pcp2-Cre, and Slc1a3-Cre mice express Cre recombinase in DAergic neurons, most PCs, and glia, respectively. We followed the genotyping and husbandry protocols provided by the vendors. For Cre-expressing mice, the heterozygous offspring were used. To generate Drd1 cKO mice, Slc1a3-Cre mice were crossed to Drd1tm2.1 mice and tamoxifen (T5648, Sigma-Aldrich) prepared in corn oil was administered (i.p., 100 mg/kg) for 5 consecutive days to their offspring starting at postnatal day 30 (PND 30). Homozygous Drd1 cKO mice were used for experiments at age of 3 months.
Stereotactic injections
All adeno-associated viruses (AAVs) were obtained from Addgene and delivered via stereotactic intracranial injections. Mice at PND 20-27 were anesthetized with 4% isoflurane in 100% oxygen gas; anesthesia was maintained with 1-2.5% isoflurane. Mice were placed in a stereotactic frame (David Kopf Instruments), and their body temperature was maintained with a heating pad. The scalp was shaved and then sterilized with 70% ethanol. A rostral-caudal incision was made to access the skull, a hole was drilled, and virus (0.5 µl at a titer of 0.5-1×1013 GC/ml) was delivered through a 2.5 µl syringe (Hamilton Company) at a rate of 0.25 µl/min using a microsyringe pump (UMP3 UltraMicroPump, Micro4, World Precision Instruments). For expression of the DA sensor, Ca2+ indicator, and excitatory DREADD, pAAV5-CAG-dLight1.1, pZac2.1-gfaABC1D-cyto-GCaMP6f, and pAAV-GFAP-hM3D(Gq)-mCherry was injected into the cerebellum, respectively (AP = −6.75 mm, ML = −1.8 mm, DV = −1.2 mm). For expression of the DA sensor in the striatum, pAAV5-CAG-dLight1.1 was injected (AP = 0.7 mm, ML = −1.75 mm, DV = −2.9 mm). For expression of YFP in DAergic neurons, pAAV5-Ef1a-DIO EYFP was injected into the VTA and SNc (AP = −3.0 mm, ML = ±1.3 mm, DV = −4.1 mm), or the LC (AP = −5.52 mm, ML = ±0.8 mm, DV = −3.0 mm) of Slc6a3-Cre mice. Following the injections, the incision was closed with surgical glue. Topical antibiotic ointment (bacitracin zinc, neomycin sulfate, and polymyxin B sulfates; Actavis) was applied to the incision, and carprofen (i.p. 5 mg/kg; Zoetis) was administered. Mice were used for experiments 3-4 wks later.
For identification of the possible DAergic input from the SNc to the cerebellum, mice at PND 40 were injected with Dextran-Alexa Fluor 488 (500 nl, Thermo Fisher Scientific) in the SNc and retrobeads (200 nl, Lumafluor) in the cerebellar crus 1 and 2. Mice were used for experiments 4 d later.
In vitro cerebellar slices
Mice at 2-3 months were deeply anesthetized with a ketamine and xylazine mixture, and transcardially perfused with ice-cold cutting artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) containing (in mM): 87 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 0.5 CaCl2, 7 MgCl2, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 25 NaHCO3, 25 glucose, and 75 sucrose, bubbled with 95% O2/5% CO2. The brain was rapidly removed and cut transversely at 300 µm using a vibratome (VT1200S, Leica Microsystems). Slices were transferred to normal aCSF containing (in mM): 119 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 2.5 CaCl2, 1.3 MgCl2, 1.3 NaH2PO4, 26 NaHCO3, and 20 glucose, at 32 °C for 30 min and then allowed to recover for 1 h at room temperature before recordings.
Whole-cell patch clamp electrophysiology
Individual slices were transferred to a submerged chamber mounted on a fixed-stage upright microscope (Axio Examiner.D1, Zeiss) and continuously perfused with normal oxygenated aCSF at room temperature. Signals in BGs or PCs were acquired with a MultiClamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices), filtered at 2 kHz, and digitized at 10 kHz with ITC-18 A/D-D/A interfaces (Instrutech)109. Input resistance was measured with hyperpolarizing voltage pulses (50 ms, 20 mV). Cells with series resistances above 25 MΩ were discarded, and cells were also excluded if any whole-cell parameter (i.e. Cm, Ri, Rs) changed by ≥ 20% during the recordings.
For current or voltage clamps in BGs, the intracellular solution contained (in mM): 135 K-gluconate, 10 KCl, 10 HEPES, 1 MgCl2, 2 Mg-ATP, 0.3 Na-GTP (290-300 mOsm, pH 7.3, 4-5 MΩ). To evaluate the effect of D1R activation on cell-intrinsic property, BGs were recorded at resting membrane potential (RMP) and membrane potential was monitored in slices treated with SKF83822 (10 µM, Tocris). For slices expressing hM3D(Gq), membrane potential in BGs was recorded during CNO (1 µM, Tocris) treatment. To test inward rectification in BGs, S-AMPA (10 µM, 20-ms duration, Tocris) was pressure puffed to slices in the presence of the GABAAR antagonist picrotoxin (50 µM, Tocris), the NMDAR antagonist D-AP5 (50 µM, Tocris), and cyclothiazide (CTZ; 50 µM, Tocris). The internal solution was supplemented with spermine (100 µM, Tocris). NASPM (50 µM, Tocris) was used to selectively inhibit the activity of GluA2-lacking AMPARs; the AMPAR antagonist CBQX (20 µM, Tocris) was used to completely block all AMPAR channels.
To evaluate synaptic input to PCs, PFs and CFs were alternately stimulated with a 10-s interval by using aCSF-filled pipettes connected to an isolated stimulator (ISO-Flex, AMPI); PSCs were recorded in PCs held at −60 mV under the voltage-clamp mode. The intracellular solution contained (mM): 120 Cs-gluconate, 17.5 CsCl, 10 Na-HEPES, 4 Mg-ATP, 0.4 Na-GTP, 10 Na2-creatine phosphate, 0.2 Na-EGTA (290-300 mOsm, pH 7.3, 3-4 MΩ). To record PSCs without the addition of the Na+ channel blocker QX314 (5 mM, Tocris) to the intracellular solution, stimulus intensity was adjusted to ensure that EPSCs were clamped. Following the stable baseline recording, SKF83822 was applied to slices in normal aCSF. The amplitude of PSCs was analyzed during SKF83822 treatment. In slices expressing hM3D(Gq), CNO (1 μM) was perfused to slices. PC spikes were then recorded at near firing threshold under the current-clamp mode, or evoked PSCs were recorded at PF-PC synapses under the voltage-clamp mode. To record spontaneous spiking in PCs, aCSF-filled pipettes formed a loose seal on PCs and action potentials were recorded under the cell-attached mode. The number of spikes was binned each min and compared during SKF83822 treatment. The coefficient of variation (CV) 2 was defined as \(\frac{2\times|{{ISI}}_{i+1}-{{ISI}}_{i}|}{{{ISI}}_{i+1}+{{ISI}}_{i}}\), where ISIi indicates the ith inter-spike interval. The average CV2 of all consecutive spikes was calculated for a neuron.
Spontaneous EPSCs (sEPSCs) in PCs were recorded with membrane voltage held at −60 mV in the presence of picrotoxin. Spontaneous IPSCs (sIPSCs) in PCs were recorded at −60 mV in the presence of NBQX (20 µM) and D-AP5 (50 µM). The intracellular solution for sEPSC recordings is described as above. The intracellular solution for sIPSC recordings contained (in mM): 140 CsCl, 2 MgCl2, 10 HEPES, 5 EGTA, 4 Mg-ATP, 0.4 Na-GTP, 0.5 CaCl2. sEPSCs and sIPSCs were analyzed using the MiniAnalysis program (Synaptosoft) with the detection threshold set at 8 pA. Their frequency and amplitude were binned and then analyzed.
Ca2+ and DA sensor imaging
For Ca2+ imaging with the organic Ca2+ indicators, Fluo 5F (100 μM, Thermo Fisher Scientific) or OGB-1 (200 μM, Thermo Fisher Scientific) was included in the intracellular solution and imaged in BGs 15 min after whole-cell access. For the Fluo 5F imaging, Alexa Fluor 594 (100 μM, Thermo Fisher Scientific) was also dissolved in the solution and introduced into the cell. Ca2+ imaging with the genetically encoded Ca2+ indicator was performed in BGs expressing AAV5-gfaABC1D-cyto-GCaMP6f. Fluo 5 F, OGB-1, GCaMP6f was excited by a laser-LED hybrid source using less than 10% of full power (460-495 nm, X-Cite Turbo) and signals were detected with a quantitative electron-multiplying CCD camera (QuantEM:5125 C, Photometrics). For fast recordings of Ca2+ signals, images were acquired with 60-ms exposures under continuous light excitation, whereas for recording of slow spontaneous signals, images were captured at 1 frame/s using a light-dark discontinuous mode. Fluo 5F imaging was performed in control BGs held at −80 mV, or BGs depolarized to −20 mV or hyperpolarized to −120 mV three times at 0.1 Hz with 1-s duration each, and Ca2+ signals were evaluated during the 10-min recordings. OGB-1 imaging was performed in BGs in combination with whole-cell recordings of membrane of potentials, and Ca2+ signals induced by local application of AMPA were evaluated during treatments of NASPM and CNQX. These Ca2+ signals were measured within regions of interest (ROIs) defined over the proximal region of BG soma after the background fluorescence intensities had been subtracted; fluorescence bleaching was corrected by exponential fit subtraction. Ca2+ signals obtained via GCaMP6f imaging in BGs were analyzed using AQuA software as described previously110. To examine the role of D1R activation in BG Ca2+ signaling, SKF83822, SKF83822 in the presence of 2-APB (50 µM, Tocris), or SCH23390 (10 µM, Tocris) was applied during imaging. In cerebellar slices expressing both GCaMP6f and hM3D(Gq), CNO was perfused to activate hM3D(Gq). Ca2+ frequency or peak ΔF/F was compared before (2-min duration) with after current injections or pharmacological treatments (8-min duration).
DA sensor imaging was performed in slices expressing AAV5-CAG-dLight1.142. DA was applied at different concentrations (0.5, 2, and 8 µM) to control slices or slices pretreated with SCH23390 (10 µM). dLight1.1 imaging was also conducted in mice or slices treated with fluoxetine, quinidine, or reserpine. Background-subtracted fluorescence intensity was measured within randomly selected ROIs in cerebellar molecular layer, and the amplitude of dLight1.1 intensity was calculated.
Immunostaining
Mice were anesthetized and transcardially perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS. Brains were removed and postfixed overnight at 4 °C. Sagittal or coronal brain sections were cut at 60 μm using a vibratome. Sections were permeabilized with 0.25% Triton X-100 for 2 h and blocked with 10% normal goat serum for 1 h. Sections were incubated at 4 °C overnight with blocking solution containing the following primary antibodies: mouse anti-GFAP (1:500, Sigma-Aldrich), rabbit anti-DsRed (1:2000, TaKaRa), mouse anti-RFP (1:1000, Rockland Immunochemicals), mouse anti-D1R (1:200, Sigma-Aldrich), rabbit anti-D1R (1:200, Abcam), mouse anti-DAT (1:200, Thermo Fisher Scientific), rabbit anti-GFP (1:2000, Abcam), chicken anti-GFP (1:2000, Abcam), mouse anti-calbindin D28K (CB) (1:500, Santa Cruz Biotechnology), rabbit anti-N terminal CYP2D6 (1:500; AV41675, Sigma-Aldrich), rabbit anti-C terminal CYP2D6 (1:500; PA5-79129, Thermo Fisher Scientific), mouse anti-TH (1:500, Sigma-Aldrich), rabbit anti-mCherry (1:2000, Abcam), rabbit anti-S100β (1:800, Sigma-Aldrich), guinea pig anti-c-fos (1:800, Synaptic Systems), or guinea pig anti-PV (1:800, Synaptic Systems). After the primary antibodies, sections were rinsed with PBS three times each 10 min and incubated for 2 h at room temperature with the secondary antibodies, including Alexa Fluor 488 goat anti-mouse, Alexa Fluor 488 goat anti-rabbit, Alexa Fluor 488 goat anti-chicken, Alexa Fluor 488 goat anti-guinea pig, Alexa Fluor 594 goat anti-mouse, Alexa Fluor 594 goat anti-rabbit (1:1000, Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories). Sections were coverslipped with Vectashield mounting media (Vector Laboratories). All images were acquired using ×10 (0.3 NA), ×20 (0.8 NA), or ×63 (1.4 NA) objectives in an LSM-800 Airyscan confocal microscope (Zeiss). For counting the number of puncta and measuring the fluorescence intensity of immunostaining, images were acquired with the same settings of laser power and PMT voltage and gain for semi-quantitative comparison using ImageJ (NIH). The number of puncta was counted by unbiased ImageJ particle analysis with defined puncta size range.
To identify the distribution of AMPAR subunits in the cerebellum, BGs or PCs in acute slices were filled with biocytin (8 mM, Sigma-Aldrich) during whole-cell recordings. To minimize biocytin diffusion among BGs, slices were pretreated with the gap junction blocker carbenoxolone (50 μM, Tocris) to minimize biocytin diffusion. Following the fixation, permeabilization, and blocking, slices were incubated with rabbit anti-GluA1(1:500, Sigma-Aldrich), rabbit anti-GluA2 (1:500, Thermo Fisher Scientific), mouse anti-GluA2 (1:500, Thermo Fisher Scientific), or rabbit anti-GluA4 (1:500, Sigma-Aldrich). Slices were then incubated with streptavidin-conjugated Alexa Fluor 488 (1:200, Thermo Fisher Scientific) and Alexa Fluor 594 goat anti-mouse or anti-rabbit secondary antibodies. z-stack confocal images of BGs or PCs and GluA1, GluA2, or GluA4 immunostaining were acquired using LSM-800 Airyscan confocal microscope and their spatial relationship was evaluated in three dimensions. To identify the distribution of D1Rs in BGs, BGs were filled with biocytin and immunostained with anti-DsRed in Drd1a-tdTomato slices or anti-D1R in control slices. To identify the path of CFs to PCs, the anterograde tracer Dextran-Alexa Fluor 594 (500 nl, Thermo Fisher Scientific) was injected into the inferior olive and allowed 4 days for labeling CF afferents in the cerebellum, followed by immunostaining with CB for labeling PCs.
Single-molecule RNA FISH
Mice were transcardially perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS. Brains were removed and postfixed overnight at 4 °C. Sagittal sections were cut at 30 μm. Sections were washed three times with PBS and permeabilized in 70% ethanol at 4 °C for 5 min. Sections were then washed in Stellaris Wash buffer A for 5 min at room temperature. Sections were transferred into hybridization buffer containing Dr1d mRNA probes (0.5 nM) and incubated for 15 h at 37 °C with gentle rocking. Sections were washed three times in Stellaris Wash buffer A at 37 °C, and then in Stellaris Wash buffer B for 5 min. Sections were mounted and coverslipped. All images were acquired using LSM-800 Airyscan confocal microscope with a 20× (0.8 NA) objective.
Western immunoblotting
Fresh whole cerebellar samples or acute slices were homogenized in NP-40 buffer (20 mM Tris, pH 8.0, 137 mM NaCl, 10% glycerol, 1% Nonidet P-40, 2 mM EDTA) containing protease and phosphatase inhibitor cocktail (Thermo Fisher Scientific). The homogenates were maintained with constant agitation for 2 h at 4 °C and centrifuged at 10,000 g for 20 min. The supernatants were aspirated and protein concentrations determined by the Lowry method. Equal amounts of protein sample were denatured in loading buffer (125 mM Tris at pH 6.8, 20% glycerol, 6% SDS, and 5% 2-mercaptoethanol), boiled for 3 min, and subjected to SDS-PAGE. Proteins were transferred to PVDF membrane and blocked with 5% nonfat milk in TBST (20 mM Tris, pH 7.6, 150 mM NaCl, and 0.1% Tween-20) for 1 h. Membranes were incubated with rabbit anti-GluA1(1:500, Sigma-Aldrich), rabbit anti-GluA2 (1:500, Thermo Fisher Scientific), mouse anti-GluA2 (1:500, Thermo Fisher Scientific), rabbit anti-GluA4 (1:500, Sigma-Aldrich), rabbit anti-p831 GluA1 (1:200, Sigma-Aldrich), rabbit anti-p845 GluA1 (1:200, Thermo Fisher Scientific), rabbit anti-mTOR (1:800, Cell Signaling Technology), or rabbit anti-p2448 mTOR (1:500, Cell Signaling Technology). After rinsed with PBS three times each 10 min, the membrane was incubated for 2 h at room temperature with corresponding peroxidase-conjugated secondary antibodies (1:2000, Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories). The proteins were detected using the Pierce ECL Substrate (Thermo Fisher Scientific), and signals were captured on autoradiography film and quantified by computer-assisted densitometry. Membranes were re-probed for the loading control with β-actin (1:2000, Thermo Fisher Scientific) and detected with IRDye 800CW goat anti-mouse using Odyssey infrared imaging system (Li-Cor Bioscience).
Surface biotinylation
Slices were rinsed with ice-cold PBS for 30 min and then incubated with Sulfo-NHS-SS-biotin (1 mg/ml, Thermo Scientific) at 4 °C for 45 min. Unreacted biotinylation reagents were quenched in buffer containing 192 mM glycine, 25 mM Tris, pH 8.3). Slices were lysed in NP-40 lysis buffer and protein concentration of each lysate was quantified. Equal amounts of protein lysates were incubated overnight with streptavidin agarose beads (Thermo Scientific). Beads were then washed three times with ice-cold lysis buffer, and biotinylated protein were eluted with sample buffer. Surface or total proteins were then subjected to Western immunoblotting.
Fluoxetine, quinidine, or reserpine treatment
In vitro fluoxetine or quinidine treatment was performed in acute cerebellar slices that expressed dLight1.1. Slices were submerged in a 12-well plate connected to a custom-built perfusion system, and perfused with fluoxetine (100 μM) or quinidine (50 µM) for 2 h at a rate of 3 ml/min. In vitro reserpine (1 µM) treatment was performed in slices for 10 min. In vivo fluoxetine treatment was performed by administering fluoxetine (i.p., 10 mg/kg) for 2 wks to mice that expressed dLight1.1 in the cerebellum and acute cerebellar slices were then prepared. Control groups received vehicle (DMSO) perfusion or injection. Imaging of dLight1.1 was then performed to measure DA levels.
SKF83822 microinfusion
Mice were anesthetized and then placed in a stereotactic frame. Cannulae were bilaterally built in the cerebellum (AP = −6.75 mm, ML = ±1.8 mm, DV = -1.2 mm) and secured using screws and C&B Metabond cement (Parkell). Mice were allowed to recovery for 7 d. Microinfusion of SKF83822 (5 mM, 1 µl) into the bilateral cerebellum were performed at a rate of 0.1 µl/min by using a microsyringe pump connected to an internal cannula. Control mice were subject to the same surgery procedure and injected with saline. Microinfusion was performed for 5 consecutive days, and behavioral tests were conducted 1 h after the microinfusion on the last day.
In vivo fiber photometry
After viral injection, a 2-mm fiber optic cannula (Neurophotometrics or RWD) was implanted at the same coordination as the one for viral injections and affixed to the skull surface using C&B Metabond. After at least 14 days of dLight1.1 expression, fiber photometry data was acquired using a commercial system (FP3002, Neurophotometrics) controlled by an open-source program (Bonsai). The system delivered 470-nm (emit) and 415-nm LED (control), each having a light intensity of around 40 mW, to excite the sensor and the emitted fluorescent signal was collected at 16 frames per second (FPS). During the recording, freely moving animals were put in an open cage (26 cm × 15 cm × 14 cm) for 5 min after acclimation. Fiber photometry data collected from the system were analyzed using published MATLAB script111.
Open field test
Drd1 cKO (Slc1a3-Cre+/-;Drd1tm2.1+/+, with tamoxifen), or control (Slc1a3-Cre+/-;Drd1tm2.1+/+, without tamoxifen) were placed in the center of 60 × 60 or 30 × 40 cm open arenas. Following 10-min habituation, mice were recorded for 10 or 30 min with IR-sensitive Gigabit Ethernet video camera (ace acA780-75gm, Basler). The arena floor was virtually divided into 9 zones. The recorded activity was analyzed for the total distance, the amount of time spent in the central zone over the total time, or the distance traveled in the central zone over the total distance. These analyses were performed by using video tracking software (Ethovision XT 16, Noldus).
Rotarod test
Drd1 cKO or control mice were trained for 3 consecutive days on a rotarod apparatus (ROTOR-ROD, San Diego Instruments). Mice were placed on the rotating rod for a maximum of 1 min and returned to their home cage between trials. Mice were given 4 trials per day with a 10-min inter-trial interval. On the following day, mice were given 3 test trials. The duration spent on the rod was recorded for each trial and the average time was calculated for latency to fall.
Grip strength test
For forelimb strength test, Drd1 cKO or control mice were placed on a grip strength apparatus (San Diego Instruments). Mice were lifted by holding their tail and gently pulled backwards until the grip was lost. Mice were given 5 test trials and the peak force of each measurement was recorded. The average value was calculated for analysis.
Ladder rung walking
To assess walking ability and leg placement, Drd1 cKO or control mice were tested on horizontal ladder rungs (Bio-FMA, Bioseb). Mice were allowed to move one end to the other end of the ladder rungs. Mouse movement and paw misplacements were detected by two sets of infrared sensors placed above and below the ladder. Mice were given 3 trials with a 10-min inter-trial interval. The average number of footslip errors was calculated for forelimbs and hindlimbs.
Catwalk
A Catwalk system (CatWalk XT, Noldus) was used to assess gait performance in Drd1 cKO or control mice. High-speed camera was mounted beneath the glass platform to record walking patterns. Mice were trained for 3 d to travel from one end of the corridor to the other end and were given 3 trials with a 10-min inter-trial interval on the test day. The recorded videos were analyzed for the average stride length of right forepaw (RF), right hindpaw (RH), left forepaw (LF), and left hindpaw (LH).
Three-chamber social test
Drd1 cKO or control mice were acclimated to being handled for 3 d. Mice were placed in the center chamber of the arena (60 × 30 × 20 cm) and allowed to freely explore the entire arena for 5 min. Mice were shepherded back to the center chamber and blocked there. A novel mouse was placed under one of the pencil cups located in two side chambers. The blocks were removed, and the test mouse was allowed to explore the chambers for 10 min. After this time, the test mouse was returned to the center chamber and blocked there again. A second novel mouse was put under the previously empty cup. The blocks were lifted, and the test mouse was allowed to explore the chambers for anther 10 min. The amount of time the test mouse spent sniffing either the empty cup or the cup containing sentinel mice was quantified (Ethovision XT 16, Noldus). The preference index was calculated as \(\frac{{{{{{\rm{time}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{investigating}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{the}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{cup}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{with}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{the}}}}}}\,1{{{{{\rm{st}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{novel}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{mouse}}}}}}-{{{{{\rm{time}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{investigating}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{the}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{empty}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{cup}}}}}}\,}{{{{{{\rm{time}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{investigating}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{the}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{empty}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{cup}}}}}}+{{{{{\rm{time}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{investigating}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{the}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{cup}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{with}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{the}}}}}}\,1{{{{{\rm{st}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{novel}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{mouse}}}}}}}\times 100\) for the sociability test and \(\frac{{{{{{\rm{time}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{investigating}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{the}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{cup}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{with}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{the}}}}}}\,2{{{{{\rm{nd}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{novel}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{mouse}}}}}}-{{{{{\rm{time}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{investigating}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{the}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{cup}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{with}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{the}}}}}}\,1{{{{{\rm{st}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{novel}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{mouse}}}}}}}{{{{{{\rm{time}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{investigating}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{the}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{cup}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{with}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{the}}}}}}\,1{{{{{\rm{st}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{novel}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{mouse}}}}}}+{{{{{\rm{time}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{investigating}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{the}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{cup}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{with}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{the}}}}}}\,2{{{{{\rm{nd}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{novel}}}}}}\,{{{{{\rm{mouse}}}}}}}\times 100\) for the social memory test.
Marble burying
Standard polycarbonate cages (40 × 30 × 22 cm) were filled with bedding to a depth of 5 cm. On top of the bedding, 5 rows of 4 marbles were placed and mice were given access to the marbles for 30 min. A marble was scored as buried if 2/3 of its surface area was covered. The total number of buried marbles was counted for each mouse.
Nestlet building
Mice were singe housed and a cotton fiber nestlet (5.5 × 5.5 × 0.3 cm, ~ 2 g) was added to the cage. Mice were allowed 16 h to shred the nestlet and build the nest. Nest building was blindly scored, based on a 5-point rating scale: 1, nestlet not notably touched; 2, nestlet slightly torn; 3, nestlet mostly shredded, but no visible nest; 4, identifiable but flat nest; 5, fine nest. The average score was calculated and compared between genotypes.
Delay and trace eyeblink conditioning
Stereotactic surgery was performed to implant a custom-built stainless headplate, as described previously112. Briefly, mice under isoflurane anesthesia were maintained on a stereotactic frame. An incision was made in the midline to expose the skull, and the overlying fascia was removed. The custom-built head plate was secured on the skull using screws and C&B Metabond cement (Parkell). Following the installation, the incision was closed with surgical glue and stitches. Mice were allowed to recover from surgical procedures for 7 d. Mice were acclimated to being head-restrained and running on a foam cylinder for 2 d. Mice were then given 1 training session per day. During the entire experiment, mice underwent a continuous training phase including 12 acquisition sessions, 3 extinction sessions, and 4 reacquisition sessions, and 2 wks later received 2 retention sessions. Each session consisted of 110 trials; the inter-trial interval was set to a random duration of 10−15 s. In all sessions during the acquisition, reacquisition, and retention, the CS paired with the US was delivered in each trial, except for every 10th trial in which only the CS was given. In those sessions during the extinction, the CS was presented without the US for all trials. For the delay eyeblink conditioning, each CS-US pairing consisted of a 350-ms CS of blue LED light (M470F1, Thorlabs), and a 30-ms US of air puff via a 23-gauge needle (6–8 psi), which was co-terminated with the CS. For the trace eyeblink conditioning, each CS-US pairing consisted of a 50-ms CS, a 270-ms stimulus-free interval, and a 30-ms US.
The training procedure was performed under infrared illumination by an LED light (IR56-56, CMVision), and eyelid position was captured by a high-speed (250 frames/s) monochrome camera equipped with a 25-mm lens (Mako G-040, Allied Vision). Subframes that contained only the eye and surrounding fur were acquired by image acquisition toolbox (MATLAB) interfaced with custom-written scripts. Off-line analysis of the individual frames was performed by custom-written MATLAB codes. Briefly, each frame was converted to binary by setting a threshold such that the value of each pixel of the eye is 0 and the value of each pixel of the fur is 1. The values of all the pixels were summed and the difference values were calculated by subtracting the average value of the baseline frames (100-ms duration, before CS-US pairing) from the value of individual frame throughout the trial. The ratio of the difference value to the average baseline value was further calculated and fraction eyelid closure (FEC) was obtained by normalizing these ratios into 0 (eye fully open) and 1 (eye fully closed). CRs were considered obtained if the FEC during the CS but prior to the US presentation is > 0.1. The percent CR in each trial was calculated by dividing the number of CRs by the total number of trials. CR amplitude was obtained by the peak FEC from every 10th trial, where only CS was given.
Statistics
All behavioral experiments were performed blinded to the genotypes and treatment groups. The implementation of computer-assisted behavioral assessments and MATLAB scripts to analyze data provides additional assurance for unbiased data acquisition and analyses. All data were analyzed using Prism (GraphPad) and MATLAB. Comparisons between two groups were analyzed by two-tailed unpaired or paired Student’s t test or non-parametric Mann–Whitney test. Comparisons among more than two groups were analyzed by one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s or Tukey’s multiple comparison test or non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis test with Dunn’s multiple comparison test. Two-way ANOVA repeated measures were used for analyses of the eyeblink conditioning in control and Drd1 cKO groups. Sample sizes including the total number of experiments and the number of animals used are provided in the main text, or within associated main or supplemental figure legends. All data are shown as the mean ± SEM. Statistical differences were considered as significant at *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, and ***p < 0.001.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Data availability
Source data are provided with this paper. All data are available upon request to the Source data are provided with this paper.
Code availability
The code for eyeblink conditioning video frame acquisition, processing, and analysis is available at GitHub113 (https://zenodo.org/record/7569717#.Y_4sM-zMJ25). All relevant code is available upon request to the corresponding authors.
References
Björklund, A. & Dunnett, S. B. Dopamine neuron systems in the brain: an update. Trends Neurosci. 30, 194–202 (2007).
Klein, M. O. et al. Dopamine: functions, signaling, and association with neurological diseases. Cell Mol. Neurobiol. 39, 31–59 (2019).
Cepeda, C., Murphy, K. P., Parent, M. & Levine, M. S. The role of dopamine in Huntington’s disease. Prog. Brain Res. 211, 235–254 (2014).
Grace, A. A. Dysregulation of the dopamine system in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia and depression. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 17, 524–532 (2016).
Wise, R. A. & Robble, M. A. Dopamine and addiction. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 71, 79–106 (2020).
Kosillo, P. & Bateup, H. S. Dopaminergic dysregulation in syndromic autism spectrum disorders: insights from genetic mouse models. Front. Neural Circuits 15, 700968 (2021).
Del Campo, N., Chamberlain, S. R., Sahakian, B. J. & Robbins, T. W. The roles of dopamine and noradrenaline in the pathophysiology and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 69, e145–e157 (2011).
Wu, T. & Hallett, M. The cerebellum in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 136, 696–709 (2013).
Phillips, J. R., Hewedi, D. H., Eissa, A. M. & Moustafa, A. A. The cerebellum and psychiatric disorders. Front. Public Health 3, 66 (2015).
Moreno-Rius, J. Opioid addiction and the cerebellum. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 107, 238–251 (2019).
Bruchhage, M. M. K., Bucci, M. P. & Becker, E. B. E. Cerebellar involvement in autism and ADHD. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 155, 61–72 (2018).
Caligiore, D. et al. Consensus paper: towards a systems-level view of cerebellar function: the interplay between cerebellum, basal ganglia, and cortex. Cerebellum 16, 203–229 (2017).
Carta, I., Chen, C. H., Schott, A. L., Dorizan, S. & Khodakhah, K. Cerebellar modulation of the reward circuitry and social behavior. Science 363, eaav0581 (2019).
Holloway, Z. R. et al. Cerebellar modulation of mesolimbic dopamine transmission is functionally asymmetrical. The Cerebellum 18, 922–931 (2019).
Low, A. Y. T. et al. Reverse-translational identification of a cerebellar satiation network. Nature 600, 269–273 (2021).
Flace, P. et al. The cerebellar dopaminergic system. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 15, 650614 (2021).
Panagopoulos, N. T., Papadopoulos, G. C. & Matsokis, N. A. Dopaminergic innervation and binding in the rat cerebellum. Neurosci. Lett. 130, 208–212 (1991).
Panagopoulos, N. T. & Matsokis, N. A. Pharmacologic characterization of [3H]dopamine and [3H]spiperone binding in mouse cerebellum. Gen. Pharmacol. 25, 131–137 (1994).
Schoeps, K. O. et al. Preparation of [1-11C]dopamine, [1-11C]p-tyramine and [1-11C]m-tyramine. Autoradiography and PET examination of [1-11C]dopamine in primates. Nucl. Med. Biol. 20, 669–678 (1993).
Dethy, S. et al. Cerebellar spongiform degeneration induced by acute lithium intoxication in the rat. Neurosci Lett 224, 25–28 (1997).
Glaser, P. E. et al. Cerebellar neurotransmission in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: does dopamine neurotransmission occur in the cerebellar vermis? J. Neurosci. Methods 151, 62–67 (2006).
Quansah, E., Ruiz-Rodado, V., Grootveld, M. & Zetterstrom, T. S. C. Methylphenidate alters monoaminergic and metabolic pathways in the cerebellum of adolescent rats. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 28, 513–528 (2018).
Kim, Y. S., Shin, J. H., Hall, F. S. & Linden, D. J. Dopamine signaling is required for depolarization-induced slow current in cerebellar Purkinje cells. J. Neurosci. 29, 8530–8538 (2009).
Cutando, L. et al. Cerebellar dopamine D2 receptors regulate social behaviors. Nat. Neurosci. 25, 900–911 (2022).
Cerminara, N. L., Lang, E. J., Sillitoe, R. V. & Apps, R. Redefining the cerebellar cortex as an assembly of non-uniform Purkinje cell microcircuits. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 79–93 (2015).
De Zeeuw, C. I. & Hoogland, T. M. Reappraisal of Bergmann glial cells as modulators of cerebellar circuit function. Front. Cell Neurosci. 9, 246 (2015).
Poblete-Naredo, I. et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor and its receptors in Bergmann glia cells. Neurochem. Int. 59, 1133–1144 (2011).
Saab, A. S. et al. Bergmann glial AMPA receptors are required for fine motor coordination. Science 337, 749–753 (2012).
Balakrishnan, S. & Bellamy, T. C. Depression of parallel and climbing fiber transmission to Bergmann glia is input specific and correlates with increased precision of synaptic transmission. Glia 57, 393–401 (2009).
Chrobak, A. A. & Soltys, Z. Bergmann glia, long-term depression, and autism spectrum disorder. Mol. Neurobiol. 54, 1156–1166 (2017).
Dooves, S. et al. Bergmann glia translocation: a new disease marker for vanishing white matter identifies therapeutic effects of Guanabenz treatment. Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol. 44, 391–403 (2018).
Shiwaku, H., Yagishita, S., Eishi, Y. & Okazawa, H. Bergmann glia are reduced in spinocerebellar ataxia type 1. Neuroreport 24, 620–625 (2013).
Ade, K. K., Wan, Y., Chen, M., Gloss, B. & Calakos, N. An improved BAC transgenic fluorescent reporter line for sensitive and specific identification of striatonigral medium spiny neurons. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 5, 32 (2011).
Gong, S. et al. A gene expression atlas of the central nervous system based on bacterial artificial chromosomes. Nature 425, 917–925 (2003).
Daubner, S. C., Le, T. & Wang, S. Tyrosine hydroxylase and regulation of dopamine synthesis. Arch. Biochem. Biophys. 508, 1–12 (2011).
Ranjbar-Slamloo, Y. & Fazlali, Z. Dopamine and noradrenaline in the brain; overlapping or dissociate functions? Front. Mol. Neurosci. 12, 334 (2019).
Hiroi, T., Imaoka, S. & Funae, Y. Dopamine formation from tyramine by CYP2D6. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 249, 838–843 (1998).
Bromek, E., Haduch, A. & Daniel, W. A. The ability of cytochrome P450 2D isoforms to synthesize dopamine in the brain: an in vitro study. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 626, 171–178 (2010).
Wang, X., Li, J., Dong, G. & Yue, J. The endogenous substrates of brain CYP2D. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 724, 211–218 (2014).
Thompson, C. M., Capdevila, J. H. & Strobel, H. W. Recombinant cytochrome P450 2D18 metabolism of dopamine and arachidonic acid. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 294, 1120–1130 (2000).
Kuban, W. & Daniel, W. A. Cytochrome P450 expression and regulation in the brain. Drug Metab. Rev. 53, 1–29 (2021).
Patriarchi, T. et al. Ultrafast neuronal imaging of dopamine dynamics with designed genetically encoded sensors. Science 360, eaat4422 (2018).
Daniel, W. A., Haduch, A. & Wojcikowski, J. Inhibition and possible induction of rat CYP2D after short- and long-term treatment with antidepressants. J. Pharm. Pharmacol. 54, 1545–1552 (2002).
Preskorn, S. H. Clinically relevant pharmacology of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. An overview with emphasis on pharmacokinetics and effects on oxidative drug metabolism. Clin. Pharmacokinet 32, 1–21 (1997).
Yu, A. M. & Haining, R. L. Expression, purification, and characterization of mouse CYP2d22. Drug Metab. Dispos. 34, 1167–1174 (2006).
Giri, P. et al. Consequences of daily corticosteroid dosing with or without pre-treatment with quinidine on the in vivo cytochrome P450 2D (CYP2D) enzyme in rats: effect on O-demethylation activity of dextromethorphan and expression levels of CYP2D1 mRNA. Xenobiotica 48, 1–10 (2018).
German, C. L., Baladi, M. G., McFadden, L. M., Hanson, G. R. & Fleckenstein, A. E. Regulation of the dopamine and vesicular monoamine transporters: pharmacological targets and implications for disease. Pharmacol. Rev. 67, 1005–1024 (2015).
Surmeier, D. J. et al. The role of dopamine in modulating the structure and function of striatal circuits. Prog Brain Res. 183, 149–167 (2010).
Yu, X., Duan, K. L., Shang, C. F., Yu, H. G. & Zhou, Z. Calcium influx through hyperpolarization-activated cation channels (I(h) channels) contributes to activity-evoked neuronal secretion. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101, 1051–1056 (2004).
Guatteo, E., Stanness, K. A. & Janigro, D. Hyperpolarization-activated ion currents in cultured rat cortical and spinal cord astrocytes. Glia 16, 196–209 (1996).
Atasoy, D. & Sternson, S. M. Chemogenetic tools for causal cellular and neuronal biology. Physiol. Rev. 98, 391–418 (2018).
Sun, X., Zhao, Y. & Wolf, M. E. Dopamine receptor stimulation modulates AMPA receptor synaptic insertion in prefrontal cortex neurons. J. Neurosci. 25, 7342–7351 (2005).
Sun, X., Milovanovic, M., Zhao, Y. & Wolf, M. E. Acute and chronic dopamine receptor stimulation modulates AMPA receptor trafficking in nucleus accumbens neurons cocultured with prefrontal cortex neurons. J. Neurosci. 28, 4216–4230 (2008).
Song, R. S. et al. ERK regulation of phosphodiesterase 4 enhances dopamine-stimulated AMPA receptor membrane insertion. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 15437–15442 (2013).
Iino, M. et al. Glia-synapse interaction through Ca2+-permeable AMPA receptors in Bergmann glia. Science 292, 926–929 (2001).
Douyard, J., Shen, L., Huganir, R. L. & Rubio, M. E. Differential neuronal and glial expression of GluR1 AMPA receptor subunit and the scaffolding proteins SAP97 and 4.1N during rat cerebellar development. J. Comp. Neurol. 502, 141–156 (2007).
Hollmann, M., Hartley, M. & Heinemann, S. Ca2+ permeability of KA-AMPA–gated glutamate receptor channels depends on subunit composition. Science 252, 851–853 (1991).
Mangiavacchi, S. & Wolf, M. E. D1 dopamine receptor stimulation increases the rate of AMPA receptor insertion onto the surface of cultured nucleus accumbens neurons through a pathway dependent on protein kinase A. J. Neurochem. 88, 1261–1271 (2004).
Carvalho, A. L., Duarte, C. B. & Carvalho, A. P. Regulation of AMPA receptors by phosphorylation. Neurochem. Res. 25, 1245–1255 (2000).
Zhang, R. S., Liakath-Ali, K. & Südhof, T. C. Latrophilin-2 and latrophilin-3 are redundantly essential for parallel-fiber synapse function in cerebellum. Elife 9, e54443 (2020).
Boele, H. J., Koekkoek, S. K., De & Zeeuw, C. I. Cerebellar and extracerebellar involvement in mouse eyeblink conditioning: the ACDC model. Front. Cell Neurosci. 3, 19 (2010).
Kim, S. & Thompson, R. F. c-Fos, Arc, and stargazin expression in rat eyeblink conditioning. Behav. Neurosci. 125, 117–123 (2011).
Minatohara, K., Akiyoshi, M. & Okuno, H. Role of immediate-early genes in synaptic plasticity and neuronal ensembles underlying the memory trace. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 8, 78 (2015).
Moreno-Rius, J. Is there an “antisocial” cerebellum? Evidence from disorders other than autism characterized by abnormal social behaviours. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 89, 1–8 (2019).
Sato, A. mTOR, a potential target to treat autism spectrum disorder. CNS Neurol. Disord. Drug Targets 15, 533–543 (2016).
Ricci, A., Zaccheo, D. & Amenta, F. Identification of age-related changes of dopamine D1-like receptors in the rat cerebellar cortex. Synapse 21, 37–44 (1995).
Ricci, A., Mammola, C. L., Vega, J. A., Zaccheo, D. & Amenta, F. Density and pattern of dopamine D2-like receptors in the cerebellar cortex of aged rats. Neurobiol. Aging 17, 45–52 (1996).
Delis, F., Mitsacos, A. & Giompres, P. Pharmacological characterization and anatomical distribution of the dopamine transporter in the mouse cerebellum. Cerebellum 7, 242–251 (2008).
Zanassi, P., Paolillo, M., Montecucco, A., Avvedimento, E. V. & Schinelli, S. Pharmacological and molecular evidence for dopamine D(1) receptor expression by striatal astrocytes in culture. J. Neurosci. Res. 58, 544–552 (1999).
Shao, W. et al. Suppression of neuroinflammation by astrocytic dopamine D2 receptors via αB-crystallin. Nature 494, 90–94 (2013).
Montoya, A. et al. Dopamine receptor D3 signalling in astrocytes promotes neuroinflammation. J. Neuroinflammation 16, 258 (2019).
Huang, C., Wu, J., Liao, R. & Zhang, W. SKF83959, an agonist of phosphatidylinositol-linked D(1)-like receptors, promotes ERK1/2 activation and cell migration in cultured rat astrocytes. PLoS ONE 7, e49954 (2012).
Requardt, R. P. et al. Ca2+ signals of astrocytes are modulated by the NAD+/NADH redox state. J. Neurochem. 120, 1014–1025 (2012).
Liu, J. et al. Activation of phosphatidylinositol-linked novel D1 dopamine receptor contributes to the calcium mobilization in cultured rat prefrontal cortical astrocytes. Cell Mol. Neurobiol. 29, 317–328 (2009).
Mann, A. et al. The neuroprotective enzyme CYP2D6 increases in the brain with age and is lower in Parkinson’s disease patients. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 2160–2171 (2012).
Ur Rasheed, M. S., Mishra, A. K. & Singh, M. P. Cytochrome P450 2D6 and Parkinson’s disease: polymorphism, metabolic role, risk and protection. Neurochem. Res. 42, 3353–3361 (2017).
Peñas-LLedó, E. M. & LLerena, A. CYP2D6 variation, behaviour and psychopathology: implications for pharmacogenomics-guided clinical trials. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 77, 673–683 (2014).
Fujii, T., Sakai, M. & Nagatsu, I. Immunohistochemical demonstration of expression of tyrosine hydroxylase in cerebellar Purkinje cells of the human and mouse. Neurosci. Lett. 165, 161–163 (1994).
Ikai, Y., Takada, M., Shinonaga, Y. & Mizuno, N. Dopaminergic and non-dopaminergic neurons in the ventral tegmental area of the rat project, respectively, to the cerebellar cortex and deep cerebellar nuclei. Neuroscience 51, 719–728 (1992).
Melchitzky, D. S. & Lewis, D. A. Tyrosine hydroxylase- and dopamine transporter-immunoreactive axons in the primate cerebellum. Neuropsychopharmacology 22, 466–472 (2000).
Nelson, T. E., King, J. S. & Bishop, G. A. Distribution of tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoreactive afferents to the cerebellum differs between species. J. Comp. Neurol. 379, 443–454 (1997).
Tang, X., Taniguchi, K. & Kofuji, P. Heterogeneity of Kir4.1 channel expression in glia revealed by mouse transgenesis. Glia 57, 1706–1715 (2009).
Podda, M. V., Riccardi, E., D’Ascenzo, M., Azzena, G. B. & Grassi, C. Dopamine D1-like receptor activation depolarizes medium spiny neurons of the mouse nucleus accumbens by inhibiting inwardly rectifying K+ currents through a cAMP-dependent protein kinase A-independent mechanism. Neuroscience 167, 678–690 (2010).
Undieh, A. S. Pharmacology of signaling induced by dopamine D(1)-like receptor activation. Pharmacol. Ther. 128, 37–60 (2010).
Fieblinger, T. et al. Mechanisms of dopamine D1 receptor-mediated ERK1/2 activation in the parkinsonian striatum and their modulation by metabotropic glutamate receptor type 5. J. Neurosci. 34, 4728–4740 (2014).
Zhang, X. et al. Activation of phosphatidylinositol-linked D1-like receptor modulates FGF-2 expression in astrocytes via IP3-dependent Ca2+ signaling. J. Neurosci. 29, 7766–7775 (2009).
Lee, S. P. et al. Dopamine D1 and D2 receptor Co-activation generates a novel phospholipase C-mediated calcium signal. J Biol Chem 279, 35671–35678 (2004).
Liu, J. et al. Phosphatidylinositol-linked novel D(1) dopamine receptor facilitates long-term depression in rat hippocampal CA1 synapses. Neuropharmacology 57, 164–171 (2009).
Mizuno, K., Kurokawa, K. & Ohkuma, S. Dopamine D1 receptors regulate type 1 inositol 1,4,5-trisphosphate receptor expression via both AP-1- and NFATc4-mediated transcriptional processes. J. Neurochem. 122, 702–713 (2012).
Yu, Y. et al. Neuroprotective effects of atypical D1 receptor agonist SKF83959 are mediated via D1 receptor-dependent inhibition of glycogen synthase kinase-3 beta and a receptor-independent anti-oxidative action. J. Neurochem. 104, 946–956 (2008).
Medvedev, I. O. et al. D1 dopamine receptor coupling to PLCβ regulates forward locomotion in mice. J. Neurosci. 33, 18125–18133 (2013).
Bordey, A. & Sontheimer, H. Modulation of glutamatergic transmission by bergmann glial cells in rat cerebellum in situ. J. Neurophysiol. 89, 979–988 (2003).
Wang, F., Xu, Q., Wang, W., Takano, T. & Nedergaard, M. Bergmann glia modulate cerebellar Purkinje cell bistability via Ca2+-dependent K+ uptake. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 109, 7911–7916 (2012).
Lonchamp, E. et al. Pre and post synaptic NMDA effects targeting purkinje cells in the mouse cerebellar cortex. PLoS ONE 7, e30180 (2012).
Delaney, A. J. & Jahr, C. E. Kainate receptors differentially regulate release at two parallel fiber synapses. Neuron 36, 475–482 (2002).
Crupi, R., Impellizzeri, D. & Cuzzocrea, S. Role of Metabotropic Glutamate Receptors in Neurological Disorders. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 12, 20 (2019).
Lorez, M., Humbel, U., Pflimlin, M.-C. & Kew, J. N. C. Group III metabotropic glutamate receptors as autoreceptors in the cerebellar cortex. Br. J. Pharmacol. 138, 614–625 (2003).
Hull, C. Prediction signals in the cerebellum: beyond supervised motor learning. Elife 9, e54073 (2020).
Ito, H. T. & Schuman, E. M. Frequency-dependent signal transmission and modulation by neuromodulators. Front. Neurosci. 2, 138–144 (2008).
Rosen, Z. B., Cheung, S. & Siegelbaum, S. A. Midbrain dopamine neurons bidirectionally regulate CA3-CA1 synaptic drive. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1763–1771 (2015).
Nimmerjahn, A., Mukamel, E. A. & Schnitzer, M. J. Motor behavior activates Bergmann glial networks. Neuron 62, 400–412 (2009).
Ozden, I., Dombeck, D. A., Hoogland, T. M., Tank, D. W. & Wang, S. S. Widespread state-dependent shifts in cerebellar activity in locomoting mice. PLoS ONE 7, e42650 (2012).
Thompson, R. F. An essential memory trace found. Behav. Neurosci. 127, 669–675 (2013).
Attwell, P. J. E., Cooke, S. F. & Yeo, C. H. Cerebellar function in consolidation of a motor memory. Neuron 34, 1011–1020 (2002).
Backman, C. M. et al. Characterization of a mouse strain expressing Cre recombinase from the 3’ untranslated region of the dopamine transporter locus. Genesis 44, 383–390 (2006).
Barski, J. J., Dethleffsen, K. & Meyer, M. Cre recombinase expression in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Genesis 28, 93–98 (2000).
Wang, Y. et al. Norrin/Frizzled4 signaling in retinal vascular development and blood brain barrier plasticity. Cell 151, 1332–1344 (2012).
Sarinana, J., Kitamura, T., Kunzler, P., Sultzman, L. & Tonegawa, S. Differential roles of the dopamine 1-class receptors, D1R and D5R, in hippocampal dependent memory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, 8245–8250 (2014).
Inoue, T. TI Workbench, an integrated software package for electrophysiology and imaging. Microscopy (Oxf) 67, 129–143 (2018).
Wang, Y. et al. Accurate quantification of astrocyte and neurotransmitter fluorescence dynamics for single-cell and population-level physiology. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 1936–1944 (2019).
Lopez-Rojas, J., de Solis, C. A., Leroy, F., Kandel, E. R. & Siegelbaum, S. A. A direct lateral entorhinal cortex to hippocampal CA2 circuit conveys social information required for social memory. Neuron 110, 1559–1572.e1554 (2022).
Heiney, S. A., Wohl, M. P., Chettih, S. N., Ruffolo, L. I. & Medina, J. F. Cerebellar-dependent expression of motor learning during eyeblink conditioning in head-fixed mice. J. Neurosci. 34, 14845–14853 (2014).
Li, C. Purkinje cell dopaminergic inputs to astrocytes regulate cerebellar-dependent behavior. cl1551/Eyeblink-Conditioning: v1.0 (v1.0). Zenodo https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7569717 (2023).
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to Dr. Lucas Pozzo-Miller for the discussions and comments on the manuscript. We thank Yijian Zhang for maintaining mouse colony and writing MATLAB scripts for eyeblink conditioning. We thank Dr. Xin Xu and Cesar Acevedo for the discussions and Alejandro Osorio (University of Lausanne, Switzerland) for writing the initial MATLAB scripts. Funding was provided by grants from the United States National Institutes of Health (R21NS097913, R21NS108508, R21NS120315, and R01NS121542 to W.L.).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
W.L. designed the research. C.L., N.B.S., H.M., N.A.L., K.K., R.S., D.M., and W.L. conducted the research, analyzed the results. C.L., N.B.S., H.M., N.A.L., D.M., and W.L. wrote the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Communications thanks the anonymous reviewers for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Source data
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Li, C., Saliba, N.B., Martin, H. et al. Purkinje cell dopaminergic inputs to astrocytes regulate cerebellar-dependent behavior. Nat Commun 14, 1613 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-023-37319-w
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-023-37319-w
This article is cited by
-
Cerebellar damage with inflammation upregulates oxytocin receptor expression in Bergmann Glia
Molecular Brain (2024)
-
Mitochondrial heterogeneity and adaptations to cellular needs
Nature Cell Biology (2024)
-
Astrocytic Neuroligin-3 influences gene expression and social behavior, but is dispensable for synapse number
Molecular Psychiatry (2024)
-
Consensus Paper: Cerebellum and Reward
The Cerebellum (2024)
-
Dopamine receptors of the rodent fastigial nucleus support skilled reaching for goal-directed action
Brain Structure and Function (2023)
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.