Abstract
Depression is the leading cause of disability worldwide, exerting a profound negative impact on quality of life in those who experience it. Depression is associated with disruptions to several closely related neural and cognitive processes, including dopamine transmission, fronto-striatal brain activity and connectivity, reward processing and motivation. Physical activity, especially aerobic exercise, reduces depressive symptoms, but the mechanisms driving its antidepressant effects are poorly understood. Here we propose a novel hypothesis for understanding the antidepressant effects of exercise, centred on motivation, across different levels of explanation. There is robust evidence that aerobic exercise decreases systemic inflammation. Inflammation is known to reduce dopamine transmission, which in turn is strongly implicated in effort-based decision making for reward. Drawing on a broad range of research in humans and animals, we propose that by reducing inflammation and boosting dopamine transmission, with consequent effects on effort-based decision making for reward, exercise initially specifically improves ‘interest-activity’ symptoms of depression—namely anhedonia, fatigue and subjective cognitive impairment - by increasing propensity to exert effort. Extending this framework to the topic of cognitive control, we explain how cognitive impairment in depression may also be conceptualised through an effort-based decision-making framework, which may help to explain the impact of exercise on cognitive impairment. Understanding the mechanisms underlying the antidepressant effects of exercise could inform the development of novel intervention strategies, in particular personalised interventions and boost social prescribing.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Depression is the leading cause of disability worldwide [1]. In addition to persistent low mood, core features of depression include anhedonia, fatigue and subjective cognitive impairment, known collectively as ‘interest-activity’ symptoms [2]. These symptoms can prevent people from engaging in work, disrupt social relationships and curtail the pleasure people take in life. Depression is also difficult to treat: fewer than half of people respond to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, with only around one-third remitting from their first pharmacological treatment [3] and still fewer remit after switching to a second treatment [4]. Even individuals with apparently similar symptoms of depression may respond differently to the same treatment, probably because depression is highly mechanistically heterogeneous [5].
Impaired motivation is an important feature of depression that is associated with interest-activity symptoms [6] and linked to poor treatment outcome [6]. Specifically, patients with depression - particularly those with anhedonia - have impaired reward-processing [7] and are less willing to exert effort in order to obtain reward [8,9,10]. Given that dopamine function is associated with anhedonia [11, 12], and with the propensity to exert effort in both animals [13] and humans [14] (with greater effects of dopamine manipulation in individuals with lower dopamine transmission [15]), a candidate alternative to SSRIs to treat depression is to use medications that enhance dopamine transmission. However, with the exception of bupropion [16], trials using dopaminergic agents have had limited success, likely due to the mechanistic heterogeneity of depression which may render a one-size-fits-all treatment approach ineffective [17].
Several studies indicate that dopamine transmission is enhanced by physical activity, in particular aerobic exercise [18, 19], indicating this as a potentially useful alternative method to boost dopamine and increase motivation in depression. Indeed, it is well established that any level of physical activity can help prevent [20] and treat [21, 22] depression in both adults and adolescents, with a longer duration of intervention and more intense activity having a greater effect [23]. Objectively measured cognitive impairment in depression, which is not improved by most serotonergic antidepressants [24], has also been reported to be improved by physical activity [25, 26], although the picture is more mixed than for subjective depressive symptoms such as mood. Cognitive impairment in depression is especially important and under-studied, representing a clear unmet clinical need, as it can have a profound impact on functioning and many depressed individuals struggle with enduring cognitive impairment even after other symptoms resolve [27].
Reduced inflammation is likely to be just one of several mechanisms through which exercise has an antidepressant effect. Substantial prior research indicates that exercise alters several biological processes that could influence depressive symptoms [28], such as elevated brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) [29], increased grey matter volume [30], decreased oxidative stress [31] and altered neuroendocrine responses [32, 33]. Understanding the mechanisms driving the effect of physical activity on symptoms of depression could allow exercise programmes to be made more accessible, effective and individually tailored to patients likely to respond to physical activity as an intervention. However, although there has been extensive research on the biological, neural and behavioural effects of exercise in animal models of depression, to date the mechanisms driving its antidepressant effects in humans are poorly understood [34, 35].
In this review we outline a novel framework for understanding the antidepressant effect of physical activity, especially aerobic exercise, in depression. We initially summarise possible mechanisms underlying the motivational symptoms of depression - including dopamine-driven changes in effort-based decision making and associated brain circuitry - and their relationship with inflammation. We then propose a pathway whereby through reducing inflammation and consequently boosting dopamine transmission, with downstream effects on brain circuitry involved in decision making and reward processing, exercise primarily alters interest-activity symptoms of depression by increasing propensity to exert effort to obtain reward. We hypothesise that exercise also treats cognitive impairment in depression through altering effort-related brain circuitry, thereby increasing propensity to exert cognitive as well as physical effort.
Motivational dysfunction in depression
Motivation, reward processing and effort-based decision making
Apathy and anhedonia are two important motivational symptoms. Apathy refers to a loss of enthusiasm for a variety of activities, which is often expressed as lower motivation to perform goal-directed behaviour across different domains of function [36]. Apathy is commonly seen in mental health conditions such as depression and schizophrenia and neuropsychiatric conditions such as Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease [2]. Anhedonia is defined as diminished interest or pleasure in previously enjoyable activities and is a core part of the interest-activity cluster of depressive symptoms [37]. It has been hypothesised that both apathy and anhedonia are driven by an impairment in reward processing [38,39,40,41,42]. Depressed individuals with high anhedonia show poorer reward learning compared to those with low anhedonia [43, 44], as well as lower reward sensitivity [45], even following remission [46]. However, one difficulty in interpreting his literature is that many tasks used to measure reward learning do not dissociate reward learning from reward value (the internal expectation assigned to potentially rewarding outcomes [47]).
The use of computational modelling to decompose different parameters contributing to behaviour is helpful to distinguish between parameters of reward processing; for example, one study showed that anhedonia was associated with lower inverse temperature (often interpreted as reflecting lower reward value) rather than lower learning rates [45]. More research applying computational models to reward learning paradigms is required to dissociate these features of reward processing. More broadly, reward processing is known to be disrupted in depression [7], with case-control differences most reliably observed on tests of reward bias (assessed through an asymmetrically rewarded signal detection task), cost-benefit decision making (how potential rewards are used to guide decisions) and reinforcement learning (updating expectations from outcomes to guide future choices) [48]. At the neural level, these changes are thought to be driven by altered activation during reward processing in the ventral striatum, caudate and anterior mid-cingulate cortex (aMCC, often referred to as dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), or simply ACC) in depression [49,50,51,52].
Anhedonia is associated with disruption to both dopamine function [53, 54] and striatal activation [52], consistent with preclinical studies implicating the mesolimbic dopamine system in motivational dysfunction [55, 56]. However, the mechanisms driving anhedonia are not well understood, arguably because it is driven by a variety of cognitive processes that have not been fully characterised and dissociated [57]. One potential mechanism is an increased sensitivity to the expenditure of effort to gain reward. Accordingly, depressed patients are less willing to exert effort for reward, are less able to use information about reward probability and magnitude to guide choices and performance on effort-based decision-making tasks is associated with the duration of depressive episodes [8,9,10]. Self-reported anhedonia correlates with various markers of reward processing, including behaviour and neural activation during effort-based decision-making [9, 58, 59], the ability to sustain an optimal reward response over time [44, 60], reinforcement learning [43, 61] and reward sensitivity [45]. However, based on the extant literature it is difficult to conclude that this association is truly specific to anhedonia as opposed to other symptoms of depression, as only a minority of studies have reported associations between anhedonia and reward processing that survive controlling for overall symptom severity; although this has been shown in some cases, for example reward prediction error in the striatum [62], reward learning [61] and reward response over time [44]. Anhedonia is especially important to understand because it is associated with antidepressant treatment failure [6], a more chronic course of depression [63, 64] and fewer depression-free days following antidepressant treatment [65]. Antidepressants are generally ineffective for treating motivational dysfunction and can even exacerbate these symptoms in some patients, indicating an important unmet clinical need [66,67,68].
A common method used to assess effort-based decision-making is to vary the level of physical effort required to obtain reward, as in the Effort-Expenditure for Rewards Task (EEfRT) [69]. In this task, physical effort is manipulated by the number of button presses required in a fixed amount of time and the potential reward that can be obtained is varied [69]. Motivation is indexed by the proportion of trials on which participants opt for the ‘high effort/high reward’ option, relative to the ‘low effort/low reward’ option, similar to analogous tests in rodents [70]. Studies using the EEfRT have revealed lower willingness to expend effort for reward in patients with subsyndromal depression, first-episode depression and recurrent major depressive disorder, compared to controls, particularly in those with high anhedonia [8, 9]. Importantly, choices to engage on the EEfRT also correlate with trait anhedonia in healthy participants [69]. Another physical effort task is the grip-force task: participants are required to squeeze a gripper at different intensities to win differing levels of reward. Healthy participants with high apathy traits (a motivational impairment which overlaps with anhedonia [42]) show an increased modulation of grip force by effort level [71].
However, the above results are confounded by physical ability (either button-pressing speed or strength). One way to overcome this confound is to calibrate the required level of physical exertion to each individual participant and examine the decision to accept or reject a challenge, instead of comparing high vs low effort options. In such designs participants decide on each trial whether to accept an effortful challenge to win varying amounts of reward, as in the Apple Gathering Task (AGT) [72]. Motivation on this task is increased when Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients take their usual dopaminergic medication, highlighting the strong link between dopamine transmission and effort-based decision making [73]. Further, healthy participants with high apathy scores are more sensitive to effort (less likely to accept effort trials) on the AGT [74].
Cognitive control
Cognitive control (often called executive function) describes a set of effortful cognitive processes [75] which allow for flexible adaptation of behaviour in line with goals [76], including functions such as working memory, task-switching, response inhibition and attentional control [77]. Cognitive control is impaired in depression [77, 78] which may be in part due to changes in motivation - cognitive control is typically experienced as effortful [75] which could explain why people with depression avoid exerting cognitive control. Poorer performance on tests of cognitive control in depression is accompanied by other cognitive impairments, including on selective attention and memory tasks, which are not resolved by standard antidepressant treatment [79]. One possible explanation for such a broad pattern of impairment is that reduced cognitive control, arising from a failure to allocate cognitive effort, causes more general impairments in cognitive processing [79,80,81,82]. Reduced cognitive control has also been proposed as a potential mechanism driving other symptoms of depression, including indecisiveness, negative automatic thoughts, poor concentration and distorted cognitive processing [83].
Neural correlates of effort-based decision making
In humans, lesions in the basal ganglia [84, 85] and dmPFC [86, 87] are associated with a motivational deficits such as inertia and apathy, indicating the importance of these structures in signalling motivation. Motivated behaviour is signalled by an interconnected network of regions with the aMCC and ventral striatum playing key roles, demonstrated by the importance of these structures in apathy across clinical disorders [88] (Fig. 1A). A meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies showed that the aMCC and anterior insula (AI) are central to signalling effort [89]. As well as the aMCC and AI, activity in the striatum, ventral tegmental area and posterior parietal cortex have been found to signal actual and expected effort [90,91,92,93]. Direct electrical stimulation of the aMCC (during brain surgery) induces a feeling of imminent challenge [94] and in line with the importance of the aMCC in motivated behaviour, apathy is associated with increased effort sensitivity and with decreased connectivity between this region and the supplementary motor area [74].
Meta-analysis of fMRI studies has shown that whilst the aMCC and AI signal effort, the vmPFC, ventral striatum and midbrain chiefly signal reward value [89] (Fig. 1A). Specific parts of these circuits appear to integrate effort and reward to compute a net value of performing an action [48, 89]; effort and reward expectations are integrated in the ventral striatum, midbrain and aMCC [90, 95, 96]. Reward devaluation by both cognitive and physical demands is signalled in the aMCC and AI, as well as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and intraparietal sulcus [97]. The supplementary motor area and aMCC have also been shown to encode the difference in reward and effort between chosen and unchosen options, providing a signal to guide decisions about whether to engage in an effortful challenge [98].
The expected value of control (EVC) theory proposes that cognitive control is modulated by a decision-making process weighing up the potential gain of control against its cost [99]. EVC is calculated based on its efficacy (probability of an outcome, similar to confidence), value, and effort cost. The aMCC is thought to compute this decision-making process, and to signal to other regions such as the dlPFC to implement control (Fig. 1A); accordingly dlPFC activity is reduced in depression [100,101,102], including during cognitive control [103] and activity in the dlPFC predicts improvements in depressive symptoms after non-invasive brain stimulation treatment [104]. In line with an overlap between effort-related and control-related processes, the neural circuits implicated in the allocation of effort [105] and cognitive control [104] converge on a circuit known as the ‘cingulo-opercular network’ (CON, often termed the ‘salience network’ in the resting-state fMRI literature) which includes the aMCC, AI, ventrolateral thalamus and medial caudate [106,107,108], indicating shared functionality between effort allocation and cognitive control.
Dopamine, anhedonia and effort-based decision-making
Dopamine is associated with anhedonia, consistent with the link between anhedonia and motivation; indeed, it is well established that dopamine plays a central role in motivation [11, 48, 56, 109]. Molecular imaging studies indicate that D2/3 receptor availability in the ventral striatum correlates negatively with anhedonia in depressed patients [12] and with lower dopamine transporter binding in the striatum, caudate and putamen in depressed patients with anhedonia compared to controls [110]. In patients with Parkinson’s disease, anhedonia and apathy are negatively correlated with striatal dopamine transporter binding, especially as neurodegeneration progresses [11].
There is substantial evidence that dopamine transmission influences effort exertion. Administration of amphetamine, which directly releases dopamine, increases willingness to exert effort [111], although interestingly one animal study showed that while lower doses of amphetamine increased high-effort choices, higher doses had the inverse effect, decreasing high-effort choices [112]; indicating that the relationship between dopamine and effort is complex, possibly corresponding to the well-documented ‘inverted U-shaped’ function of dopaminergic modulation of PFC functioning [113]. In animals, dopamine D2 receptor antagonism decreases effort expenditure [112, 114] specifically in the nucleus accumbens (equivalent to the ventral striatum in humans) [70]. In healthy humans, individual differences in dopamine release in the striatum and vmPFC, measured with positron emission tomography (PET), were associated with willingness to expend effort [14]. This result is consistent with animal research showing that dopamine activity in the nucleus accumbens is greater in high-effort responders [115]. Effort is typically considered to be signalled by tonic dopamine transmission [56, 116], but when studied across multiple timescales in rodents, tonic (minute-by-minute) dopamine release correlates with both reward rate and motivational vigour, whilst phasic (second-by-second) dopamine release encodes expected reward, suggesting that dopamine may encode a single decision variable representing the available reward for investment of effort across multiple timescales [109].
Inflammation, anhedonia, dopamine and effort-based decision-making
There is good evidence that the motivational symptoms of depression are related to inflammation. Systemic inflammation is associated with increased odds of developing depression [117] and is dysregulated in depression [118], particularly in those with pronounced anhedonia [119]. Conversely, inhibition of inflammatory cytokines reduces depressive symptoms in patients with inflammatory conditions [120] and reduces anhedonia in depressed individuals with high inflammatory cytokines as well as in those with inflammatory conditions [121, 122]. Further, chronic inflammation is thought to result in cognitive impairment and is associated with poorer cognitive control in depression [123,124,125]. It is worth noting that findings into the antidepressant effects of anti-inflammatory medication are mixed [126], and although a meta-analysis found an overall antidepressant effect [127], important methodological limitations remain: in particular, because increased inflammation is only present in a subset of depressed individuals [128], participants should ideally be stratified by their inflammatory status at baseline and inflammatory markers should be assessed pre-and post-treatment. Further, given evidence for a link between anhedonia and inflammation [119], it is important to assess changes in anhedonia specifically, alongside depressive symptoms [129].
Substantial research suggests that dopamine activity is affected by peripheral inflammation. A recent study showed that depressed participants with high levels of inflammation who received the dopamine precursor levodopa had increased functional connectivity between the ventral striatum and vmPFC. In this study a decrease in anhedonia after levodopa was correlated with ventral striatum-vmPFC connectivity only in patients with high levels of inflammation [130], underscoring the link between inflammation, dopamine functioning, brain circuitry and anhedonia. Administration of levodopa has also been shown to reverse the effect of inflammation on striatal dopamine release in non-human primates [131]. Dysregulation of inflammation in depression has knock-on effects on reward processing: transiently increasing inflammatory cytokines such as interleukin-6 (IL-6) and tumour necrosis factor-alpha (TNF-α) decreases reward responsivity in the ventral striatum, as well as dopamine transmission in the caudate and putamen and increases depressive symptoms including anhedonia [132, 133]; and ventral striatal response to reward anticipation has been reported to mediate the effect of inflammation on mood [134].
In line with a link between inflammation and effort sensitivity, several animal studies have indicated that inducing inflammatory responses increases effort sensitivity and depression-like behaviours [135,136,137]. In humans, induced inflammation resulted in a greater sensitivity to the probability of winning when choosing to exert effort, which was also related to sleepiness [138] and in another study, inflammation increased effort sensitivity [139]. A recent study reported that depressed participants with high levels of inflammation who received levodopa did not show a decrease in effort sensitivity assessed by behaviour on the EEfRT [130], but these results may have been affected by the completion of the EEfRT after the peak concentration of levodopa had passed; additionally, this study did not include a healthy control group comparison, limiting interpretability.
How does aerobic exercise treat motivational symptoms of depression?
There are various mechanisms by which exercise might treat symptoms of depression, which are not mutually exclusive [28]. Exercise has been shown to increase BDNF [29] which may mediate the effect of exercise on mood and cognition, albeit this effect is stronger on acute measures of BDNF immediately after exercise (ES ~ 0.5) than on resting BDNF after regular programmed exercise (ES ~ 0.3) [29]. Further, the effect of exercise on BDNF has been examined mainly in animals due to the difficulty of measuring cellular and molecular changes in human brains [140]. There is also preliminary evidence that exercise increases cortical volume [30] and decreases oxidative stress [31]. There is mixed evidence for a modulation of the neuroendocrine system in depressed individuals by exercise [33], including decreased adrenocorticotrophic hormone [32] and cortisol levels [33] which can be dysregulated in depression [141]. Exercise also has psychological benefits; it enhances self-esteem [142] and self-efficacy [143,144,145] which could mediate its antidepressant effects [146], underlining the importance of assessing the effects of exercise at multiple levels of explanation.
We have summarised a possible pathway driving interest-activity symptoms of depression, whereby inflammation reduces dopamine transmission, increasing effort sensitivity and thereby anhedonia and fatigue, which can also explain cognitive impairment. In this next section, we present evidence that physical activity, especially aerobic exercise, modulates each stage in this process and propose that this represents one pathway by which such interventions reduce depressive symptoms (Fig. 1).
Antidepressant effects of physical activity
There is good meta-analytic evidence from observational studies indicating that individuals with higher levels of physical activity have lower odds of developing depression [20]; for example, those exercising for at least four hours per week had 18% lower risk of depression, with a 25% reduction in those who exercised for at least nine hours per week [147]. Based on such evidence it has been estimated that 12` of incident depression could be prevented if all adults exercised for at least nine hours per week [147], albeit this figure is derived from observational studies, which may be confounded and therefore needs to be interpreted with caution. Another meta-analysis suggested that increasing levels of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity is negatively associated with the incidence of depression as well as the occurrence of subclinical symptoms [148].
Consistent with a causal effect, meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials of aerobic exercise interventions estimate a substantial effect on overall depressive symptoms [149, 150], in both adults (ES ~ 0.8) [21] and young people (ES ~ 0.8) [22], although a lower effect size was reported in a meta-analysis including less vigorous interventions (ES ~ 0.3) [151]. Another meta-analysis found a moderate effect in favour of exercise combined with standard treatments (namely medication and psychotherapy), with the greatest benefits observed in more severely depressed patients [152]. It is important to note that it is more difficult to blind participants in exercise studies than in drug studies because of the physical nature of the intervention; however, it has been reported that participants have similar efficacy expectations for aerobic versus non-aerobic activities (such as stretching or toning exercises) [153], making any differences between an aerobic active group and a non-aerobic stretching control group less likely to be due to the placebo effect. Additionally, the standardised effect size of the antidepressant effect of exercise in the most rigorously controlled trial (ES ~ 0.7) [21] is numerically higher than for the most common psychological therapies (ES ~ 0.5) [154], which if anything face even greater challenges with blinding. Withdrawing regular physical activity increases negative mood in people who do not have depression [155], which appears to be related to changes in an inflammatory marker, IL-6 [156]. A recent review summarised the meta-analyses and systematic reviews assessing the antidepressant effects of exercise [157], concluding that it has a moderate-to-large antidepressant effect when compared to no-treatment or control groups [152, 158,159,160], is no less effective than antidepressant medication or psychological therapy [158, 159] and has a moderate benefit over treatment as usual [159]. One challenge with using exercise as a treatment for depression may be that for many patients, their symptoms can make it more difficult to engage with exercise, due to low motivation [161,162,163], fatigue [161, 162], or low mood [161, 162, 164, 165]. Additional barriers to exercise in depressed individuals include a lack of support [164], low confidence in ability [161] and fear of injury [161].
Physical activity reduces inflammation
Inflammation is a complex process involving multiple biological pathways across different tissues and systems. The physiological responses to exercise are similarly complex and vary depending on the type, frequency and intensity of the exercise performed. There are also differences between the acute response to a single bout of physical activity and the long-term adaptations which occur in response to a training programme (repeated bouts of aerobic exercise over a prolonged period).
Despite this complexity, it is widely accepted that physical activity has a net anti-inflammatory effect, as evidenced by a range of biomarkers [166]. There are multiple mechanisms by which physical activity may reduce inflammation. Whilst inflammation may be mediated by physiological adaptations such as regulating autonomic function and reducing allostatic load, it is also directly related to muscle contraction [167]. Muscle is increasingly recognised as an endocrine organ because it secretes cytokines (or myokines) to coordinate immediate responses to exercise, indicating the importance of the musculature in the anti-inflammatory effects of exercise [167,168,169]. The anti-inflammatory effects of exercise are also mediated via changes to adipose tissue, as adipocytes can be pro-inflammatory [170]. It is not the overall volume of adiposity per se which has an inflammatory effect, but rather the size of individual adipose cells and their location, with hypertrophic cells and visceral location having the biggest effect [171, 172]. Intervention studies indicate that exercise exerts an anti-inflammatory effect over adipose tissue, promoting smaller adipocyte size and loss of visceral fat [173]. Importantly, this effect is seen over and above weight loss [173].
Whilst there is robust evidence from observational studies of a negative relationship between physical activity and inflammation, results from intervention studies are more mixed, although the overall pattern is that exercise interventions tend to yield an anti-inflammatory effect [174]. An anti-inflammatory effect is observed most consistently in men, which could be due to differences in adiposity between the sexes [175], or cyclical variations in baseline levels of inflammation in women which may confound results [176]. It is also important to note that the menstrual cycle and associated fluctuations in hormones may interact with other variables that might affect the antidepressant effect of exercise, such as dopamine function [177] and exercise performance itself [178], though limited human studies and heterogeneous study designs make it difficult to draw firm conclusions. While exercise has an anti-inflammatory effect, a sedentary lifestyle is independently associated with chronic low-grade inflammatory illnesses [179, 180]. It is thus important to assess an individual’s daily activity levels in addition to their engagement in structured exercise when evaluating the effects of any intervention [179, 180].
Further complications in exercise intervention studies relate to the heterogeneity in inflammatory markers and the physical activity intervention under examination. Both the acute and chronic responses to aerobic vs resistance training differ, as do the responses to moderate vs high intensity aerobic training [181]. This is not only important when considering which intervention may be most beneficial, but also whether certain exercise regimes could be potentially harmful. Given that participants with depression may already have elevated inflammation [117], the acute stress placed on the body by certain forms of activity (for example high intensity interval training) could potentially be deleterious, negating any potential long term benefits [182]. This underlines the importance of tailoring exercise interventions to the individual. Indeed, while there is increasing evidence for the anti-inflammatory effect of exercise across inflammatory disorders affecting the skeletal system, the cardiovascular system and the nervous system [181, 183], further work is needed to assess which type of exercise intervention—and which intensity, duration and frequency - has the greatest anti-inflammatory effect in depression.
Physical activity boosts dopamine transmission and neural reward circuitry
Given the evidence that exercise reduces inflammation [184], and the inverse association between inflammation and dopamine function [130], it is plausible that exercise might enhance dopamine transmission [185]. Indeed, animal studies show that exercise protects against the loss of dopamine neurons associated with inflammation [186], elicits dopamine release [187] and increases striatal activity [187]. In one study in Parkinson’s disease patients, a 30-minute session of aerobic exercise resulted in increased striatal dopamine release, more so in habitual exercisers, who also showed an increase in striatal activation to reward as well as lower apathy [18]. The causal effect of physical activity on dopamine functioning has been further explored in a 36-session intervention study, in which Parkinson’s disease patients in the active group showed increased striatal activation to reward and enhanced dopamine release in the caudate nucleus [19], relative to the control group. This is consistent with the hypothesis that exercise alters striatal response to reward, potentially via enhancing transmission in the mesolimbic dopaminergic pathway [19].
Counter-intuitively, some (but not all [188]) neuroimaging studies suggest that exercise results in an acute decreased striatal response to reward (i.e. in the immediate aftermath of activity), which might reflect an increase in tonic extracellular dopamine, inhibiting the magnitude of phasic dopamine release [189, 190]. A recent study also examined the effect of eight weeks of aerobic exercise on a reward-related electroencephalography (EEG) component termed the ‘reward positivity’, thought to correspond to ventral striatum-aMCC signalling. Reward positivity was not changed by the intervention, although its amplitude did predict who experienced an improvement in symptoms: individuals with larger pre-treatment reward positivity were more likely to respond to aerobic exercise, again implicating reward processing in the effect of exercise on depression [191]. The causal effect of exercise on tonic and phasic dopamine function and reward processing over time, and its association with interest-activity symptoms, merit further investigation.
Physical activity increases effort exertion and decreases anhedonia
Given its impact on both dopamine transmission and reward processing, it seems likely that exercise increases propensity to engage in effort, although few studies have directly tested this hypothesis. An exploratory analysis of one study showed that immediately after a single 20-minute running session, healthy individuals who had been running for more years exhibited increased willingness to exert effort, with the opposite pattern in those who had been running for fewer years [192], suggesting that fitness level interacts with the acute effect of physical exercise on effort sensitivity. The only study that has assessed neural activation during effort-based decision-making after a course of exercise (in this case, a three-month intervention, again in healthy participants) reported reduced effort sensitivity and effort-related aMCC activation; albeit there was no control group which limits interpretability [193]. Further investigation of the effects of exercise on effort-based decision-making, in randomised studies using appropriate control groups, is warranted.
In line with its effects on both effort sensitivity and depression, physical activity is negatively associated with anhedonia [194] and fatigue [195]. A recent study reported that anhedonia was reduced in people with depressive symptoms after an eight-week exercise intervention, although an improvement in general depressive symptoms was also observed, making it difficult to assess the specificity of this finding [196]. A causal relationship between physical activity and motivation was indicated in a 12-week martial arts intervention in children, which yielded large improvements specifically on observer ratings of motivation, perseverance, will and engagement [197, 198]. The beneficial effect of exercise on anhedonia has been further demonstrated in a three-month intervention study as an augmentation treatment for patients with treatment-resistant depression, finding improved anhedonia and other motivational symptoms [199]. Both anhedonia and motivational changes were correlated with change in depression severity, and changes in motivation preceded improvement in overall symptoms, further indicating a directional effect of exercise on anhedonia and mood [199] (Fig. 1B).
Physical activity improves cognitive control
According to EVC theory, cognitive control results from a decision-making process in which the potential rewards and costs of cognitive control are weighed up and the brain allocates resources accordingly [75, 77]. The CON [89, 106, 108] is thought to play a key role in this process, activated both when deciding to perform an effortful action [105] and during cognitive control tasks [104, 200]; importantly, CON connectivity is also disrupted in depression [201]. This lends credence to the notion that the CON has a central role in the motivational and cognitive symptoms of depression. As discussed above, exercise might reduce CON activity during effort-based decision-making (especially for higher levels of effort) and consequently reduce how challenging a given level of cognitive effort is perceived to be, driving an increased tendency to allocate resources to cognitive control tasks [105].
Consistent with this hypothesis, acute exercise alters PFC function and this varies depending on the level of depressive symptoms [202]. One study showed that exercise indeed reduced aMCC activation and increased dlPFC activation during cognitive control, and improved performance on a flanker task, following a six-month intervention in older adults [203]. However, interventions of similar durations in children yielded only marginal improvements in performance on flanker and antisaccade tasks [200, 204], and increased aMCC activity [204] and decreased dlPFC activity [200], raising the possibility of age-related differences in the impact of exercise. Indeed, age has been shown to moderate the association between fitness and cognitive performance, with the clearest positive relationships beyond young adulthood [205]. Interestingly, cross-sectional and longitudinal studies suggest that striatal and frontal dopamine transporters and D1 and D2 receptors reduce with age [206,207,208], which could be one reason that older adults are more sensitive to the psychological effects of exercise.
Behavioural studies also suggest that exercise improves cognitive control [35], though results are less consistent in depression [209]. A meta-analysis of the effect of exercise interventions in healthy older adults revealed robust effects, with moderate-to-large effects on cognitive control tasks (ES = 0.48) [25]. In depression, a recent meta-analysis found that exercise interventions had a beneficial effect on working memory, but nonsignificant effects on inhibition and cognitive flexibility and concluded that there are not yet enough high-quality studies to assess this relationship [209]. Given that it increases cognitive control, exercise might also improve performance on other cognitive processes. While two recent reviews reported that most studies indicate an improvement in cognitive outcomes more broadly after long-term exercise interventions, including in depression [35, 210], many studies in depression have used shorter exercise interventions. In one study, a four-week exercise intervention improved cognitive performance in depressed participants [211], although this was not replicated in another study using a three-week intervention [212]. Other methodological issues in this literature include low levels of attendance at exercise programmes, a lack of blinding and small sample sizes. Further, using wait-list controls could introduce bias due to uncontrolled variables in the control group such as habitual exercise [209].
In summary, preliminary results indicate a potentially beneficial influence of exercise on cognitive control in depression, although studies using randomised designs, appropriate control groups and suitably long intervention durations are required to confirm this. Whether such effects are related to motivational changes, as we hypothesise here, remains unknown. To test this, ideally both cognitive control and effort processing would be assessed throughout an intervention, allowing for time-lagged analyses which could help assess possible mediation of cognitive enhancement by motivational factors.
Practical applications of the physical activity—inflammation—dopamine—effort framework
Understanding the potential mechanisms underlying the antidepressant effects of exercise in depression—reducing inflammation, enhancing dopamine, and reducing effort sensitivity, consequently improving both interest-activity symptoms and objective cognitive impairment - could inform both nosology and the development of novel intervention strategies. It may provide initial indications of whether there are sub-groups of depressed individuals who are particularly likely to benefit from exercise (for example, individuals with chronic inflammation). This information could also lead to strategies to personalise activity prescription based on motivational factors; for example, psychological interventions such as behavioural activation therapy for individuals with pronounced motivational dysfunction who may struggle to exercise.
Further, individuals are likely to vary in the form of exercise that they most enjoy, and the types of skills they enjoy mastering, which is important to tailor to maximise the likelihood of long-term adherence. This could also pave the way for augmentative approaches, for example combining exercise with psychological interventions. Such knowledge could contribute to social prescribing—the use of non-medical interventions to tackle wider influences on health and support individuals to effectively manage their health [213]—increasingly a priority for mental healthcare. Information about mechanisms may also make primary care providers more likely to prescribe exercise and encourage policy to establish services.
Exercise is highly scalable, low cost, well suited to early intervention and has beneficial impacts on physical health co-morbidities. Explaining the mechanisms underlying the beneficial effect of exercise on mental health, and the links between them, may help persuade people that exercise is worthwhile for them. This could be important more broadly in addressing the alarmingly low levels of physical activity within the general population. Finally, in the longer term, our ability to develop new interventions for depression will benefit from understanding the mechanisms by which exercise improves symptoms, through back-translation.
Conclusion
We have summarised a wide range of research indicating that depression, especially anhedonia, is associated with raised levels of inflammatory cytokines and other markers of inflammation, disrupted dopamine transmission and reward processing and in particular lower propensity to exert both physical and cognitive effort. We propose that this may be an important pathway through which exercise exerts an antidepressant effect. The antidepressant effect of aerobic exercise has been convincingly demonstrated through randomised controlled trials, but its mechanism is not well-understood, in part because it likely involves a variety of biological and psychological mechanisms; alongside its effect on inflammation, dopamine and reward processing, exercise also increases neuroplasticity, reduces oxidative stress, alters neuroendocrine levels and improves self-esteem and self-efficacy. Substantial mechanistic research, mainly in animals but with some convergent findings in humans, demonstrates that exercise decreases systemic inflammation which boosts dopamine transmission and increases propensity to exert effort. Future research should continue to utilise randomised controlled trial designs for a rigorous assessment of the antidepressant effects of exercise, in larger samples than tested to date, whilst additionally measuring the effect of exercise on putative mechanistic variables—such as inflammation, dopamine transmission and reward processing—ideally both at baseline, follow-up and over the course of the intervention, to help assess causality. It would also be important to investigate the potential barriers to implementing exercise, particularly in depressed populations and strategies to encourage exercise use [214]. Understanding the mechanisms underlying the antidepressant effects of physical activity in depression could inform understanding of the mechanisms causing depression as well as the development of novel intervention strategies, in particular personalised intervention and social prescribing.
References
Friedrich MJ. Depression is the leading cause of disability around the world. JAMA. 2017;317:1517.
American Psychiatric Association. Multiaxial assessment.DSM-IV-TR: diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2002.
Trivedi MH, Rush AJ, Wisniewski SR, Nierenberg AA, Warden D, Ritz L, et al. Evaluation of outcomes with citalopram for depression using measurement-based care in STAR*D: implications for clinical practice. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:28–40.
Rush AJ, Trivedi MH, Wisniewski SR, Stewart JW, Nierenberg AA, Thase ME, et al. Bupropion-SR, sertraline, or venlafaxine-XR after failure of SSRIs for depression. N Engl J Med. 2006;354:1231–42.
Fried EI, Nesse RM. Depression is not a consistent syndrome: an investigation of unique symptom patterns in the STAR*D study. J Affect Disord. 2015;172:96–102.
Uher R, Perlis RH, Henigsberg N, Zobel A, Rietschel M, Mors O, et al. Depression symptom dimensions as predictors of antidepressant treatment outcome: replicable evidence for interest-activity symptoms. Psychol Med. 2012;42:967–80.
Halahakoon DC, Kieslich K, O’Driscoll C, Nair A, Lewis G, Roiser JP. Reward-processing behavior in depressed participants relative to healthy volunteers: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2020;77:1286–95.
Treadway MT, Bossaller NA, Shelton RC, Zald DH. Effort-based decision-making in major depressive disorder: a translational model of motivational anhedonia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012;121:553–8.
Yang X-H, Huang J, Zhu C-Y, Wang Y-F, Cheung EFC, Chan RCK, et al. Motivational deficits in effort-based decision making in individuals with subsyndromal depression, first-episode and remitted depression patients. Psychiatry Res. 2014;220:874–82.
Cléry-Melin M-L, Schmidt L, Lafargue G, Baup N, Fossati P, Pessiglione M. Why don’t you try harder? An investigation of effort production in major depression. PLOS ONE. 2011;6:e23178.
Costello H, Yamamori Y, Reeves S, Schrag A, Howard R, Roiser JP. Longitudinal decline in striatal dopamine transporter binding in Parkinson’s disease: associations with apathy and anhedonia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2023;94:863–70.
Peciña M, Sikora M, Avery ET, Heffernan J, Peciña S, Mickey BJ, et al. Striatal dopamine D2/3 receptor-mediated neurotransmission in major depression: implications for anhedonia, anxiety and treatment response. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017;27:977–86.
Salamone JD, Yohn SE, López-Cruz L, San Miguel N, Correa M. Activational and effort-related aspects of motivation: neural mechanisms and implications for psychopathology. Brain J Neurol. 2016;139:1325–47.
Treadway MT, Buckholtz JW, Cowan RL, Woodward ND, Li R, Ansari MS, et al. Dopaminergic mechanisms of individual differences in human effort-based decision-making. J Neurosci. 2012;32:6170–6.
Westbrook A, van den Bosch R, Määttä JI, Hofmans L, Papadopetraki D, Cools R, et al. Dopamine promotes cognitive effort by biasing the benefits versus costs of cognitive work. Science. 2020;367:1362–6.
Maron E, Eller T, Vasar V, Nutt DJ. Effects of bupropion augmentation in escitalopram-resistant patients with major depressive disorder: an open-label, naturalistic study. J Clin Psychiatry. 2009;70:1054–6.
Argyropoulos SV, Nutt DJ. Anhedonia revisited: is there a role for dopamine-targeting drugs for depression? J Psychopharmacol. 2013;27:869–77.
Sacheli MA, Murray DK, Vafai N, Cherkasova MV, Dinelle K, Shahinfard E, et al. Habitual exercisers versus sedentary subjects with Parkinson’s disease: multimodal PET and fMRI study. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc. 2018;33:1945–50.
Sacheli MA, Neva JL, Lakhani B, Murray DK, Vafai N, Shahinfard E, et al. Exercise increases caudate dopamine release and ventral striatal activation in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc. 2019;34:1891–1900.
Schuch FB, Vancampfort D, Firth J, Rosenbaum S, Ward PB, Silva ES, et al. Physical activity and incident depression: a meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Am J Psychiatry. 2018;175:631–48.
Morres ID, Hatzigeorgiadis A, Stathi A, Comoutos N, Arpin-Cribbie C, Krommidas C, et al. Aerobic exercise for adult patients with major depressive disorder in mental health services: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Depress Anxiety. 2019;36:39–53.
Bailey AP, Hetrick SE, Rosenbaum S, Purcell R, Parker AG. Treating depression with physical activity in adolescents and young adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Psychol Med. 2018;48:1068–83.
Krogh J, Nordentoft M, Sterne JAC, Lawlor DA. The effect of exercise in clinically depressed adults: systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Clin Psychiatry. 2011;72:529–38.
Shilyansky C, Williams LM, Gyurak A, Harris A, Usherwood T, Etkin A. Effect of antidepressant treatment on cognitive impairments associated with depression: a randomised longitudinal study. Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3:425–35.
Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci. 2003;14:125–30.
Oertel-Knöchel V, Mehler P, Thiel C, Steinbrecher K, Malchow B, Tesky V, et al. Effects of aerobic exercise on cognitive performance and individual psychopathology in depressive and schizophrenia patients. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2014;264:589–604.
Rock P, Roiser J, Riedel W, Blackwell A, Rock PL, Roiser JP, et al. Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2013;44:1–12.
Kandola A, Ashdown-Franks G, Hendrikse J, Sabiston CM, Stubbs B. Physical activity and depression: towards understanding the antidepressant mechanisms of physical activity. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;107:525–39.
Szuhany KL, Bugatti M, Otto MW. A meta-analytic review of the effects of exercise on brain-derived neurotrophic factor. J Psychiatr Res. 2015;60:56–64.
Gujral S, Aizenstein H, Reynolds CF, Butters MA, Grove G, Karp JF, et al. Exercise for depression: a feasibility trial exploring neural mechanisms. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2019;27:611–6.
Schuch FB, Vasconcelos-Moreno MP, Borowsky C, Zimmermann AB, Wollenhaupt-Aguiar B, Ferrari P, et al. The effects of exercise on oxidative stress (TBARS) and BDNF in severely depressed inpatients. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2014;264:605–13.
Alghadir AH, Gabr SA. Hormonal function responses to moderate aerobic exercise in older adults with depression. Clin Interv Aging. 2020;15:1271–83.
Beserra AHN, Kameda P, Deslandes AC, Schuch FB, Laks J, Moraes HSde. Can physical exercise modulate cortisol level in subjects with depression? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 2018;40:360–8.
Schuch FB, Deslandes AC, Stubbs B, Gosmann NP, Silva CTBda, Fleck MPdeA. Neurobiological effects of exercise on major depressive disorder: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;61:1–11.
Heinze K, Cumming J, Dosanjh A, Palin S, Poulton S, Bagshaw AP, et al. Neurobiological evidence of longer-term physical activity interventions on mental health outcomes and cognition in young people: a systematic review of randomised controlled trials. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;120:431–41.
Robert P, Onyike CU, Leentjens AFG, Dujardin K, Aalten P, Starkstein S, et al. Proposed diagnostic criteria for apathy in Alzheimer’s disease and other neuropsychiatric disorders. Eur Psychiatry. 2009;24:98–104.
Treadway MT, Zald DH. Reconsidering anhedonia in depression: lessons from translational neuroscience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35:537–55.
Zald DH, Treadway MT. Reward processing, neuroeconomics, and psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2017;13:471–95.
Rømer Thomsen K, Whybrow PC, Kringelbach ML. Reconceptualizing anhedonia: novel perspectives on balancing the pleasure networks in the human brain. Front Behav Neurosci. 2015;9:49.
Der-Avakian A, Markou A. The neurobiology of anhedonia and other reward-related deficits. Trends Neurosci. 2012;35:68–77.
Pizzagalli DA. Depression, stress, and anhedonia: toward a synthesis and integrated model. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2014;10:393–423.
Costello H, Husain M, Roiser J. Apathy and motivation: biological basis and drug treatment. 2023. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/m3vjy.
Vrieze E, Pizzagalli DA, Demyttenaere K, Hompes T, Sienaert P, de Boer P, et al. Reduced reward learning predicts outcome in major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;73:639–45.
Pizzagalli DA, Iosifescu D, Hallett LA, Ratner KG, Fava M. Reduced hedonic capacity in major depressive disorder: evidence from a probabilistic reward task. J Psychiatr Res. 2008;43:76–87.
Huys QJ, Pizzagalli DA, Bogdan R, Dayan P. Mapping anhedonia onto reinforcement learning: a behavioural meta-analysis. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord. 2013;3:12.
Pechtel P, Dutra SJ, Goetz EL, Pizzagalli DA. Blunted reward responsiveness in remitted depression. J Psychiatr Res. 2013;47:1864–9.
Kieslich K, Valton V, Roiser JP. Pleasure, reward value, prediction error and anhedonia. In: Pizzagalli DA, (ed). Anhedonia: preclinical, translational, and clinical integration. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2022. p. 281–304.
Husain M, Roiser JP. Neuroscience of apathy and anhedonia: a transdiagnostic approach. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2018;19:470–84.
Pizzagalli DA, Holmes AJ, Dillon DG, Goetz EL, Birk JL, Bogdan R, et al. Reduced caudate and nucleus accumbens response to rewards in unmedicated individuals with major depressive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166:702–10.
Knutson B, Bhanji JP, Cooney RE, Atlas LY, Gotlib IH. Neural responses to monetary incentives in major depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:686–92.
Gorka SM, Huggins AA, Fitzgerald DA, Nelson BD, Phan KL, Shankman SA. Neural response to reward anticipation in those with depression with and without panic disorder. J Affect Disord. 2014;164:50–56.
Stringaris A, Vidal-Ribas Belil P, Artiges E, Lemaitre H, Gollier-Briant F, Wolke S, et al. The brain’s response to reward anticipation and depression in adolescence: dimensionality, specificity, and longitudinal predictions in a community-based sample. Am J Psychiatry. 2015;172:1215–23.
Yadid G, Friedman A. Dynamics of the dopaminergic system as a key component to the understanding of depression. Prog Brain Res. 2008;172:265–86.
Dunlop BW, Nemeroff CB. The role of dopamine in the pathophysiology of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:327–37.
Haber SN, Knutson B. The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:4–26.
Salamone JD, Correa M. The mysterious motivational functions of mesolimbic dopamine. Neuron. 2012;76:470–85.
Cooper JA, Arulpragasam AR, Treadway MT. Anhedonia in depression: biological mechanisms and computational models. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2018;22:128–35.
Rzepa E, Fisk J, McCabe C. Blunted neural response to anticipation, effort and consummation of reward and aversion in adolescents with depression symptomatology. J Psychopharmacol. 2017;31:303–11.
Ubl B, Kuehner C, Kirsch P, Ruttorf M, Diener C, Flor H. Altered neural reward and loss processing and prediction error signalling in depression. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2015;10:1102–12.
Liu W, Chan RCK, Wang L, Huang J, Cheung EFC, Gong Q, et al. Deficits in sustaining reward responses in subsyndromal and syndromal major depression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2011;35:1045–52.
Chase HW, Frank MJ, Michael A, Bullmore ET, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Approach and avoidance learning in patients with major depression and healthy controls: relation to anhedonia. Psychol Med. 2010;40:433–40.
Rothkirch M, Tonn J, Köhler S, Sterzer P. Neural mechanisms of reinforcement learning in unmedicated patients with major depressive disorder. Brain J Neurol. 2017;140:1147–57.
Wardenaar KJ, Giltay EJ, van Veen T, Zitman FG, Penninx BWJH. Symptom dimensions as predictors of the two-year course of depressive and anxiety disorders. J Affect Disord. 2012;136:1198–203.
Spijker J, Bijl RV, de Graaf R, Nolen WA. Determinants of poor 1-year outcome of DSM-III-R major depression in the general population: results of the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (NEMESIS). Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2001;103:122–30.
McMakin DL, Olino TM, Porta G, Dietz LJ, Emslie G, Clarke G, et al. Anhedonia predicts poorer recovery among youth with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment-resistant depression. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51:404–11.
Nutt D, Demyttenaere K, Janka Z, Aarre T, Bourin M, Canonico PL, et al. The other face of depression, reduced positive affect: the role of catecholamines in causation and cure. J Psychopharmacol. 2007;21:461–71.
Padala PR, Padala KP, Monga V, Ramirez DA, Sullivan DH. Reversal of SSRI-associated apathy syndrome by discontinuation of therapy. Ann Pharmacother. 2012;46:e8.
Fava M, Ball S, Nelson JC, Sparks J, Konechnik T, Classi P, et al. Clinical relevance of fatigue as a residual symptom in major depressive disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2014;31:250–7.
Treadway MT, Buckholtz JW, Schwartzman AN, Lambert WE, Zald DH. Worth the ‘EEfRT’? The effort expenditure for rewards task as an objective measure of motivation and anhedonia. PloS One. 2009;4:e6598.
Salamone JD, Correa M, Farrar A, Mingote SM. Effort-related functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine and associated forebrain circuits. Psychopharmacology. 2007;191:461–82.
Bonnelle V, Veromann K-R, Burnett Heyes S, Lo Sterzo E, Manohar S, Husain M. Characterization of reward and effort mechanisms in apathy. J Physiol Paris. 2015;109:16–26.
Chong T-T-J, Bonnelle V, Husain M. Quantifying motivation with effort- based decision-making paradigms in health and disease. Prog Brain Res. 2016;229:71–100.
Chong TT-J, Bonnelle V, Manohar S, Veromann K-R, Muhammed K, Tofaris GK, et al. Dopamine enhances willingness to exert effort for reward in Parkinson’s disease. Cortex J Devoted Study Nerv Syst Behav. 2015;69:40–46.
Bonnelle V, Manohar S, Behrens T, Husain M. Individual differences in premotor brain systems underlie behavioral apathy. Cereb Cortex. 2015;26:bhv247.
Shenhav A, Musslick S, Lieder F, Kool W, Griffiths TL, Cohen JD, et al. Toward a rational and mechanistic account of mental effort. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2017;40:99–124.
Botvinick MM, Cohen JD. The computational and neural basis of cognitive control: charted territory and new frontiers. Cogn Sci. 2014;38:1249–85.
Grahek I, Shenhav A, Musslick S, Krebs RM, Koster EHW. Motivation and cognitive control in depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;102:371–81.
Marchetti I, Shumake J, Grahek I, Koster EHW. Temperamental factors in remitted depression: the role of effortful control and attentional mechanisms. J Affect Disord. 2018;235:499–505.
Rock PL, Roiser JP, Riedel WJ, Blackwell AD. Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2014;44:2029–40.
Disner SG, Beevers CG, Haigh EAP, Beck AT. Neural mechanisms of the cognitive model of depression. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:467–77.
Gotlib IH, Joormann J. Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010;6:285–312.
Snyder HR. Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: a meta-analysis and review. Psychol Bull. 2013;139:81–132.
Rayner G, Jackson G, Wilson S. Cognition-related brain networks underpin the symptoms of unipolar depression: Evidence from a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;61:53–65.
Laplane D, Levasseur M, Pillon B, Dubois B, Baulac M, Mazoyer B, et al. Obsessive-compulsive and other behavioural changes with bilateral basal ganglia lesions. A neuropsychological, magnetic resonance imaging and positron tomography study. Brain J Neurol. 1989;112:699–725.
Adam R, Leff A, Sinha N, Turner C, Bays P, Draganski B, et al. Dopamine reverses reward insensitivity in apathy following globus pallidus lesions. Cortex J Devoted Study Nerv Syst Behav. 2013;49:1292–303.
Manohar SG, Husain M. Human ventromedial prefrontal lesions alter incentivisation by reward. Cortex. 2016;76:104–20.
Kang SY, Kim JS. Anterior cerebral artery infarction: stroke mechanism and clinical-imaging study in 100 patients. Neurology. 2008;70:2386–93.
Le Heron C, Apps MaJ, Husain M. The anatomy of apathy: a neurocognitive framework for amotivated behaviour. Neuropsychologia. 2018;118:54–67.
Pessiglione M, Vinckier F, Bouret S, Daunizeau J, Le Bouc R. Why not try harder? Computational approach to motivation deficits in neuro-psychiatric diseases. Brain J Neurol. 2018;141:629–50.
Hauser TU, Eldar E, Dolan RJ. Separate mesocortical and mesolimbic pathways encode effort and reward learning signals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2017;114:E7395–E7404.
Skvortsova V, Palminteri S, Pessiglione M. Learning to minimize efforts versus maximizing rewards: computational principles and neural correlates. J Neurosci. 2014;34:15621–30.
Kroemer NB, Guevara A, Ciocanea Teodorescu I, Wuttig F, Kobiella A, Smolka MN. Balancing reward and work: anticipatory brain activation in NAcc and VTA predict effort differentially. NeuroImage. 2014;102:510–9.
Suzuki S, Lawlor VM, Cooper JA, Arulpragasam AR, Treadway MT. Distinct regions of the striatum underlying effort, movement initiation and effort discounting. Nat Hum Behav. 2021;5:378–88.
Parvizi J, Rangarajan V, Shirer WR, Desai N, Greicius MD. The will to persevere induced by electrical stimulation of the human cingulate gyrus. Neuron. 2013;80:1359–67.
Croxson PL, Walton ME, O’Reilly JX, Behrens TEJ, Rushworth MFS. Effort-based cost–benefit valuation and the human brain. J Neurosci. 2009;29:4531–41.
Kurniawan IT, Guitart-Masip M, Dayan P, Dolan RJ. Effort and valuation in the brain: the effects of anticipation and execution. J Neurosci. 2013;33:6160–9.
Chong TT-J, Apps M, Giehl K, Sillence A, Grima LL, Husain M. Neurocomputational mechanisms underlying subjective valuation of effort costs. PLoS Biol. 2017;15:e1002598.
Klein-Flügge MC, Kennerley SW, Friston K, Bestmann S. Neural signatures of value comparison in human cingulate cortex during decisions requiring an effort-reward trade-off. J Neurosci. 2016;36:10002–15.
Shenhav A, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD. The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron. 2013;79:217–40.
Pizzagalli DA. Frontocingulate dysfunction in depression: toward biomarkers of treatment response. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:183–206.
Davidson RJ, Pizzagalli D, Nitschke JB, Putnam K. Depression: perspectives from affective neuroscience. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:545–74.
Gotlib IH, Hamilton JP. Neuroimaging and depression: current status and unresolved issues. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2008;17:159–63.
Eickhoff SB, Etkin A, Huemer J, Carreon DM, Jiang Y, McTeague LM. Identification of common neural circuit disruptions in cognitive control across psychiatric disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174:676–85.
Nord CL, Halahakoon DC, Limbachya T, Charpentier C, Lally N, Walsh V, et al. Neural predictors of treatment response to brain stimulation and psychological therapy in depression: a double-blind randomized controlled trial. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019;44:1613–22.
Armbruster-Genç DJN, Valton V, Neil L, Vuong V, Freeman ZCL, Packer KC, et al. Altered reward and effort processing in children with maltreatment experience: a potential indicator of mental health vulnerability. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2022;47:1063–70.
Sadaghiani S, D’Esposito M. Functional characterization of the cingulo-opercular network in the maintenance of tonic alertness. Cereb Cortex. 2015;25:2763–73.
Han SW, Eaton HP, Marois R. Functional fractionation of the cingulo-opercular network: alerting insula and updating cingulate. Cereb Cortex. 2019;29:2624–38.
Seeley WW. The salience network: a neural system for perceiving and responding to homeostatic demands. J Neurosci. 2019;39:9878–82.
Hamid AA, Pettibone JR, Mabrouk OS, Hetrick VL, Schmidt R, Vander Weele CM, et al. Mesolimbic dopamine signals the value of work. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19:117–26.
Sarchiapone M, Carli V, Camardese G, Cuomo C, Di Giuda D, Calcagni M-L, et al. Dopamine transporter binding in depressed patients with anhedonia. Psychiatry Res. 2006;147:243–8.
Wardle MC, Treadway MT, Mayo LM, Zald DH, de Wit H. Amping up effort: effects of d-amphetamine on human effort-based decision-making. J Neurosci. 2011;31:16597–602.
Robles CF, Johnson AW. Disruptions in effort-based decision-making and consummatory behavior following antagonism of the dopamine D2 receptor. Behav Brain Res. 2017;320:431–9.
Cools R, D’Esposito M. Inverted-U–shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69:e113–e25.
Floresco SB, Tse MTL, Ghods-Sharifi S. Dopaminergic and glutamatergic regulation of effort- and delay-based decision making. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:1966–79.
Randall PA, Pardo M, Nunes EJ, Cruz LL, Vemuri VK, Makriyannis A, et al. Dopaminergic modulation of effort-related choice behavior as assessed by a progressive ratio chow feeding choice task: pharmacological studies and the role of individual differences. PLOS ONE. 2012;7:e47934.
Niv Y, Daw ND, Joel D, Dayan P. Tonic dopamine: opportunity costs and the control of response vigor. Psychopharmacology. 2007;191:507–20.
Bell JA, Kivimäki M, Bullmore ET, Steptoe A, MRC ImmunoPsychiatry Consortium, Carvalho LA. Repeated exposure to systemic inflammation and risk of new depressive symptoms among older adults. Transl Psychiatry. 2017;7:e1208.
Beurel E, Toups M, Nemeroff CB. The bidirectional relationship of depression and inflammation: double trouble. Neuron. 2020;107:234–56.
Carvalho LA, Bergink V, Sumaski L, Wijkhuijs J, Hoogendijk WJ, Birkenhager TK, et al. Inflammatory activation is associated with a reduced glucocorticoid receptor alpha/beta expression ratio in monocytes of inpatients with melancholic major depressive disorder. Transl Psychiatry. 2014;4:e344.
Kappelmann N, Lewis G, Dantzer R, Jones PB, Khandaker GM. Antidepressant activity of anti-cytokine treatment: a systematic review and meta-analysis of clinical trials of chronic inflammatory conditions. Mol Psychiatry. 2018;23:335–43.
Raison CL, Rutherford RE, Woolwine BJ, Shuo C, Schettler P, Drake DF, et al. A randomized controlled trial of the tumor necrosis factor antagonist infliximab for treatment-resistant depression: the role of baseline inflammatory biomarkers. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70:31–41.
Tyring S, Gottlieb A, Papp K, Gordon K, Leonardi C, Wang A, et al. Etanercept and clinical outcomes, fatigue, and depression in psoriasis: double-blind placebo-controlled randomised phase III trial. Lancet Lond Engl. 2006;367:29–35.
Nicoletti R, Adolfo Porro C, Brighetti G, Monti D, Pagnoni G, Guido M, et al. Long-term effects of vaccination on attentional performance. Vaccine. 2004;22:3877–81.
Allison DJ, Ditor DS. The common inflammatory etiology of depression and cognitive impairment: a therapeutic target. J Neuroinflammation. 2014;11:151.
Giollabhui NM, Swistun D, Murray S, Moriarity DP, Kautz MM, et al. Executive dysfunction in depression in adolescence: the role of inflammation and higher body mass. Psychol Med. 2020;50:683–91.
Husain MI, Chaudhry IB, Husain MO, Hodsoll J, Ansari MA, Naqvi HA, et al. Minocycline and celecoxib as adjunctive treatments for bipolar depression: a multicentre, factorial design randomised controlled trial. Lancet Psychiatry. 2020;7:515–27.
Bai S, Guo W, Feng Y, Deng H, Li G, Nie H, et al. Efficacy and safety of anti-inflammatory agents for the treatment of major depressive disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2020;91:21–32.
Osimo EF, Baxter LJ, Lewis G, Jones PB, Khandaker GM. Prevalence of low-grade inflammation in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis of CRP levels. Psychol Med. 2019;49:1958–70.
Miller AH, Pariante CM. Trial failures of anti-inflammatory drugs in depression. Lancet Psychiatry. 2020;7:837.
Bekhbat M, Li Z, Mehta ND, Treadway MT, Lucido MJ, Woolwine BJ, et al. Functional connectivity in reward circuitry and symptoms of anhedonia as therapeutic targets in depression with high inflammation: evidence from a dopamine challenge study. Mol Psychiatry. 2022;27:4113–21.
Felger JC, Hernandez CR, Miller AH. Levodopa reverses cytokine-induced reductions in striatal dopamine release. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2015;18:pyu084.
Capuron L, Pagnoni G, Drake DF, Woolwine BJ, Spivey JR, Crowe RJ, et al. Dopaminergic mechanisms of reduced basal ganglia responses to hedonic reward during interferon alfa administration. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69:1044–53.
Felger JC, Treadway MT. Inflammation effects on motivation and motor activity: role of dopamine. Neuropsychopharmacol Off Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017;42:216–41.
Eisenberger NI, Berkman ET, Inagaki TK, Rameson LT, Mashal NM, Irwin MR. Inflammation-induced anhedonia: endotoxin reduces ventral striatum responses to reward. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68:748–54.
Vichaya EG, Hunt SC, Dantzer R. Lipopolysaccharide reduces incentive motivation while boosting preference for high reward in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39:2884–90.
Nunes EJ, Randall PA, Estrada A, Epling B, Hart EE, Lee CA, et al. Effort-related motivational effects of the pro-inflammatory cytokine interleukin 1-beta: studies with the concurrent fixed ratio 5/ chow feeding choice task. Psychopharmacology. 2014;231:727–36.
Yohn SE, Arif Y, Haley A, Tripodi G, Baqi Y, Müller CE, et al. Effort-related motivational effects of the pro-inflammatory cytokine interleukin-6: pharmacological and neurochemical characterization. Psychopharmacology. 2016;233:3575–86.
Lasselin J, Treadway MT, Lacourt TE, Soop A, Olsson MJ, Karshikoff B, et al. Lipopolysaccharide alters motivated behavior in a monetary reward task: a randomized trial. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;42:801–10.
Draper A, Koch RM, van der Meer JW, AJ Apps M, Pickkers P, Husain M, et al. Effort but not reward sensitivity is altered by acute sickness induced by experimental endotoxemia in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2018;43:1107–18.
Voss MW, Vivar C, Kramer AF, van Praag H. Bridging animal and human models of exercise-induced brain plasticity. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17:525–44.
Belvederi Murri M, Pariante C, Mondelli V, Masotti M, Atti AR, Mellacqua Z, et al. HPA axis and aging in depression: systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;41:46–62.
Knapen J, Van de Vliet P, Van Coppenolle H, David A, Peuskens J, Pieters G, et al. Comparison of changes in physical self-concept, global self-esteem, depression and anxiety following two different psychomotor therapy programs in nonpsychotic psychiatric inpatients. Psychother Psychosom. 2005;74:353–61.
Haller N, Lorenz S, Pfirrmann D, Koch C, Lieb K, Dettweiler U, et al. Individualized web-based exercise for the treatment of depression: randomized controlled trial. JMIR Ment Health. 2018;5:e10698.
Pickett K, Yardley L, Kendrick T. Physical activity and depression: a multiple mediation analysis. Ment Health Phys Act. 2012;5:125–34.
Wipfli B, Landers D, Nagoshi C, Ringenbach S. An examination of serotonin and psychological variables in the relationship between exercise and mental health. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2011;21:474–81.
White K, Kendrick T, Yardley L. Change in self-esteem, self-efficacy and the mood dimensions of depression as potential mediators of the physical activity and depression relationship: Exploring the temporal relation of change. Ment Health Phys Act. 2009;2:44–52.
Pearce M, Garcia L, Abbas A, Strain T, Schuch FB, Golubic R, et al. Association between physical activity and risk of depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2022;79:550–9.
Dishman RK, McDowell CP, Herring MP. Customary physical activity and odds of depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis of 111 prospective cohort studies. Br J Sports Med. 2021;55:926–34.
Singh B, Olds T, Curtis R, Dumuid D, Virgara R, Watson A, et al. Effectiveness of physical activity interventions for improving depression, anxiety and distress: an overview of systematic reviews. Br J Sports Med. 2023;57:1203–9.
Heissel A, Heinen D, Brokmeier LL, Skarabis N, Kangas M, Vancampfort D, et al. Exercise as medicine for depressive symptoms? A systematic review and meta-analysis with meta-regression. Br J Sports Med. 2023;57:1049–57.
Recchia F, Bernal JDK, Fong DY, Wong SHS, Chung P-K, Chan DKC, et al. Physical activity interventions to alleviate depressive symptoms in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2023;177:132–40.
Lee J, Gierc M, Vila-Rodriguez F, Puterman E, Faulkner G. Efficacy of exercise combined with standard treatment for depression compared to standard treatment alone: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Affect Disord. 2021;295:1494–511.
Stothart CR, Simons DJ, Boot WR, Kramer AF. Is the effect of aerobic exercise on cognition a placebo effect? PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e109557.
Plessen CY, Karyotaki E, Miguel C, Ciharova M, Cuijpers P. Exploring the efficacy of psychotherapies for depression: a multiverse meta-analysis. BMJ Ment Health. 2023;26. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjment-2022-300626.
Edwards MK, Loprinzi PD. Effects of a sedentary behavior-inducing randomized controlled intervention on depression and mood profile in active young adults. Mayo Clin Proc. 2016;91:984–98.
Endrighi R, Steptoe A, Hamer M. The effect of experimentally induced sedentariness on mood and psychobiological responses to mental stress. Br J Psychiatry J Ment Sci. 2016;208:245–51.
Ross RE, VanDerwerker CJ, Saladin ME, Gregory CM. The role of exercise in the treatment of depression: biological underpinnings and clinical outcomes. Mol Psychiatry. 2023;28:298–328.
Cooney GM, Dwan K, Greig CA, Lawlor DA, Rimer J, Waugh FR et al. Exercise for depression. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD004366.pub6.
Kvam S, Kleppe CL, Nordhus IH, Hovland A. Exercise as a treatment for depression: a meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2016;202:67–86.
Schuch FB, Vancampfort D, Richards J, Rosenbaum S, Ward PB, Stubbs B. Exercise as a treatment for depression: a meta-analysis adjusting for publication bias. J Psychiatr Res. 2016;77:42–51.
Busch AM, Ciccolo JT, Puspitasari AJ, Nosrat S, Whitworth JW, Stults-Kolehmainen M. Preferences for exercise as a treatment for depression. Ment Health Phys Act. 2016;10:68–72.
Carpiniello B, Primavera D, Pilu A, Vaccargiu N, Pinna F. Physical activity and mental disorders: a case–control study on attitudes, preferences and perceived barriers in Italy. J Ment Health. 2013;22:492–500.
Fraser SJ, Chapman JJ, Brown WJ, Whiteford HA, Burton NW. Physical activity attitudes and preferences among inpatient adults with mental illness. Int J Ment Health Nurs. 2015;24:413–20.
Firth J, Rosenbaum S, Stubbs B, Gorczynski P, Yung AR, Vancampfort D. Motivating factors and barriers towards exercise in severe mental illness: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2016;46:2869–81.
Padin AC, Wilson SJ, Bailey BE, Malarkey WB, Lustberg MB, Farrar WB, et al. Physical activity after breast cancer surgery: does depression make exercise feel more effortful than it actually is? Int J Behav Med. 2019;26:237–46.
Hsu F-C, Kritchevsky SB, Liu Y, Kanaya A, Newman AB, Perry SE, et al. Association between inflammatory components and physical function in the health, aging, and body composition study: a principal component analysis approach. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2009;64:581–9.
Severinsen MCK, Pedersen BK. Muscle-organ crosstalk: the emerging roles of myokines. Endocr Rev. 2020;41:bnaa016.
Kistner TM, Pedersen BK, Lieberman DE. Interleukin 6 as an energy allocator in muscle tissue. Nat Metab. 2022;4:170–9.
Chow LS, Gerszten RE, Taylor JM, Pedersen BK, van Praag H, Trappe S, et al. Exerkines in health, resilience and disease. Nat Rev Endocrinol. 2022;18:273–89.
Saltiel AR, Olefsky JM. Inflammatory mechanisms linking obesity and metabolic disease. J Clin Invest. 2017;127:1–4.
Olefsky JM, Glass CK. Macrophages, inflammation, and insulin resistance. Annu Rev Physiol. 2010;72:219–46.
Samaras K, Botelho NK, Chisholm DJ, Lord RV. Subcutaneous and visceral adipose tissue gene expression of serum adipokines that predict type 2 diabetes. Obes Silver Spring Md. 2010;18:884–9.
Verheggen RJHM, Maessen MFH, Green DJ, Hermus ARMM, Hopman MTE, Thijssen DHT. A systematic review and meta-analysis on the effects of exercise training versus hypocaloric diet: distinct effects on body weight and visceral adipose tissue. Obes Rev. 2016;17:664–90.
Beavers KM, Brinkley TE, Nicklas BJ. Effect of exercise training on chronic inflammation. Clin Chim Acta Int J Clin Chem. 2010;411:785–93.
Chang E, Varghese M, Singer K. Gender and sex differences in adipose tissue. Curr Diab Rep. 2018;18:69.
Evans J, Salamonsen LA. Inflammation, leukocytes and menstruation. Rev Endocr Metab Disord. 2012;13:277–88.
Sacher J, Okon-Singer H, Villringer A. Evidence from neuroimaging for the role of the menstrual cycle in the interplay of emotion and cognition. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:374.
McNulty KL, Elliott-Sale KJ, Dolan E, Swinton PA, Ansdell P, Goodall S, et al. The effects of menstrual cycle phase on exercise performance in eumenorrheic women: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 2020;50:1813–27.
Falconer CL, Cooper AR, Walhin JP, Thompson D, Page AS, Peters TJ, et al. Sedentary time and markers of inflammation in people with newly diagnosed type 2 diabetes. Nutr Metab Cardiovasc Dis. 2014;24:956–62.
Healy GN, Matthews CE, Dunstan DW, Winkler EAH, Owen N. Sedentary time and cardio-metabolic biomarkers in US adults: NHANES 2003-06. Eur Heart J. 2011;32:590–7.
Nimmo MA, Leggate M, Viana JL, King JA. The effect of physical activity on mediators of inflammation. Diabetes Obes Metab. 2013;15:51–60.
Paolucci EM, Loukov D, Bowdish DME, Heisz JJ. Exercise reduces depression and inflammation but intensity matters. Biol Psychol. 2018;133:79–84.
Metsios GS, Moe RH, Kitas GD. Exercise and inflammation. Best Pract Res Clin Rheumatol. 2020;34:101504.
Kohut ML, McCann DA, Russell DW, Konopka DN, Cunnick JE, Franke WD, et al. Aerobic exercise, but not flexibility/resistance exercise, reduces serum IL-18, CRP, and IL-6 independent of beta-blockers, BMI, and psychosocial factors in older adults. Brain Behav Immun. 2006;20:201–9.
Marques A, Marconcin P, Werneck A, Ferrari G, Gouveia E, Kliegel M, et al. Bidirectional association between physical activity and dopamine across adulthood-a systematic review. Brain Sci. 2021. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11070829.
Wu S-Y, Wang T-F, Yu L, Jen CJ, Chuang J-I, Wu F-S, et al. Running exercise protects the substantia nigra dopaminergic neurons against inflammation-induced degeneration via the activation of BDNF signaling pathway. Brain Behav Immun. 2011;25:135–46.
Hattori S, Naoi M, Nishino H. Striatal dopamine turnover during treadmill running in the rat: relation to the speed of running. Brain Res Bull. 1994;35:41–49.
Saanijoki T, Nummenmaa L, Tuulari JJ, Tuominen L, Arponen E, Kalliokoski KK, et al. Aerobic exercise modulates anticipatory reward processing via the μ-opioid receptor system. Hum Brain Mapp. 2018;39:3972–83.
Bothe N, Zschucke E, Dimeo F, Heinz A, Wüstenberg T, Ströhle A. Acute exercise influences reward processing in highly trained and untrained men. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2013;45:583–91.
Crabtree DR, Chambers ES, Hardwick RM, Blannin AK. The effects of high-intensity exercise on neural responses to images of food. Am J Clin Nutr. 2014;99:258–67.
Brush CJ, Hajcak G, Bocchine AJ, Ude AA, Muniz KM, Foti D, et al. A randomized trial of aerobic exercise for major depression: examining neural indicators of reward and cognitive control as predictors and treatment targets. Psychol Med. 2022;52:893–903.
Wardle MC, Lopez-Gamundi P, LaVoy EC. Effects of an acute bout of physical exercise on reward functioning in healthy adults. Physiol Behav. 2018;194:552–9.
Bernacer J, Martinez-Valbuena I, Martinez M, Pujol N, Luis EO, Ramirez-Castillo D, et al. An amygdala-cingulate network underpins changes in effort-based decision making after a fitness program. NeuroImage. 2019;203:116181.
Leventhal AM. Relations between anhedonia and physical activity. Am J Health Behav. 2012;36:860–72.
Soini E, Rosenström T, Määttänen I, Jokela M. Physical activity and specific symptoms of depression: a pooled analysis of six cohort studies. J Affect Disord. 2024;348:44–53.
Sun C-W, Wang Y-J, Fang Y-Q, He Y-Q, Wang X, So BCL, et al. The effect of physical activity on anhedonia in individuals with depressive symptoms. PsyCh J. 2022;11:214–26.
Lakes K. The response to challenge scale (RCS): the development and construct validity of an observer-rated measure of children’s self-regulation. Int J Educ Psychol Assess. 2012;10:83–96.
Lakes KD, Hoyt WT. Promoting self-regulation through school-based martial arts training. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2004;25:283–302.
Toups M, Carmody T, Greer T, Rethorst C, Grannemann B, Trivedi MH. Exercise is an effective treatment for positive valence symptoms in major depression. J Affect Disord. 2017;209:188–94.
Chaddock-Heyman L, Erickson K, Voss M, Knecht A, Pontifex M, Castelli D, et al. The effects of physical activity on functional MRI activation associated with cognitive control in children: a randomized controlled intervention. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00072. Accessed 8 Jan 2023.
Wu X, Lin P, Yang J, Song H, Yang R, Yang J. Dysfunction of the cingulo-opercular network in first-episode medication-naive patients with major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord. 2016;200:275–83.
Crum J, Ronca F, Herbert G, Funk S, Carmona E, Hakim U, et al. Decreased exercise-induced changes in prefrontal cortex hemodynamics are associated with depressive symptoms. Front Neuroergonomics. 2022;3. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnrgo.2022.806485. Accessed 29 Mar 2023.
Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Scalf P, McAuley E, Cohen NJ, et al. Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101:3316–21.
Krafft CE, Schwarz NF, Chi L, Weinberger AL, Schaeffer DJ, Pierce JE, et al. An 8-month randomized controlled exercise trial alters brain activation during cognitive tasks in overweight children. Obesity. 2014;22:232–42.
Etnier JL, Nowell PM, Landers DM, Sibley BA. A meta-regression to examine the relationship between aerobic fitness and cognitive performance. Brain Res Rev. 2006;52:119–30.
Karrer TM, Josef AK, Mata R, Morris ED, Samanez-Larkin GR. Reduced dopamine receptors and transporters but not synthesis capacity in normal aging adults: a meta-analysis. Neurobiol Aging. 2017;57:36–46.
Nordin K, Gorbach T, Pedersen R, Panes Lundmark V, Johansson J, Andersson M, et al. DyNAMiC: a prospective longitudinal study of dopamine and brain connectomes: A new window into cognitive aging. J Neurosci Res. 2022;100:1296–320.
Karalija N, Johansson J, Papenberg G, Wåhlin A, Salami A, Köhncke Y, et al. Longitudinal dopamine D2 receptor changes and cerebrovascular health in aging. Neurology. 2022;99:e1278–e89.
Contreras-Osorio F, Ramirez-Campillo R, Cerda-Vega E, Campos-Jara R, Martínez-Salazar C, Reigal RE, et al. Effects of physical exercise on executive function in adults with depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19:15270.
Dotson VM, Gradone AM, Bogoian HR, Minto LR, Taiwo Z, Salling ZN. Be fit, be sharp, be well: the case for exercise as a treatment for cognitive impairment in late-life depression. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2021;27:776–89.
Buschert V, Prochazka D, Bartl H, Diemer J, Malchow B, Zwanzger P, et al. Effects of physical activity on cognitive performance: a controlled clinical study in depressive patients. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2019;269:555–63.
Brüchle W, Schwarzer C, Berns C, Scho S, Schneefeld J, Koester D, et al. Physical activity reduces clinical symptoms and restores neuroplasticity in major depression. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12:660642.
Drinkwater C, Wildman J, Moffatt S. Social prescribing. BMJ. 2019;364:l1285.
Glowacki K, Duncan MJ, Gainforth H, Faulkner G. Barriers and facilitators to physical activity and exercise among adults with depression: a scoping review. Ment Health Phys Act. 2017;13:108–19.
Acknowledgements
EJH is funded by Rosetrees Trust (grant PGL21/10005). GL and MH acknowledge the support of UCLH Biomedical Research Centre.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
EJH conducted the literature review, synthesised the findings, and drafted the manuscript. ASD contributed to the drafting of the manuscript. GL and MH contributed to the conceptualisation of the review and provided critical feedback on the manuscript. JPR conceptualised the review topic, drafted an outline for the paper, and provided critical feedback on the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
JPR has held a PhD studentship co-funded by Cambridge Cognition Ltd and performed consultancy work for GH Research Ltd and GE Ltd. The authors declare no other competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Hird, E.J., Slanina-Davies, A., Lewis, G. et al. From movement to motivation: a proposed framework to understand the antidepressant effect of exercise. Transl Psychiatry 14, 273 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-024-02922-y
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-024-02922-y