Introduction

Genetic testing for cancer predispositions was used for the first time in 1993/1994 when testing for hereditary breast and ovarian cancer became possible [1, 2]. Pre-test genetic counselling aims to enable individuals to make an informed decision regarding the genetic test and facilitates self-management. Besides disease-specific information, genetic counselling should provide information regarding possible psychological, social and legal consequences of genetic testing. Healthcare professionals offering genetic counselling should tailor the content of the consultation to the individual needs and expectations of their counsellees [3]. Thus, it is essential for them to anticipate which needs and expectations counsellees have in the decision-making process for or against genetic testing.

The main reasons for undergoing genetic testing for cancer predisposition are the early detection of cancer and a feeling of responsibility towards family members [4,5,6,7]. Many at-risk individuals make their decision intuitively and have already decided before genetic counselling to undergo genetic testing [8, 9]. Reasons declining genetic testing for cancer predisposition include lack of knowledge, anxiety, perception of non-relevance, objections from relatives and privacy concerns [10,11,12]. While people perceive themselves as well informed, they perform poorly on knowledge surveys [13].

Most studies about the decision-making process for genetic testing for cancer predisposition stem from English-speaking countries, and there is a particular lack of data from the German-speaking area. While recent studies have investigated this phenomenon in Southern Europe, particularly in Italy [8, 9], there are considerable differences between European countries regarding legislation [14,15,16], public discourse [17] and culture [8, 18, 19]. This study aims to fill the existing research gap by interviewing at-risk individuals from the German-speaking part of Switzerland.

In Switzerland, Article 14/1 of the Federal Act on Human Genetic Testing (HGTA) requires professional genetic counselling before and after predictive genetic testing [20]. The decision-making process starts when a person first realises that he or she might be genetically predisposed to cancer. After that, the first genetic consultation takes place, which is mandatory in order to get a test referral. The healthcare professional offering genetic counselling discloses and discusses information about testing procedures and the meaning of test results with the counsellee [3, 20]. That includes implications for the patient and relatives, the meaning of positive and negative test results and the medical consequences of the test. There is a mandatory reflection period (usually several weeks) after the pre-test genetic consultation (Article 14/4 HGTA) [20].

In order to help people through the inherently complex decision-making process of genetic testing for cancer predispositions, it is vital to assess how they make decisions about whether or not to take the genetic test and identify influencing factors. This article aims to explore how at-risk individuals in Switzerland experience the decision-making process of genetic testing for cancer predisposition.

Materials and methods

Strategy

In this qualitative interview study, we used a grounded theory approach for study design, sampling and data analysis [21, 22]. This well-established qualitative method aims to construct a theory grounded on data. We chose this method because we intended to theorise our findings and build new hypotheses, applying a post-positivist research paradigm [23]. One researcher (BZ) has a background in genetics and a positive attitude towards genetic testing. Another team member (IK) is a medical doctor working in general practice and somewhat sceptical towards genetic testing. The different attitudes allowed for a critical and constant reflection on personal views and opinions. We also asked our participants for feedback regarding the results of this study and performed two expert interviews with clinicians after the patient interviews to triangulate data and verify, support and complement our findings. The regional ethics committees of Northwest and Central Switzerland, as well as Bern, approved the study (no. 2017-00316). All at-risk individuals signed an informed consent form before the interview. Experts gave their informed consent verbally, in line with research ethics committee recommendations and Swiss law.

Recruitment

We recruited at-risk individuals through four medical doctors responsible for genetic counselling at Basel and Bern University Hospitals. They informed eligible counsellees about the study and handed over the study information leaflet. Those interested in participation contacted the interviewer proactively, except for four at-risk individuals who permitted the medical doctor to forward their contact details to be contacted directly by the interviewer. The recruitment procedure varied slightly according to doctors’ preferences for recruiting. However, all used the same inclusion criteria. Eligible for the study were men and women aged 18–70 who underwent at least one genetic counselling session for genetic testing for any cancer predisposition, were psychologically stable and not pregnant. Both cancer patients and people opting for presymptomatic genetic testing for cancer predisposition were included. The recruiting medical doctors controlled for these criteria before handing over the study information leaflet. Selection criteria were refined during the study to allow for theoretical saturation: to compensate for the higher percentage of women and those in favour of testing among participating at-risk individuals, we specifically recruited male at-risk individuals and those who decided against testing. For the expert interviews, we recruited healthcare professionals providing genetic counselling.

Data collection

We collected all data between August 2017 and February 2019. The first author (BZ) held all interviews face to face at the university hospital or the participants’ home, according to the participants’ preferences. Using a semi-structured interview guide (Supplementary Material), we asked the at-risk individuals to talk about their experiences regarding genetic counselling and genetic testing. They also answered questions regarding other kinds of testing to assess their more general attitudes, such as predictive genetic testing for nonactionable genetic variants, and predictive genetic testing for diseases with environmental influence (such as diabetes) in the form of hypothetical vignettes. The experts were asked about themes and observations that were identified during the analysis of the patient interviews. The interview guides are provided as Supplementary Material.

Data analysis

All patient and expert interviews were held, recorded and transcribed by the first author. One interview was in French, and three in High German. They were transcribed ad verbatim. The spoken language of the remaining 16 interviews was Swiss-German dialect. The first author translated them to High German during transcription, and all transcripts were anonymised. Following grounded theory, we started our inductive analysis during data collection. We used MaxQDA 2018 (VERBI GmbH) for data analysis. We first analysed all interviews by open coding and discussed them. Reflection on interpretative patterns of the researchers, constant comparison of the content of the different interviews and identification of new concepts and aspects were central for this first analytical step. We built concepts and categories by grouping and connecting initial codes constantly. At the same time, we added abductive steps, asking questions to the data, collecting information on interesting findings in further interviews, and creating and testing hypotheses on our data. We then started connecting different concepts and categories, using the coding paradigm proposed by Strauss and Corbin [21]. Finally, we performed several selective coding steps, where we critically examined and discussed hypotheses and questions raised during data analysis. In all steps, we wrote memos and discussed the analysis.

For this publication, we aimed for an English translation of illustrating quotations that reflects their meaning and context. Three researchers independently checked the quotations’ translation for appropriateness. The two expert interviews took place before the selective coding and were used to inform and confirm the analysis. Since these two expert interviews did not add any new aspect to our analysis, we assumed theoretical saturation. At the end stage of the project, we sent the participating at-risk individuals our preliminary results, asking them to comment on how well they could identify themselves with them. Eleven out of eighteen (61%) replied. Two of them indicated a slightly different position than we would have anticipated from the interview, and we included their additional comments in our analysis.

Results

We interviewed 18 at-risk individuals (Table 1) and two medical doctors specialised in genetics. Most at-risk individuals felt well informed and were happy with the quality and content of the genetic consultation. Criticism focused on clinical procedures and psychological components such as unexpected emotional distress while waiting for the test result. Most at-risk individuals had sustainably consolidated decisions regarding genetic testing and showed little uncertainty in this regard.

Table 1 Sample characteristics of interviewed counsellees (N = 18).

The theory

How at-risk individuals decided regarding genetic testing for cancer predispositions was a reflection of their general life philosophy. Those who preferred to control their life tended to have a positive attitude towards genetic testing. For them, knowledge created certainty and thus gave them a sense of control over their lives, both in the present and in the future. Hence, testing was important and powerful to them. Some of these individuals found it difficult to understand when others decided against testing (see Table 2 for quotation Q1). In contrast, other at-risk individuals preferred to leave their future to fate. They felt more comfortable with living in the present rather than dealing with possible future scenarios and did not wish to gain certainty through genetic knowledge. They tended to reject genetic testing (Q2). We theorise that these two positions are the two extremes of what we term the control-fate continuum. There might be numerous positions within this continuum and we do not claim that we can represent all possible positions. Instead, we provide seven examples that illustrate positions that we identified from our interview data (Box 1). We also identified psychosocial factors influencing the position on the control-fate continuum, and thus the genetic testing decision-making process (Fig. 1).

Table 2 Quotes supporting the results.
Fig. 1: Model for decision-making regarding genetic testing for cancer predisposition and nonactionable diseases in relation to life philosophy and psychosocial factors.
figure 1

1Perception that genetic testing reduces uncertainty; 2perception that genetic testing increases uncertainty.

Psychosocial factors influencing decision-making

Looking for certainty

At-risk individuals were looking for certainty when deciding whether to take genetic testing. The outcome of the decision depended on their life philosophy. On the one hand, for those who preferred control, genetic testing reduced uncertainty because it provided facts and the possibility to make informed life decisions (example 1, Q3). They were sure that knowing their genetic risk would be beneficial and expressed confidence in scientific progress. On the other hand, genetic testing increased uncertainty for those leaving life to fate (examples 4 and 7). Because the genetic test result only provided disease probabilities, it did not provide certainty. Genetic testing was not considered meaningful as long as scientific knowledge is incomplete (Q4). Moreover, some individuals explained that life was full of uncertainties and risks; therefore, they preferred to live in the present rather than in the unforeseeable future (Q5).

Anticipating consequences

Some at-risk individuals strongly focussed on the consequences of testing. They considered only genetic testing for cancer predisposition beneficial and would not take genetic testing for nonactionable diseases (examples 2 and 3). Therefore, their position on the control-fate continuum is somewhat to the right of those who prefer control in any situation.

Having children and being affected by cancer tended to strengthen at-risk individuals’ focus on consequences. Some cancer patients perceived the testing procedure as intimidating and wanted to postpone it until after cancer treatment because they did not see immediate medical consequences. Moreover, young individuals perceived genetic testing as less urgent, and some postponed genetic testing until they reached the age when their healthcare professionals recommended preventive measures (Q6). Finally, some participants stated that they would refrain from testing if the results would determine their life too much (example 3, Q7).

Four levels of consequences that at-risk individuals anticipated were identified: medical, familial, psychological and discriminatory consequences. First, some individuals based their decision on the potential for medical consequences (examples 2 and 3). These individuals proactively thought about the integration of the test result and its consequences in their life and had already decided on what preventive measures they would take if they were risk variant carriers (Q8). While some perceived frequent screening as a sufficient and satisfying measure, others needed the option of risk-reducing preventive surgery or medication to be available in order to take the test. This might have helped them rationalise and structure their decision-making process, thus reducing uncertainty.

Second, some at-risk individuals strongly anticipated consequences for their family and considered this the main reason for genetic testing (example 5). While some of them accepted that some relatives might exercise their right not to know, others did not reflect on this aspect and motivated their relatives to get genetic testing without further consideration (Q9).

Third, most at-risk individuals anticipated adverse psychological reactions, as discussed in genetic counselling. This anticipation helped them overcome feelings of anxiety and uncertainty (Q10). Still, one participant failed to anticipate psychological consequences and started to brood afterwards (Q11). Fourth, discriminatory consequences were anticipated by some few individuals. They were careful about sharing their test results because they were aware of possible discrimination by employers or private insurance (in Switzerland basic health insurance is obligatory and insurance companies cannot refuse any patients; however, this is not the case for private and semi-private insurance [20]). Still, participants did not refuse genetic testing for cancer predispositions out of privacy concerns (Q12). One interviewee even described a positive experience with his health insurance company (Q13).

Being socially influenced

A sense of duty and social influences from family, healthcare professionals and society left some at-risk individuals with no real alternative but to test even if doing so was against their life philosophy (examples 5 and 6, Q14). First, family considerations had an influence (example 5): some at-risk individuals felt that they would be excluded from their families without genetic testing (Q15). Others felt a duty towards their offspring and other family members despite their sceptical attitude towards genetic testing (Q16). Because of this feeling of obligation, one participant even wished he had never heard about the possibility of testing (Q17). Some at-risk individuals perceived not doing genetic testing as reckless, because to them genetic testing implied taking responsibility for themselves and their social environment (Q18). Some also considered that their genetic test would reduce healthcare costs, which made them advocate the test to family members (Q19).

Second, despite the non-directiveness of genetic counselling, some affected individuals followed the advice of their counselling healthcare professional to be tested (example 6). That happened particularly in the context of a newly diagnosed cancer, where genetic testing might provide therapy-relevant information. The extent to which medical doctors influenced at-risk individuals in their decision-making process depended on their relationships as well as on the at-risk individuals’ general attitudes towards doctors (Q20). One participant stressed that it was important to her to receive genetic counselling that was not influenced by her prior knowledge on the topic (Q21).

Third, we observed various societal influences. For instance, those who had decided against testing did not like to broadcast this decision because they did not want to justify their decision (Q22). By doing a genetic test, “one has done everything one can” (Doris, healthy, untested), which they felt was easier for others to understand. Moreover, online communities, such as Facebook groups or forums, provided another form of external influence. They had variable effects on at-risk individuals, some felt encouraged, others rather scared when reading how carriers dealt with their increased cancer risk. Some were emotionally neutral and used test results as an additional information source in their decision-making process.

Deciding intuitively vs reflectively

Individuals at both extremes of the control-fate continuum tended to decide quickly and intuitively: testing was naturally good for those who gained control through it (example 1), and those who preferred to leave their future life to fate rejected genetic testing equally intuitively (example 7). In addition, those who did testing mainly for their family or on their doctor’s advice intuitively outsourced their decision to these third parties (examples 5 and 6; see also “Being socially influenced”). By contrast, those in the middle of the continuum tended to reflect upon genetic testing intensively, weighing pros and cons. Their decision depended on the circumstances of testing (examples 3 and 4). One participant rejected genetic testing intuitively at first but made a more reflective decision later on, still choosing not to take the test (Q23).

Genetic counselling was vital for a reflective decision-making process (Q6). However, some individuals had already made their decision upfront and thus did not include the content of the genetic consultation in their decision (Q24). Some of them attended the genetic consultation because it was required to get the test. They admitted that it was interesting to get detailed information about the testing procedure as well as the implications and consequences of the test.

Simplifying risks

Because of the complexity of genetic risks, simplification was crucial for an effective decision-making process. While all at-risk individuals simplified risks, they did so to different extents. Some individuals simplified risks and probabilities by breaking them down into a black-and-white picture, thus reinterpreting uncertainty (Q25). Consequently, not finding a function-affecting genetic variant sometimes led to the misconception that the risk of cancer was zero. In contrast, one individual felt that being a carrier of a function-affecting genetic variant would inevitably lead to cancer, even if in reality, the penetrance is not 100% (Q26). She thus rejected genetic testing due to fear.

Another simplification concerned the anticipation of the test result. Some individuals made an intuitive estimation (Q27); others received a risk calculation during genetic counselling (Q28). Those who thought their risk of being a carrier was high tended to decide clearly and intuitively (Q29). Individuals who thought they were probably not carrying a function-affecting genetic variant tended to scrutinise the meaningfulness of genetic testing and tended to reflect more on their decision (see also “Deciding intuitively vs reflectively”).

Discussion

Our findings illustrate how people undergoing genetic counselling for cancer predisposition in Switzerland perceive the decision-making process for genetic testing. The study provides new findings of decision-making by individuals at risk for hereditary cancer in continental Europe. Our results help healthcare professionals and at-risk individuals to structure the decision-making process. We propose that at-risk individuals position themselves on the control-fate continuum, which derives of their general life philosophy. Medical doctors and genetic counsellors should understand and support individuals in this process. The position on the control-fate continuum is an essential factor for genetic testing decision-making. Those who prefer a control approach to life might benefit from genetic testing, even if the genetic variant that is tested for is not actionable [24]. However, they might also overestimate genetic testing outcomes and feel determined to do genetic testing once it is offered. In contrast, those leaving their future life to fate might refuse genetic testing decisively and be better off without it, even if preventative measures could reduce their disease risk. Several psychosocial factors influence this underlying aspect of life philosophy and thus the decision-making process.

Our findings connect to other studies of decision-making for genetic testing for cancer predisposition, in particular the psychosocial factors identified. First, it has been shown that people are looking for certainty when they do genetic testing [25]. Our findings suggest that the nature and level of certainty at-risk individuals attribute to genetic testing largely depend on their life philosophy. Second, people tend to anticipate consequences more when receiving genetic counselling. In settings without genetic counselling, many people do not anticipate the consequences of genetic testing [26].

Moreover, simplifying risks due to misinterpretation, over- or underestimation is part of human psychology [27] and is thus commonly studied in the context of genetic testing [28,29,30]. In addition, while intuitive decision makers need genetic counselling mainly for gathering more facts or because it is mandatory to get the genetic test, reflective decision makers might need more psychosocial support and more time to reflect the pros and cons of genetic testing [31, 32]. The social influence on decision-making leads back to the question of whether individuals have a real choice regarding genetic testing. Previous studies also conclude that some people agree to genetic testing out of a feeling of responsibility towards their relatives [4, 33]. While our theory represents this position as well, we suggest that individuals’ life philosophy dominates this motive at the two extremes of the control-fate continuum.

Our theory also accounts for the reasons of those who decline genetic testing. Previous studies showed that even though some decliners do so unreflectively out of fear or lack of knowledge, many refuse genetic testing consciously because they do not see benefits for themselves [11, 34, 35]. We suggest that genetic testing contradicts the life philosophy of some individuals. It is thus essential to counsel those deciding for genetic testing regarding how to inform these family members about the intention of being tested and test results. At-risk individuals have expressed difficulties with uninterested or resistant family members in previous studies [36,37,38], and our theory helps to explain why different life philosophies can lead to difficulties in communication because those in need of control have difficulties in understanding why others might not want to learn about their genetic risk.

The scope of the control-fate continuum theory covers genetic testing in both healthy and affected individuals provided that genetic testing has predictive components for some disease a person has not developed (yet). For instance, a breast cancer patient with a pathogenic BRCA variant still has an increased risk of ovarian cancer or bilateral breast cancer.

To our knowledge, this is the first study regarding the genetic testing decision-making process from the German-speaking region, which provides a different cultural setting from that of English-speaking countries, which are predominantly studied. Our interviewees were rarely worried about discrimination and did not take this variable into account in their decision-making process, as opposed to previous studies in other regions [39]. However, we acknowledge a possible bias in the sense that those worried regarding discrimination might have refrained from participating in this study. More research is needed to explore this potential cultural difference further.

Clinical implications

Our findings have implications for genetic counselling. It might be useful to discuss individuals’ life philosophy and highlight that these might differ from those of their family members. Our framework can thus be used to support decision-making, anticipate potential conflicts within families and, potentially, helps to understand the reasoning of those declining genetic testing. The finding that some make their decisions intuitively while others need more support for thorough reflection indicates that tailoring genetic counselling resources to actual information needs might be useful. This becomes especially relevant in the context of limited counselling resources and increasing demand for genetic testing. However, more investigations, including quantitative studies, are necessary to assess the appropriateness and consequences of such prioritisation regarding autonomous decision-making.

Limitations

This is a qualitative study with 18 at-risk individuals and two geneticists, and while this is a suitable number to get meaningful qualitative results [40], we propose using our theory to build hypotheses and to test them in more extensive studies. Despite actively searching for 18 months, we could only recruit three genetic testing decliners. Thus, we probably did not reach theoretical saturation for this subgroup. For ethical reasons, at-risk individuals had to contact us proactively in most cases, which gives a bias towards proactive and communicative people interested in genetic testing. Another possible bias results from the recruitment by the counselling doctors, as they might have preferred counsellees with characteristics other than the inclusion criteria. Moreover, qualitative analyses require interpretation, but we minimised personal interpretation bias by frequent team discussions, continuous personal reflection, constant comparison and other methods suggested for the grounded theory approach [21].

Conclusion

Our study illustrates the enormous interpersonal variability of the genetic testing decision-making process. Professional counselling is a way to account for this variability and ensure proper support in the complex decision-making process. The control-fate continuum can help medical geneticists and genetic counsellors to consider this interpersonal variability and provide counselling that respects and anticipates people’s wishes and needs so that counsellees can make informed choices. It also helps their counselling in terms of information dissemination to family members by learning about potential differences in life philosophy, interest and decision-making regarding genetic testing. Moreover, our findings help future research studies for hypothesis testing and result quantification. Because genetic testing is becoming more common, stratifying people regarding their information needs and decision-making strategy helps to rationalise genetic counselling in the future, by saving resources and adapting healthcare services to the needs of those at risk for hereditary cancer.