Abstract
General cognitive function is a prominent and relatively stable human trait that is associated with many important life outcomes. We combine cognitive and genetic data from the CHARGE and COGENT consortia, and UK Biobank (total N = 300,486; age 16–102) and find 148 genome-wide significant independent loci (P < 5 × 10−8) associated with general cognitive function. Within the novel genetic loci are variants associated with neurodegenerative and neurodevelopmental disorders, physical and psychiatric illnesses, and brain structure. Gene-based analyses find 709 genes associated with general cognitive function. Expression levels across the cortex are associated with general cognitive function. Using polygenic scores, up to 4.3% of variance in general cognitive function is predicted in independent samples. We detect significant genetic overlap between general cognitive function, reaction time, and many health variables including eyesight, hypertension, and longevity. In conclusion we identify novel genetic loci and pathways contributing to the heritability of general cognitive function.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Some individuals have generally higher cognitive function than others. These individual differences are quite persistent across the life course from later childhood onwards. Individuals with higher measured general cognitive function tend to live longer and be less deprived. Retaining general cognitive function is an important aspect of healthy ageing. The population variance in this medically- and socially-important trait has environmental and genetic aetiologies. The details of the genetic contributions are, as-yet, poorly understood.
Since the discovery of general cognitive ability (or ‘g’) in 19041, hundreds of studies have replicated the finding that around 40% of the variance in subjects’ scores on a diverse battery of cognitive tests can be accounted for by a single general factor2. Some variance is also attributable to individual cognitive domains (e.g., reasoning, memory, processing speed, and spatial ability), and some is attributable to specific cognitive skills associated with individual mental tests. However, all cognitive tests rely to a greater or lesser extent on general cognitive ability for successful execution. Figure 1 illustrates and explains this hierarchical model of cognitive ability differences3. Therefore, using a general cognitive function phenotype in a genetically-informative design is supported by the observation that the well-established positive manifold of cognitive tests may be represented by a substantially heritable, higher-order, latent general cognitive function phenotype2,4,5.
There are two commonly-used routes that are used to obtain general cognitive ability scores for each participant in a sample. First, if all members of a sample have taken the same set of diverse cognitive tests, then a data reduction procedure (such as principal components analysis (PCA) or factor analysis) can be applied. Typically, this finds that all tests load on (i.e., correlate positively with) the first unrotated component, or factor, and scores on this component can be calculated for each person; this gives each person a g score. Second, some mental tests—usually those involving complex mental work, and often those with a variety of item types—have a high g loading2. That is, scores on some individual cognitive tests can be used to obtain an acceptable proxy for general cognitive ability. An example of the latter is the Moray House Test of verbal and numerical reasoning, which has a high correlation with a PCA-derived general cognitive function score6.
General cognitive function is peerless among human psychological traits in terms of its empirical support and importance for life outcomes7,8. Individuals who have higher cognitive function in childhood and adolescence tend to stay longer in education, gain higher educational qualifications, progress to more professional and better-paid jobs, live healthier lives, and live longer. Individual differences in general cognitive function show phenotypic and genetic stability across most of the life course9,10,11. The phenotypic correlation between general cognitive function scores on the same people at age 11 and age 70–80 years is almost 0.7, and remains above 0.5 when age 11 versus age 90 scores are correlated.
Twin studies find that general cognitive function has a heritability of more than 50% from adolescence through adulthood to older age4,5,12. SNP-based estimates of heritability for general cognitive function are about 20–30%13. However, these estimates might increase to about 50% when family-based designs are used to retain the contributions made by rarer SNPs14. To date, little of this substantial heritability has been explained, i.e., only a few relevant genetic loci have been discovered (Table 1; Supplementary Fig. 1). As has been found with other highly polygenic traits, a limitation on uncovering relevant genetic loci is sample size15; to date, there have been fewer than 100,000 individuals in studies of general cognitive function13,16. The MTAG (multi-trait analysis of genome-wide association studies) method has been used to corral cognitive function and associated traits to expand the number of loci associated with general cognitive function17. However, the present study uses only cognitive function phenotypes, and amasses a total sample size of over 300,000.
The present study also tests for genetic contributions to reaction time, and examines its genetic relationship with general cognitive function. Reaction time is both phenotypically and genetically correlated with general cognitive function, and accounts for some of its association with health18,19,20. By making these comparisons between general cognitive function and reaction time, we identify regions of the genome that have a shared correlation with general cognitive function and more elementary cognitive tasks21.
Results
General cognitive function phenotypes
The psychometric characteristics of the general cognitive component from each cohort in the CHARGE consortium are shown in Supplementary Note 1. In order to address the fact that different cohorts had applied different cognitive tests, we previously showed that two general cognitive function components extracted from different sets of cognitive tests on the same participants correlate highly13. The cognitive test from the large UK Biobank sample was the so-called ‘fluid’ test, a 13-item test of verbal-numerical reasoning, which has a high genetic correlation with general cognitive function22. With the CHARGE and COGENT samples’ general cognitive function scores and UK Biobank’s verbal-numerical reasoning scores, there were 300,486 participants included in the present report’s meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies (GWASs). Note that we included four UK Biobank samples, i.e. three assessment centre-tested samples, and one online-tested sample. The genetic correlation between CHARGE’s-COGENT’s general cognitive function component and UK Biobank’s verbal-numerical reasoning test, calculated for the present study using linkage disequilibrium score (LDSC) regression, was estimated at 0.87 (SE = 0.03). This indicates very substantial overlap between the genetic variants associated with cognitive function in these two groups.
SNP-based meta-analyses of cognitive function GWASs
We performed an N-weighted meta-analysis of general cognitive function which included all of the CHARGE, COGENT, and UK Biobank samples. Meta-analysis of the results for the general cognitive function GWASs found 11,600 significant (P < 5 × 10−8) SNP associations, and 21,855 at a suggestive level (1 × 10−5 > P ≥ 5 × 10−8); see Fig. 2a, Supplementary Fig. 2a, and Supplementary Data 1 and 2. There were 434 ‘independent’ significant SNPs; see Methods section for description of independent SNP selection criteria, distributed within 148 loci across all autosomal chromosomes. Note that, for consistency, we use the term ‘independent’ here according to the definition that is used in the relevant analysis package. A comparison of these 148 loci with results from the largest previous GWASs of cognitive function16, and educational attainment24, and an MTAG analysis of cognitive function17—all of which included a subsample of individuals contributing to the present study—confirmed that 11 of 18, 24 of 74, and 89 of 187 of these were, respectively, genome-wide significant in the present study (Supplementary Data 3). Of the 148 loci found in the present study, 58 have not been reported previously in other GWA studies of cognitive function or educational attainment (novel loci are indicated in Supplementary Data 4). One hundred and seventy-eight lead SNPs were identified within these 148 loci.
For the 434 independent significant SNPs and tagged SNPs, a summary of previous SNP associations is listed in Supplementary Data 5. They have been associated with many physical (e.g., BMI, height, weight), medical (e.g., lung cancer, Crohn’s disease, blood pressure), and psychiatric (e.g., bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, autism) traits. Of the 58 new loci, we highlight previous associations with schizophrenia (2 loci), Alzheimer’s disease (1 locus), and Parkinson’s disease (1 locus).
We sought to identify independent significant and tagged SNPs within the 148 significant genomic risk loci associated with general cognitive function that are potentially functional (Fig. 3a; Supplementary Data 4). See Methods section for further details. Across many of the loci there is clear evidence of functionality including involvement in gene regulation, deleterious SNPs, eQTLs, and regions of open chromatin.
General cognitive function gene-based and gene-set results
A gene-based association analysis identified 709 genes as significantly associated with general cognitive function (Fig. 2b; Supplementary Fig. 2b; Supplementary Data 6). These 709 genes were compared to gene-based associations from previous studies of general cognitive function and educational attainment13,16,17,25; 418 were replicated in the present study, and 291 were novel. The 291 new gene-based associations are highlighted in Supplementary Data 6. Several of the specific genes associated with general cognitive function are considered in detail in the Discussion, below.
Gene-set analysis identified seven significant gene sets associated with general cognitive function: neurogenesis (P = 1.57 × 10−9), regulation of nervous system development (P = 7.52 × 10−7), neuron projection (P = 7.89 × 10−7), positive regulation of nervous system development (P = 9.42 × 10−7), neuron differentiation (P = 1.68 × 10−6), regulation of cell development (P = 1.93 × 10−6), and dendrite (P = 3.52 × 10−6) (Supplementary Data 7). Gene-property analysis can show if tissue-specific expression levels are associated with a gene’s association with a phenotype. This analysis indicated a significant association between transcription levels in all brain regions—except the brain spinal cord and cervical c1—and the association with general cognitive function. In addition, expression levels in the pituitary were associated with gene-based association with general cognitive function; these results indicate that the genes with the highest expression levels in these regions were those showing the greatest associations with general cognitive function. (Fig. 3b, c; Supplementary Table 1; Supplementary Data 8). The significance of this relationship was greatest in the cerebellum and the cortex.
SNP-based heritability of general cognitive function
We estimated the proportion of variance explained by all common SNPs using GCTA-GREML in four of the largest individual samples: English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA: N = 6661, h2 = 0.12, SE = 0.06), Understanding Society (N = 7841, h2 = 0.17, SE = 0.04), UK Biobank Assessment Centre (N = 86,010, h2 = 0.25, SE = 0.006), and Generation Scotland (N = 6,507, h2 = 0.20, SE = 0.0523) (Table 2). Genetic correlations for general cognitive function amongst these cohorts, estimated using bivariate GCTA-GREML, ranged from rg = 0.88 to 1.0 (Table 2). These results indicate that the same genetic variants contribute to phenotypic differences in general cognitive function across each of these three samples. We investigated the genetic contribution to the stability of individual differences in people’s verbal-numerical reasoning, by examining data from those individuals in UK Biobank who completed the test on two occasions (mean time gap = 4.93 years). We found a significant and perfect genetic correlation of rg = 1.0 (SE = 0.02).
Polygenic profile scores and genetic correlations
After omitting them from the meta-analysis of GWASs, we created general cognitive function polygenic profile scores in three of the larger cohorts: ELSA, Generation Scotland, and Understanding Society. The polygenic profile score for general cognitive function explained 2.63% of the variance in ELSA (β = 0.17, SE = 0.01, P = 1.70 × 10−51), 3.73% in Generation Scotland (β = 0.20, SE = 0.01, P = 5.02 × 10−68), and 4.31% in Understanding Society (β = 0.22, SE = 0.01, P = 6.17 × 10−88). Full results for all five thresholds are shown in Supplementary Table 2.
We tested the genetic correlations between general cognitive function and 52 health-related traits. Thirty-six of these health-related traits were significantly genetically correlated with general cognitive function (Supplementary Data 9). We report significant genetic correlations between general cognitive function and: hypertension (rg = −0.15, SE = 0.02), grip strength (right hand: rg = 0.09, SE = 0.02), wearing glasses or contact lenses (rg = 0.28, SE = 0.04), short-sightedness (rg = 0.32, SE = 0.03), long-sightedness (rg = −0.21, SE= 0.05), heart attack (rg = −0.17, SE = 0.03), angina (rg = −0.18, SE = 0.03), lung cancer (rg = −0.26, SE = 0.05), and osteoarthritis (rg = −0.24, SE = 0.04). We also report a significant genetic correlation with major depressive disorder (rg = −0.30, SE = 0.04); this result strengthens previously-reported non-significant correlations of around −0.1016,17. We also note the important genetic association between general cognitive function and longevity (rg = 0.17, SE = 0.06).
Reaction time results
GWAS results for mean reaction time uncovered 2022 significant SNPs in 42 independent genomic loci (Fig. 4a; Supplementary Fig. 2c; Supplementary Data 10). Suggestive findings are presented in Supplementary Data 11. Both of the significant loci previously reported for this phenotype were replicated13. SNPs within the 42 independent genomic loci showed clear evidence of functionality (Fig. 5a; Supplementary Data 12). Using gene-based GWA, a total of 191 genes attained statistical significance (Fig. 4b; Supplementary Fig. 2d; Supplementary Data 13), replicating 18 of the 23 genome-wide significant genes found previously for this phenotype13. Gene-set analysis identified no gene sets associated with reaction time (Supplementary Data 14). Gene-property analysis indicated a role for genes expressed in the brain (P = 4.66 × 10−13), with this link between gene transcription levels and gene-based association with reaction time being found across the cortex (Fig. 5b, c; Supplementary Table 3; Supplementary Data 15). Gene transcription levels observed in the pituitary gland were also linked to gene-based associations with differences in reaction time (P = 7.60 × 10−4).
The SNP-based heritability of reaction time was 7.42% (SE = 0.29). It should be noted that this estimate is likely to be an underestimation due to the method used (LD score regression)26. Significant overlap was found between the genetic architecture of reaction time and these health outcomes: ADHD, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, subjective wellbeing, hand grip strength, sleep duration, maternal longevity, hypertension and neuroticism (Supplementary Data 9). The polygenic score for reaction time explained 0.43% of the general cognitive function variance in ELSA (P = 1.42 × 10−9), 0.56% in Generation Scotland (P = 2.49 × 10−11), and 0.26% in Understanding Society (P = 1.50 × 10−6). The full results for all five thresholds can be found in Supplementary Table 2.
We found a genetic correlation (rg) of 0.247 (P = 1.28 × 10−30) between reaction time and general cognitive function. Overlapping results between the two phenotypes were explored further.
Of the 11,600 genome-wide significant SNPs for general cognitive function, 8269 had a consistent direction of effect with reaction time (sign test, P = 2.2 × 10−16) (Supplementary Data 1). For reaction time, 1070 of the 2022 significant SNPs were consistent for direction of effect with general cognitive function (sign test, P = 0.0071) (Supplementary Data 10). One hundred and sixty SNPs were genome-wide significant for both general cognitive function and reaction time, with 82 consistent for direction of effect (sign test, NS) (Supplementary Data 16). These overlapping genome-wide findings are located within six genomic loci (genomic loci: 13, 15, 19, 28, 69, 133; see Supplementary Data 4 for details of loci); two of these are novel loci for general cognitive function. In the gene-based analyses of both the general cognitive function and reaction time phenotypes, there were 39 overlapping significant genes; 13 of these are newly-identified associations with general cognitive function (Supplementary Data 17).
Discussion
In these meta-analyses of genome-wide association studies for both general cognitive function and reaction time (N = 300,486; N = 330,069, respectively), we make several original contributions. We report 148 genome-wide significant loci for general cognitive function, of which 58 loci have not been reported before. We report 42 genome-wide significant loci for reaction time, of which 40 have not been reported previously. We also report 291 gene-based associations for general cognitive function, and 173 for reaction time, which have not been reported already. Of these genome-wide significant results, six loci and 39 gene-based associations are genome-wide significant for both general cognitive function and reaction time. We are able to predict, using polygenic scoring, up to 4.31 and 0.56% of the general cognitive function variance in an independent sample, for general cognitive function and reaction time polygenic scores, respectively. We present original and updated estimates of genetic correlations with many health traits for both general cognitive function and reaction time. Gene-set analyses identified significant associations for general cognitive function with gene-sets involved in neural and cell development. Significant enrichments were observed with genes expressed in the cerebellum and the brain’s cortex for both general cognitive function and reaction time.
Upon additional exploration of the 58 newly-associated genetic loci, we find that many contain genes that are of further interest. All of the genes discussed below are also genome-wide significant in the general cognitive function gene-based association analysis (P < 2.75 × 10−6; Supplementary Data 6). Significant gene-based associations with general cognitive function have also been previously reported for GATAD2B, SLC39A1, and AUTS216,17.
GATAD2B and SLC39A1 are located on chromosome 1; locus 11. Mutations in GATAD2B have been linked to intellectual disability27. SLC39A1 has been implicated in Alzheimer’s Disease28. The ATXN1 gene (chromosome 6; locus 60), encodes a protein containing a polyglutamine tract that has previously been associated with Spinocerebellar Ataxia 129. ATXN1L, ATXN2L, and ATXN7L2 were also located in significant loci that have previously been associated with cognitive function, intelligence, or educational attainment16,17,24. The DCDC2 gene (chromosome 6; locus 64) has previously been associated with cortical morphology30, dyslexia31, and normal variation in reading and spelling32, but not with general cognitive function. TTBK1 (chromosome 6; locus 66) encodes a neuron-specific serine/threonine and tyrosine kinase, which regulates phosphorylation of tau33. Genetic variants in this gene have been associated with Alzheimer's disease34. AUTS2 (chromosome 7; locus 72) is implicated in a number of neurological disorders35. Mutations in CWF19L1 (chromosome 10; locus 91) have been associated with spinocerebellar ataxia and intellectual disability36. RBFOX1 (chromosome 16; locus 121) encodes a mRNA-splicing factor that interacts with ATXN237, and mutations in this gene lead to neurodevelopmental disorders38. Locus 131, on chromosome 17, has previously been associated with Smith-Magenis Syndrome39. The most significantly-associated SNP (P = 2.2 × 10−8) in this locus lies in an intron of the RAI1 gene. RAI1 encodes a protein containing a polymorphic polyglutamine tract that is expressed mainly in neuronal tissues. Variants in the gene are also associated with schizophrenia40.
Of the seven significant gene sets identified, one was a new finding: ‘positive regulation of nervous system development’. A more detailed description of this gene-set is: ‘any process that activates, maintains or increases the frequency, rate or extent of nervous system development, the origin and formation of nervous tissue’. The remaining six gene-sets showed replication with previous studies of general cognitive function and/or education16,17,24. Only one, ‘regulation of cell development’, was significant across all four studies16,17,24. Identification of these gene sets is consistent with genes associated with cognitive function regulating the generation of cells within the nervous system, including the formation of neuronal dendrites.
A number of not-previously-reported genetic correlations with cognitive function were found here, including with cardiovascular variables. For example, it is already known that there is a phenotypic association between cognitive function in youth and the development of hypertension by age 50 years41; we found a genetic correlation of −0.15. Other genetic correlations between cardiovascular variables and cognitive function were angina (rg = −0.18) and heart attack (rg = −0.17); again, there are known to be phenotypic associations between prior cognitive functioning and various cardiovascular outcomes41,42.
The genetic correlations between general cognitive function and eyesight were in opposite directions depending on the reported reason for wearing glasses or contact lenses; this was despite an overall positive genetic correlation between general cognitive function and wearing glasses (rg = 0.28). The result for myopia (short-sightedness; rg = 0.32) was consistent with previous evidence of a positive phenotypic43 and genetic44 correlation between this trait and cognitive function. Less genetic work has investigated the links between hyperopia (long-sightedness) and cognitive function, although our finding, a genetic correlation of rg = −0.21, was consistent with the negative phenotypic association between these variables reported in previous literature45.
We have investigated the six regions of the genome identified as having a shared effect between general cognitive function and more elementary cognitive tasks. Locus 13 on chromosome 1 contains the NMNAT2 gene. NMNAT2 is involved with Wallerian degeneration46,47; this is a neurodegenerative process which occurs after axonal injury in both the peripheral and central nervous system. Locus 15 on chromosome 2 contains ENSG00000271894, a non-coding RNA gene. SLC4A10 and DPP4 are located on chromosome 2 (locus 28). Variants in both SLC4A10 and DPP4 have been linked to schizophrenia48,49; hippocampal volume has also been linked to variants in DPP450. A variant of FOXO3 (chromosome 6; locus 69) has been shown to be associated with longevity in humans51,52; it is found in most centenarians across a variety of populations. MAPT, WNT3, CRHR1, KANSL1, and NSF are located on chromosome 17, locus 133; genetic variants within these genes have been linked to Alzheimer’s disease in APOE e4 carriers53, Parkinson’s disease54,55,56, neuroticism57, infant head circumference58, intracranial volume59, and subcortical brain region volumes60. Researchers following up the present study's results could prioritise the genetic loci uncovered herein that are associated with general cognitive function and reaction time (Supplementary Data 16 and 17), as well as those that are also associated with brain-related measures in other large GWASs. Such variants, being associated with multiple cognitive and neurological phenotypes, might help to prioritise potentially causal variants, and help to identify how differences in genotypic sequence are linked to such phenotypic consequences.
We note limitations with the cognitive phenotypes studied. For general cognitive function, phenotypic heterogeneity is a limitation, due to different tests being used in most samples. We also note the small number of cognitive tests being used in the construction of the general cognitive function phenotype in some cohorts. However, we were able to investigate this further by estimating genetic correlations for general cognitive function amongst some of the larger cohorts. These demonstrated strong positive genetic correlations that ranged from rg = 0.88–1.0 (Table 2). There were slight differences in the test questions and the testing environment for the UK Biobank’s ‘fluid’ (verbal-numerical reasoning) test in the assessment centre versus the online version. We used a bivariate GREML analysis to investigate the genetic contribution to the stability of individual differences in people’s verbal-numerical reasoning; we report a significant perfect genetic correlation. The UK Biobank’s reaction time variable is based on only four trials per participant; this is far fewer trials than would typically be measured. For example, other large UK surveys have used 40 trials in choice RT procedures61,62.
Both the overall size of the present study’s meta-analysis of GWASs and the inclusion of a single large sample, UK Biobank, are strengths, which contributed to the abundance of new findings. When compared to an analysis of only UK Biobank herein, the current meta-analysis adds 92 independent significant loci, 51 of which are novel. Yet, as genome-wide studies of other complex traits continue to increase up to and beyond a million individuals, an even larger sample size will be required in order to seek replication of these findings, identify new associations, and generate stronger polygenic predictions15,63 (Supplementary Fig. 1).
When compared to previous large studies of cognitive function and education, we replicate a large proportion, but not all, of the previously-reported significant findings. These differences in reported findings might be explained partly by differences in study populations (including age, social status, and ethnicity), phenotypes, and analysis methods. Whereas we know that there is sample overlap in the studies described, each comprises a unique set of contributing cohorts. As described above, there is substantial variation in the cognitive tests that contribute to the construction of a general cognitive function phenotype. Cognitive function is not as simple to measure as, say, height, and it is far from being standardised. This limitation applies across the GWAS meta-analysis studies, as well as within them. The use of different analysis methods—for example MTAG, which includes phenotypes other than the target phenotype—might also contribute to the different findings that have been reported. Finally, it is also possible that, although specific loci reached genome-wide significance in particular studies, there are false positives, highlighting the importance of well-powered replication studies.
Gene-based analysis has been shown to increase the power to detect associations, because the multiple testing burden is reduced, and the effects of multiple SNPs are combined together. From these gene-based analyses, the association of a gene with general cognitive function does not imply that it is causally related to this phenotype, only that the gene is in a region of strong association within a locus. These loci may contain multiple associated genes; therefore, we note that all of the associated genes that we reported may not be independent findings. However, we note that gene-based testing will not be able to detect associations that fall outside of the gene-body. This means that, if SNPs in promoter regions harbour variants that are causal to differences in general cognitive function or reaction time, they will be missed in our gene-based analyses.
General cognitive function has prominence and pervasiveness in the human life course, and it is important to understand the environmental and genetic origins of its variation in the population4. The unveiling here of many genetic loci, genes, and genetic pathways that contribute to its heritability (Fig. 2; Supplementary Data 1, 6 and 7)—which it shares, as we find here, with many health outcomes, longevity, brain structure, and processing speed—provides a foundation for exploring the mechanisms that bring about and sustain cognitive efficiency through life.
Methods
Participants and cognitive phenotypes
The present study includes 300,486 individuals of European ancestry from 57 population-based cohorts brought together by the Cohorts for Heart and Aging Research in Genomic Epidemiology (CHARGE), the Cognitive Genomics Consortium (COGENT) consortia, and UK Biobank (Supplementary Note 2). All individuals were aged between 16 and 102 years. Exclusion criteria included clinical stroke (including self-reported stroke) or prevalent dementia (Supplementary Data 18).
General cognitive function, unlike height for example, is not measured the same way in all samples. Here, this was mitigated by applying a consistent method of extracting a general cognitive function component from cognitive test data in the cohorts of the CHARGE and COGENT consortia; all individuals were of European ancestry (Supplementary Note 1).
For each of the CHARGE and COGENT cohorts, a general cognitive function component phenotype was constructed from a number of cognitive tasks. Each cohort was required to have tasks that tested at least three different cognitive domains. We avoided taking more than one cognitive test score from any individual cognitive test. Principal component analysis was applied to the cognitive test scores to derive a measure of general cognitive function. Principal component analyses results for the CHARGE cohorts were checked by one author (IJD) to establish the presence of a single component. The scree slope was examined, the percentage of variance accounted for by the first unrotated principal component was noted, and it was checked that all tests had sufficient loading on the first unrotated principal component. Scores on the first unrotated component were used as the cognitive phenotype (general cognitive function). Principal component analyses for the COGENT cohorts are described in Trampush et al. (pp. 337–338, and Supplementary Table 1)64.
UK Biobank participants were asked 13 multiple-choice questions that assessed verbal and numerical reasoning (VNR: UK Biobank calls this the ‘fluid’ cognitive test). The VNR score was the number of questions answered correctly in 2 min. Four samples of UK Biobank participants with verbal-numerical reasoning scores were used in the current analyses. The first sample (VNR Assessment Centre) consists of UK Biobank participants who completed the verbal-numerical reasoning test at baseline in assessment centres (n = 107,586). The second UK Biobank sample (VNR T2) consists of participants who did not complete the verbal-numerical reasoning test at baseline but did complete this test at the first repeat assessment visit in assessment centres (n = 11,123). The third UK Biobank sample (VNR MRI) consists of participants who did not complete the verbal-numerical reasoning test at a previous testing occasion but did complete the test at the imaging visit in assessment centres (n = 3002). The fourth UK Biobank sample (VNR Web-Based) consists of participants who did not complete the verbal-numerical reasoning test at any assessment centre visit, but did complete this test during the web-based cognitive assessment online (n = 46,322). Details of the cognitive phenotypes for all cohorts can be found in Supplementary Note 1.
At the baseline UK Biobank assessment, 496,790 participants completed the reaction time test. Details of the test can be found in Supplementary Note 1. A sample of 330,069 UK Biobank participants with scores on both the reaction time test and genotyping data was used in this study.
Genome-wide association analyses
Genotype–phenotype association analyses were performed within each cohort, using an additive model, on imputed SNP dosage scores. Adjustments for age, sex, and population stratification were included in the model for each cohort. Cohort-specific covariates—for example, site or familial relationships—were also fitted as required. Cohort-specific quality control procedures, imputation methods, and covariates are described in Supplementary Data 19. Quality control of the cohort-level summary statistics was performed using the EasyQC software65, which implemented the exclusion of SNPs with imputation quality <0.6 and minor allele count <25.
General cognitive function meta-analysis
A meta-analysis including all the CHARGE-COGENT and UK Biobank summary results was performed using the METAL package with a sample-size weighted model implemented (http://www.sph.umich.edu/csg/abecasis/Metal).
Reaction time genome-wide association analysis
The GWAS of reaction time from the UK Biobank sample was performed using the BGENIE v1.2 analysis package (https://jmarchini.org/bgenie/). A linear SNP association model was tested which accounted for genotype uncertainty. Reaction time was adjusted for the following covariates: age, sex, genotyping batch, genotyping array, assessment centre, and 40 principal components.
Genomic risk loci characterization using FUMA
Genomic risk loci were defined from the SNP-based association results, using FUnctional Mapping and Annotation of genetic associations (FUMA)23. Firstly, independent significant SNPs were identified using the SNP2GENE function and defined as SNPs with a P-value of ≤5 × 10−8 and independent of other genome wide significant SNPs at r2 < 0.6. Using these independent significant SNPs, tagged SNPs to be used in subsequent annotations were identified as all SNPs that had a MAF ≥ 0.0005 and were in LD of r2 ≥ 0.6 with at least one of the independent significant SNPs. These tagged SNPs included those from the 1000 genomes reference panel and need not have been included in the GWAS performed in the current study. Genomic risk loci that were 250 kb or closer were merged into a single locus. Lead SNPs were also identified using the independent significant SNPs and were defined as those that were independent from each other at r2 < 0.1.
Comparison with previous findings
Previous evidence of association for each of the 148 genetic loci identified herein as being associated with general cognitive function was sought in the largest published GWASs of general cognitive function16,17 and education24. We performed look-ups on all tagged SNPs (r2 > 0.6) within each locus, including all 1000 genomes SNPs, and classed any tagged SNP previously reported as genome-wide significant, as replication. Details of these findings are presented in Supplementary Data 3.
Gene-based analysis implemented in FUMA
Gene-based analysis has been shown to increase the power to detect genotype-phenotype association because the multiple testing burden is reduced, and the effect of multiple SNPs is combined together66. Gene-based analysis was conducted using MAGMA67. The test carried out using MAGMA, as implemented in FUMA, was the default SNP-wise test using the mean χ2 statistic derived on a per gene basis. SNPs were mapped to genes based on genomic location. All SNPs that were located within the gene-body were used to derive a P-value describing the association found with general cognitive function and reaction time. The SNP-wise model from MAGMA was used and the NCBI build 37 was used to determine the location and boundaries of 18,199 autosomal genes. Linkage disequilibrium within and between each gene was gauged using the 1000 genomes phase 3 release68. A Bonferroni correction was applied to control for multiple testing; the genome-wide significance threshold was P < 2.75 × 10−6.
Estimation of SNP-based heritability
The proportion of variance explained by all common SNPs was estimated using univariate GCTA-GREML analyses69 in four of the largest individual cohorts: ELSA, Understanding Society, UK Biobank, and Generation Scotland. Sample sizes for all of the GCTA analyses in these cohorts differed from the association analyses, because one individual was excluded from any pair of individuals who had an estimated coefficient of relatedness of >0.025 to ensure that effects due to shared environment were not included. The same covariates were included in all GCTA-GREML analyses as for the SNP-based association analyses.
Univariate Linkage Disequilibrium Score regression
Univariate LDSC regression was performed on the summary statistics from the GWAS on general cognitive function and reaction time. The heritability Z-score provides a measure of the polygenic signal found in each data set. Values greater than four indicate that the data are suitable for use with bivariate LDSC regression70. The mean χ2 statistic indicates the inflation of the GWAS test statistics that, under the null hypothesis of no association (i.e., no inflation of test statistics), would be one. An inflation in the test statistics can indicate population stratification, cryptic relatedness, or the presence of many alleles each with a small effect. The intercept of the LDSC regression can detect the difference between inflation due to stratification and cryptic relatedness, and the inflation due to a polygenic signal. This is because the inflation in test statistics attributable to stratification, drift, and cryptic relatedness will not correlate with LD, whereas inflation due to polygenicity will. The LDSC regression intercept, therefore, captures the inflation in the χ2 statistics that is not due to stratification or other confounds.
For each GWAS, an LD regression was carried out by regressing the GWA test statistics (χ2) on to each SNP’s LD score, which is the sum of squared correlations between the minor allele frequency count of a SNP with the minor allele frequency count of every other SNP. This regression allows for the estimation of heritability from the slope, and a means to detect residual confounders using the intercept. For general cognitive function, we report an LD score regression intercept of 1.058 (SE = 0.011) and a ratio of 0.0659; this indicates that only 6.6% of the inflation observed can be ascribed to causes other than a polygenic signal. For reaction time, we report an LD score regression intercept of 1.02 (SE = 0.009) and a ratio 0.0475; this indicates that only 4.75% of the inflation observed can be ascribed to causes other than a polygenic signal.
LD scores and weights were downloaded from (http://www.broadinstitute.org/~bulik/eur_ldscores/) for use with European populations. A minor allele frequency cut-off of >0.1 and an imputation quality score of >0.9 were applied to the GWAS summary statistics. Following this, SNPs were retained if they were found in HapMap 3 with MAF >0.05 in the 1000 Genomes EUR reference sample. Following this, indels and structural variants were removed along with strand ambiguous variants. SNPs whose alleles did not match those in the 1000 Genomes were also removed. As the presence of outliers can increase the standard error in LDSC score regression70 and so SNPs where χ2 > 80 were also removed.
Genetic correlations
Genetic correlations were estimated using two methods, bivariate GCTA-GREML71 and LDSC70. Bivariate GCTA was used to calculate genetic correlations between phenotypes and cohorts where the genotyping data were available. This method was used to calculate the genetic correlations between different cohorts for the general cognitive function phenotype. It was also employed to investigate the genetic contribution to the stability of the same UK Biobank’s participants’ verbal-numerical reasoning test scores in the assessment centre and then in web-based, online testing. In cases where only GWA summary results were available, bivariate LDSC was used to estimate genetic correlations between two traits. This was used to estimate the degree of overlap between polygenic architecture of the traits. Bivariate LDSC regression was used to estimate genetic correlations between general cognitive function, reaction time, and the following health outcomes: ADHD, age at menarche, age at menopause, Alzheimer's disease, anorexia nervosa, bipolar disorder, BMI, bone density femoral neck, bone density lumbar spine, coronary artery disease, HbA1c, HDL cholesterol, hippocampal volume, intracranial volume, LDL cholesterol, longevity, lung cancer, major depression, neuroticism, schizophrenia, smoking status, triglycerides, type 2 diabetes, waist-hip ratio, autism spectrum disorder, birth weight, depressive symptoms, hypertension, pulse wave arterial stiffness, angina, heart attack, parental longevity, forced expiratory volume in 1-second (FEV1), hand grip strength, happiness, health satisfaction, heel bone mineral density, osteoarthritis, overall health rating, wearing of glasses or contact lenses, long-sightedness, short-sightedness, sleep duration, sleeplessness/insomnia, and subjective wellbeing. For Alzheimer’s disease, a 500-kb region surrounding APOE was excluded and the analysis re-run (Alzheimer’s disease (500 kb)). Supplementary Data 20 provides further details on the sources of the GWAS summary statistics.
Polygenic prediction
Polygenic profile score analyses were used to predict cognitive test performance in Generation Scotland, the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing, and Understanding Society. Polygenic profiles were created in PRSice72 using results of a general cognitive function meta-analysis that excluded the Generation Scotland, the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing, and Understanding Society cohorts. Polygenic profiles were also created in these cohorts based on the UK Biobank GWA reaction time results. SNPs with a MAF < 0.01 were removed prior to creating the polygenic profiles. Clumping was used to obtain SNPs in linkage disequilibrium with an r2 < 0.25 within a 250 kb window. Polygenic profile scores were created at P-value thresholds of 0.01, 0.05, 0.1, 0.5, and 1 (all SNPs), based on the significance of the association in the general cognitive function and reaction time GWAS. Linear regression models were used to examine the associations between the polygenic profile and cognitive ability in GS, ELSA, and US, adjusting for age at measurement, sex, and the first 10 (GS), 15 (ELSA), and 20 (US) genetic principal components to adjust for population stratification. The false discovery rate (FDR) method was used to correct for multiple testing across the polygenic profiles at all five thresholds73.
Functional annotation implemented in FUMA23
The independent significant SNPs and those in LD with the independent significant SNPs were annotated for functional consequences on gene functions using ANNOVAR74 and the Ensembl genes build 85. A CADD score75, RegulomeDB score76, and 15-core chromatin states77,78,79 were obtained for each SNP. eQTL information was obtained from the following databases: GTEx (http://www.gtexportal.org/home/), BRAINEAC (http://www.braineac.org/), Blood eQTL Browser (http://genenetwork.nl/bloodeqtlbrowser/), and BIOS QTL browser (http://genenetwork.nl/biosqtlbrowser/). Functionally-annotated SNPs were then mapped to genes based on physical position on the genome, eQTL associations (all tissues) and chromatin interaction mapping (all tissues). Intergenic SNPs were mapped to the two closest up- and down-stream genes which can result in their being assigned to multiple genes.
Gene-set analysis implemented in FUMA
In order to test whether the polygenic signal measured in each of the GWASs clustered in specific biological pathways, a competitive gene-set analysis was performed. Gene-set analysis was conducted in MAGMA67 using competitive testing, which examines if genes within the gene set are more strongly associated with each of the cognitive phenotypes than other genes. Such competitive tests have been shown to control for Type 1 error rate as well as facilitating an understanding of the underlying biology of cognitive differences80,81. A total of 10,891 gene-sets (sourced from Gene Ontology82, Reactome83, and, SigDB84) were examined for enrichment of general cognitive function and reaction time. A Bonferroni correction was applied to control for the multiple tests performed on the 10,891 gene sets available for analysis.
Gene-property analysis implemented in FUMA
A gene-property analysis was conducted using MAGMA in order to indicate the role of particular tissue types that influence differences in general cognitive function and reaction time. The goal of this analysis was to test if, in 30 broad tissue types and 53 specific tissues, tissue-specific differential expression levels were predictive of the association of a gene with general cognitive function and reaction time. Tissue types were taken from the GTEx v6 RNA-seq database85 with expression values being log2 transformed with a pseudocount of 1 after winsorising at 50, with the average expression value being taken from each tissue. Multiple testing was controlled for using a Bonferroni correction.
Data availability
The GWAS summary results for all significant and suggestive SNPs for general cognitive function and reaction time are available in Supplementary Data 1, 2, 10 and 11. The full GWAS summary results for Reaction Time are available to download here: http://www.ccace.ed.ac.uk/node/335. Access to the full GWAS summary results for general cognitive function can be requested by application to the chairs of the CHARGE and COGENT consortia.
Change history
01 May 2019
Christina M. Lill, who contributed to analysis of data, was inadvertently omitted from the author list in the originally published version of this article. This has now been corrected in both the PDF and HTML versions of the article.
References
Spearman, C. “General Intelligence” objectively determined and measured. Am. J. Psychol. 15, 201–293 (1904).
Carroll, J. B. Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor Analytic Studies (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1993).
Deary, I. J. Intelligence. Curr. Biol. 23, R673–R676 (2013).
Plomin, R. & Deary, I. J. Genetics and intelligence differences: five special findings. Mol. Psychiatry 20, 98–108 (2015).
Tucker-Drob, E. M., Briley, D. A. & Harden, K. P. Genetic and environmental influences on cognition across development and context. Curr. Direct Psychol. Sci. 22, 349–355 (2013).
Deary, I. J., Johnson, W. & Starr, J. M. Are processing speed tasks biomarkers of cognitive ageing? Psychol. Aging 25, 219–228 (2010).
Brand, C. The importance of human intelligence. In Arthur Jensen: Consensus and Controversy (eds. Modgil, S. & Modgil, C.) 297 (Routledge, London, 1987).
Deary, I. J. Intelligence. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 63, 453–482 (2012).
Deary, I. J. The stability of intelligence from childhood to old age. Curr. Direct Psychol. Sci. 23, 239–245 (2014).
Deary, I. J. et al. Genetic contributions to stability and change in intelligence from childhood to old age. Nature 482, 212–215 (2012).
Lyons, M. J. et al. A longitudinal twin study of general cognitive ability over four decades. Dev. Psychol. 53, 1170–1177 (2017).
Haworth, C. M. et al. The heritability of general cognitive ability increases linearly from childhood to young adulthood. Mol. Psychiatry 15, 1112–1120 (2010).
Davies, G. et al. Genome-wide association study of cognitive functions and educational attainment in UK Biobank (N=112 151). Mol. Psychiatry 21, 758–767 (2016).
Hill, W. D. et al. Genomic analysis of family data reveals additional genetic effects on intelligence and personality. Mol. Psychiatry https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-017-0005-1 (2018).
Visscher, P. M. et al. 10 years of GWAS discovery: biology, function, and translation. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 101, 5–22 (2017).
Sniekers, S. et al. Genome-wide association meta-analyses of 78,308 individuals identifies new loci and genes influencing human intelligence. Nat. Genet. 49, 1107–1112 (2017).
Hill, W. D. et al. A combined analysis of genetically correlated traits identifies 187 loci and a role for neurogenesis and myelination in intelligence. Mol. Psychiatry https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-017-0001-5 (2018).
Deary, I. J. & Der, G. Reaction time explains IQ’s association with death. Psychol. Sci. 16, 64–69 (2005).
Luciano, M. et al. Genetic covariance among measures of information processing speed, working memory, and IQ. Behav. Genet 31, 581–592 (2001).
Der, G. & Deary, I. J. The relationship between intelligence and reaction time varies with age: results from three representative narrow-age cohorts at 30, 50 and 69 years. Intelligence 64, 89–97 (2017).
Deary, I. J. Human intelligence differences: Towards a combined experimental-differential approach. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 164–170 (2001).
Hill, W. D. Molecular genetic aetiology of general cognitive function is enriched in evolutionarily conserved regions. Transl. Psychiatry 6, e980 (2016).
Watanabe, K., Taskesen, E., van Bochoven, A. & Posthuma, D. Functional mapping and annotation of genetic associations with FUMA. Nat. Commun. 8, 1826 (2017).
Okbay, A. et al. Genome-wide association study identifies 74 loci associated with educational attainment. Nature 533, 539–542 (2016).
Davies, G. et al. Genetic contributions to variation in general cognitive function: a meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies in the CHARGE consortium (N= 53 949). Mol. Psychiatry 20, 183–192 (2015).
Evans, L. et al. Comparison of methods that use whole genome data to estimate the heritability and genetic architecture of complex traits. Nat. Genet. 50, 737–745 (2018).
Tim-Aroon, T. et al. 1q21.3 deletion involving GATAD2B: An emerging recurrent microdeletion syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. A 173, 766–770 (2017).
Olesen, R. H. et al. Obesity and age-related alterations in the gene expression of zinc-transporter proteins in the human brain. Transl. Psychiatry 6, e838 (2016).
Banfi, S. et al. Identification and characterization of the gene causing type 1 spinocerebellar ataxia. Nat. Genet. 7, 513–519 (1994).
Meda, S. A. et al. Polymorphism of DCDC2 reveals differences in cortical morphology of healthy individuals—a preliminary voxel based morphometry study. Brain Imaging Behav. 2, 21–26 (2008).
Meng, H. et al. DCDC2 is associated with reading disability and modulates neuronal development in the brain. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 17053–17058 (2005).
Lind, P. A. et al. Dyslexia and DCDC2: normal variation in reading and spelling is associated with DCDC2 polymorphisms in an Australian population sample. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 18, 668–673 (2010).
Sato, S. et al. Tau-tubulin kinase 1 (TTBK1), a neuron-specific tau kinase candidate, is involved in tau phosphorylation and aggregation. J. Neurochem. 98, 1573–1784 (2006).
Ikezu, S., & Ikezu, T. Tau-tubulin kinase. Front. Mol. Neurosci https://doi.org/10.3389/fnmol.2014.00033. (2014). .
Oksenberg, N. & Ahituv, N. The role of AUTS2 in neurodevelopment and human evolution. Trends Genet 29, 600–608 (2013).
Evers, C. et al. Exome sequencing reveals a novel CWF19L1 mutation associated with intellectual disability and cerebellar atrophy. Am. J. Med. Genet. A 170, 1502–1509 (2016).
Shibata, H. et al. A novel protein with RNA-binding motifs interacts with ataxin-2. Hum. Mol. Genet 9, 1303–1313 (2000).
Conboy, J. G. Developmental regulation of RNA processing by Rbfox proteins. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. RNA. 8, e1398 (2017).
Falco, M. et al. RAI1 gene mutations: mechanisms of Smith-Magenis syndrome. Appl. Clin. Genet. 10, 85–94 (2017).
Li, Z. et al. Genome-wide association analysis identifies 30 new susceptibility loci for schizophrenia. Nat. Genet. 49, 1576–1583 (2017).
Wraw, C., Deary, I. J., Gale, C. R. & Der, G. Intelligence in youth and health at age 50. Intelligence 53, 23–32 (2015).
Schaefer, J. J. et al. Early-life intelligence predicts midlife biological age. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 71, 968–977 (2016).
Teasdale, T. W., Fuchs, J. & Goldschmidt, E. Degree of myopia in relation to intelligence and educational level. Lancet 2, 1351–1354 (1988).
Williams, K. M. et al. Evidence for shared genetic factors between myopia and intelligence in the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS). Invest Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 56, 907–907 (2015).
Grosvenor, T. The neglected hyperope. Optom. Vis. Sci. 48, 376–382 (1971).
Gilley, J. & Coleman, M. P. Endogenous Nmnat2 is an essential survival factor for maintenance of healthy axons. PLOS Biol. 8, e1000300 (2010).
Watanabe, M., Tsukiyama, T. & Hatakeyama, S. Protection of vincristine-induced neuropathy by WldS expression and the independence of the activity of Nmnat1. Neurosci. Lett. 411, 228–232 (2007).
Ripke, S. et al. Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature 511, 421–427 (2014).
Goes, F. S. et al. Genome-wide association study of schizophrenia in Ashkenazi Jews. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 168, 649–659 (2015).
Bis, J. C. et al. Common variants at 12q14 and 12q24 are associated with hippocampal volume. Nat. Genet. 44, 545–551 (2012).
Willcox, B. J. et al. FOXO3A genotype is strongly associated with human longevity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 13987–13992 (2008).
Flachsbart, F. et al. Association of FOXO3A variation with human longevity confirmed in German centenarians. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 2700–2705 (2009).
Jun, G. et al. A novel Alzheimer disease locus located near the gene encoding tau protein. Mol. Psychiatry 21, 108–117 (2016).
Liu, X. et al. Genome-wide association study identifies candidate genes for Parkinson's disease in an Ashkenazi Jewish population. BMC Med. Genet. 12, 104 (2011).
Pankratz, N. et al. Meta-analysis of Parkinson's disease: identification of a novel locus, RIT2. Ann. Neurol. 71, 370–384 (2012).
Nalls, M. A. et al. Large-scale meta-analysis of genome-wide association data identifies six new risk loci for Parkinson's disease. Nat. Genet. 46, 989–993 (2014).
Smith, D. J. et al. Genome-wide analysis of over 106000 individuals identifies 9 neuroticism-associated loci. Mol. Psychiatry 21, 749–757 (2016).
Tall, H. R. et al. Common variants at 12q15 and 12q24 are associated with infant head circumference. Nat. Genet. 44, 532–538 (2012).
Ikram, M. A. et al. Common variants at 6q22 and 17q21 are associated with intracranial volume. Nat. Genet 44, 539–544 (2012).
Hibar, D. P. et al. Common genetic variants influence human subcortical brain structures. Nature 520, 224–229 (2015).
Deary, I. J., Der, G. & Ford, G. Reaction times and intelligence differences: a population-based cohort study. Intelligence 29, 389–399 (2001).
Der, G. & Deary, I. J. Reaction time age changes and sex differences in adulthood. Results from a large, population based study: the UK Health and Lifestyle Survey. Psychol. Aging 21, 62–73 (2006).
Plomin, R. & von Stumm, S. The new genetics of intelligence. Nat. Rev. Genet. 19 148–159 (2018).
Trampush, J. W. et al. GWAS meta-analysis reveals novel loci and genetic correlates for general cognitive function: a report from the COGENT consortium. Mol. Psychiatry 22, 336–345 (2017).
Winkler, T. W. et al. Quality control and conduct of genome-wide association meta-analyses. Nat. Protoc. 9, 1192–1212 (2014).
Liu, J. Z. et al. A versatile gene-based test for genome-wide association studies. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 87, 139–145 (2010).
de Leeuw, C. A., Mooij, J. M., Heskes, T. & Posthuma, D. MAGMA: Generalized Gene-Set Analysis of GWAS Data. PLOS Comp. Biol. 11, e1004219 (2015).
1000 Genomes Project Consortium. A map of human genome variation from population-scale sequencing. Nature 467, 1061–1073 (2012).
Yang, J. et al. Common SNPs explain a large proportion of the heritability for human height. Nat. Genet. 42, 565–569 (2010).
Bulik-Sullivan, B. et al. An atlas of genetic correlations across human diseases and traits. Nat. Genet. 47, 1236–1241 (2015).
Lee, S. H., Yang, J., Goddard, M. E., Visscher, P. M. & Wray, N. R. Estimation of pleiotropy between complex diseases using SNP-derived genomic relationships and restricted maximum likelihood. Bioinformatics 28, 2540–2542 (2012).
Euesden, J., Lewis, C. M. & O'Reilly, P. F. PRSice: Polygenic Risk Score software. Bioinformatics 31, 1466–1468 (2015).
Benjamini, Y. & Hochberg, Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B Stat. Methodol. 57, 289–300 (1995).
Wang, K., Li, M. & Hakonarson, H. ANNOVAR: functional annotation of genetic variants from high-throughput sequencing data. Nucleic Acids Res. 38, e164–e164 (2010).
Kircher, M. et al. A general framework for estimating the relative pathogenicity of human genetic variants. Nat. Genet. 46, 310–315 (2014).
Boyle, A. P. et al. Annotation of functional variation in personal genomes using RegulomeDB. Genome Res. 22, 1790–1797 (2012).
ENCODE Project Consortium. An integrated encyclopedia of DNA elements in the human genome. Nature 489, 57–74 (2012).
Ernst, J. & Kellis, M. ChromHMM: automating chromatin-state discovery and characterization. Nat. Methods 9, 215–216 (2012).
Kundaje, A. et al. Integrative analysis of 111 reference human epigenomes. Nature 518, 317–330 (2015).
Hill, W. D. et al. Human cognitive ability is influenced by genetic variation in components of postsynaptic signalling complexes assembled by NMDA receptors and MAGUK proteins. Transl. Psychiatry 4, e341 (2014).
de Leeuw, C. A., Neale, B. M., Heskes, T. & Posthuma, D. The statistical properties of gene-set analysis. Nat. Rev. Genet. 17, 353–364 (2016).
Ashburner, M. et al. Gene ontology: tool for the unification of biology. Nat. Genet. 25, 25–29 (2012).
Fabregat, A. et al. The reactome pathway knowledgebase. Nucleic Acids Res. 44, D481–D487 (2015).
Subramanian, A. et al. Gene set enrichment analysis: a knowledge-based approach for interpreting genome-wide expression profiles. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 15545–15550 (2005).
The GTEx Consortium. The Genotype-Tissue Expression (GTEx) pilot analysis: multitissue gene regulation in humans. Science 348, 648–660 (2015).
Davies, G. et al. Genome-wide association studies establish that human intelligence is highly heritable and polygenic. Mol. Psychiatry 16, 996–1005 (2011).
Lencz, T. et al. Molecular genetic evidence for genetic overlap between general cognitive ability and risk for schizophrenia: a report from the cognitive genomics consortium (COGENT). Mol. Psychiatry 19, 168–174 (2014).
Benyamin, B. et al. Childhood intelligence is heritable, highly polygenic and associated with FNBP1L. Mol. Psychiatry 19, 253–258 (2014).
Kirkpatrick, R. M., McGue, M., Iacono, W. G., Miller, M. B. & Basu, S. Results of a “GWAS Plus:” general cognitive ability is substantially heritable and massively polygenic. PLOS One. 9, e112390 (2014).
Acknowledgements
This research was conducted in The University of Edinburgh Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, funded by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council and Medical Research Council (MR/K026992/1). This research was conducted using the UK Biobank Resource (Application Nos. 10279 and 4844). The Neurology Working Group within the Cohorts for Heart and Aging Research in Genomic Epidemiology is partly supported by grants from the National Institute on Aging (R01 AG033193, U01 AG049505 and U01 AG052409). Cohort-specific acknowledgements are in Supplementary Note 3.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
G.D. and I.J.D. drafted the manuscript with contributions from M.Lu, S.E.H., W.D.H., S.J.R., S.P.H., C.F-R., and J.O.; G.D., J.W.T. and M.La performed quality control of the CHARGE-COGENT data. I.J.D. designed and supervised the cognitive psychometric analyses in the CHARGE cohorts. G.D. and R.E.M. performed quality control of UK Biobank data. G.D., J.W.T. and M.La analysed the data. S.E.H., W.D.H., S.P.H., and M.Lu performed/assisted with downstream analysis. G.D. and I.J.D. co-ordinated the CHARGE and UK Biobank work, and their integration with COGENT; T.L., J.W.T. and M.La co-ordinated the COGENT work. All authors supplied phenotype data, genotype data, and GWA results, and commented on and approved the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
A.M.D. is a Founder of and holds equity in CorTechs Labs, Inc., and serves on its Scientific Advisory Board. He is a member of the Scientific Advisory Board of Human Longevity, Inc., and receives funding through research agreements with General Electric Healthcare and Medtronic, Inc. The terms of these arrangements have been reviewed and approved by UCSD in accordance with its conflict of interest policies. B.M.P. serves on a DSMB for a clinical trial of a device funded by the manufacturer (Zoll LifeCor), and on the steering committee of the Yale Open Data Access Project funded by Johnson & Johnson. I.J.D. is a participant in UK Biobank. All other authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary material
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.