Abstract
Stimulants, such as methylphenidate (MPH), are effective in treating attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), but there is individual variability in response, especially in adults. To improve outcomes, we need to understand the factors associated with adult treatment response. This longitudinal study investigated whether pre-treatment anatomy of the fronto-striatal and fronto-parietal attentional networks was associated with MPH treatment response. 60 adults with ADHD underwent diffusion brain imaging before starting MPH treatment, and response was measured at two months. We tested the association between brain anatomy and treatment response by using regression-based approaches; and compared the identified anatomical characteristics with those of 20 matched neurotypical controls in secondary analyses. Finally, we explored whether combining anatomical with clinical and neuropsychological data through machine learning provided a more comprehensive profile of factors associated with treatment response. At a group level, a smaller left dorsal superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF I), a tract responsible for the voluntary control of attention, was associated with a significantly lower probability of being responders to two-month MPH-treatment. The association between the volume of the left SLF I and treatment response was driven by improvement on both inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive symptoms. Only non-responders significantly differed from controls in this tract metric. Finally, our machine learning approach identified clinico-neuropsychological factors associated with treatment response, such as higher cognitive performance and symptom severity at baseline. These novel findings add to our understanding of the pathophysiological mechanisms underlying response to MPH, pointing to the dorsal attentive network as playing a key role.
Introduction
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental condition characterized by inattentive and/or hyperactive-impulsive symptoms, which is commonly diagnosed in childhood but persists in adulthood in 40–50% of cases [1]. It therefore remains relatively common in adulthood—with the population prevalence in adults being ~4% [2]. Stimulants, such as methylphenidate (MPH), represent the first line treatment [3] and act by modulating dopamine and norepinephrine transmission in striato-cortical regions [4]. This in turn optimizes regional brain activation and connectivity supporting cognitive functions such as attention and inhibition [5, 6]. MPH has been proven to be effective in reducing ADHD core symptoms, but randomized controlled trials have reported that more than one-third of adults with ADHD fail to respond [7]. Recent meta-analytic evidence also confirmed lower response rates in adults than in children [8], which may be linked to an age-dependent decline in brain plasticity [9, 10]. This is of concern, as untreated ADHD in adults increases the risks of academic and occupational failure, substance abuse, and criminal behavior [11, 12]. Yet, despite these possible outcomes, relatively little is known about the biological mechanisms underlying variation in adult treatment response. Further, our understanding of the neurobiology of ADHD is not yet sufficient to guide prescribing and, in clinical practice, the selection of the most appropriate medication is made on a trial-and-error basis. This means that individuals often need to undergo multiple treatments before achieving a clinical response, an approach that delays recovery; exposes people to an increased risk of side effects; and ultimately is not cost-effective. There is therefore a pressing need to better understand what the factors associated with treatment response are; and where possible at an individual level [13]. This may be particularly relevant to ADHD, as it is a neurodevelopmental condition characterized by substantial clinical and biological heterogeneity and, often, by life-long impairment.
Factors associated with treatment response in ADHD potentially include clinical, neuropsychological, and neurobiological characteristics [14, 15]. Clinical studies have reported that MPH dose, treatment adherence, and psychiatric comorbidity influence treatment outcomes in ADHD; whereas gender and age do not [14]. Some studies have suggested that a higher intelligence quotient (IQ) may be associated with better outcomes, but this was not confirmed by other reports [14, 16]. Nonetheless, some prior studies reported that better performance in specific cognitive domains (e.g. sustained attention as measured by the continuous performance task (CPT)), was associated with improved treatment response [16, 17]. Research into neurobiological factors associated with treatment response has produced inconsistent findings. For instance, some genetic studies reported an association between variants of the dopamine transporter gene (DAT1) and treatment response, but others did not [18]. Nuclear medicine approaches suggested that components of the dopaminergic system, such as DAT availability, may be related to treatment response in adults with ADHD, but this needs further replication in larger samples and may have limited applicability in the ‘real world’ due to the use of ionizing radiation [19, 20]. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies in children with ADHD that compared ‘responders’ and ‘non-responders’ to treatment reported differences in the morphometry and functional connectivity of fronto-striatal, parietal, and cingulo-opercular regions [21,22,23]. These prior MRI studies were valuable first steps. However, they were only of children and did not investigate the utility of measures of brain anatomical connections as potential factors associated with treatment response in adult ADHD. This is perhaps especially important in conditions—such as ADHD—that are associated with ‘dysconnectivity’ in specific brain circuits.
Anatomical brain connections can be studied using diffusion-weighted imaging, which measures water diffusivity and quantifies the microstructural properties of white matter fibers. Prior diffusion imaging studies in ADHD mainly focused on fronto-striatal networks, in line with the dominant neuropathological hypothesis of this condition. In addition, they provided evidence that other networks may be important, e.g. the fronto-parietal network [24]. The fronto-striatal network (including the fronto-striatal tract and anterior thalamic projections) mediates executive functions, whilst the fronto-parietal network supports distinct aspects of attention and motor control through the three branches of the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF I, II, III) [25, 26]. However, the potential independent contribution of the SLF branches to ADHD symptoms—and treatment response—has not been clarified.
Therefore, in this study we used advanced diffusion tractography, based on an algorithm known as spherical deconvolution (SD), to dissect the two main divisions of the fronto-striatal network and the three branches of the SLF. We then tested whether tract metrics were associated with treatment response in adults with ADHD. Finally, as ADHD is a highly heterogeneous condition that is associated with alterations occurring at multiple biological levels, we used a multimodal approach based on machine learning to combine our strongest tract metric with clinical and neuropsychological characteristics and identify a more comprehensive profile of factors associated with treatment response [27, 28].
Methods
Sample and research protocol
This study is part of a larger trial employing a single-blind placebo-controlled cross-over design, followed by a longitudinal open-label phase. The trial was registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT 03709940) and conducted at the Maudsley Hospital (London, UK). Please see Supplementary Methods for details on the protocol, power calculation, and inclusion criteria (p. 2). In this study, we specifically tested whether pre-treatment brain anatomy was associated with treatment response in adult ADHD. Sixty male adults with ADHD completed clinical and behavioral measures under placebo (baseline) and under an acute dose of MPH (acute MPH); and underwent diffusion imaging scanning before starting treatment with the same long-acting formulation of MPH (Concerta XL, titrated up to 54 mg). Treatment response was measured at two months (follow-up) (see below). We mainly recruited ADHD medication-naïve participants, and none of the few previously treated had received pharmacological treatment for at least a year prior to this study. At baseline, intelligent quotient (IQ) was measured with the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) [29]; and handedness with a modified version of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (EHI) [30]. Clinical symptoms were measured using the Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV (BAARS-IV) [31]. Behavioral tests included the Quantitative behavior (Qb) test (https://www.qbtech.com), a computer-based test used to measure core ADHD symptoms through a CPT and infrared monitoring of participants’ movements, which was granted approval from the Food and Drug Administration to aid treatment evaluation in individuals with ADHD [32]. The Qb test measures several parameters that are then combined into three Qb scores (Qb activity, Qb impulsivity, and Qb inattention), which represent summary measures of the three cardinal symptoms of ADHD (Supplementary methods, p. 3). ADHD participants also completed urine drug screenings to ascertain abstinence from illicit drugs during the scans, and an MPH assay at follow-up to ascertain treatment adherence. According to previously published criteria, the first definition of treatment response was based on a symptomatic improvement of at least 30% at two months, as measured by the BAARS-IV (total score) [31]. This categorization, although commonly used in clinical practice and in clinical trials, is based on an arbitrary cut-off that might not reflect a biological distinction among participants or apply to a specific sample. For this reason, we also employed an experimental approach based on a data-driven analysis, known as multivariate k-mean clustering [33]. This is a common clustering algorithm for multivariate data, which defines whether individuals in the population belong to different groups according to their multiple characteristics. The attribution of individuals to a group is made so that the squared error between the empirical mean of the cluster and the observations in the cluster is minimized [33]. The segregation of our sample into responders and non-responders was based on two clinical variables (reduction of the number of either inattentive or hyperactive/impulsive symptoms from baseline) and three behavioral variables (reduction of the Qb scores for activity, impulsivity, or inattention from baseline). One advantage of this approach is the identification of two more homogeneous subgroups in terms of clinical and behavioral improvement under treatment, which may better reflect, and thus allow us to understand, underlying biological differences among groups, in line with previous reports [34]. Characteristics of the two groups are displayed in Tables S3a-b. Finally, baseline scans were also obtained from 20 neurotypical controls matched for sex, age, and IQ. All participants gave written informed consent before taking part in this research. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. Ethical approval REC number: 12/LO/0630.
Diffusion MRI: data acquisition and analysis
Scan parameters, pre-processing, and SD tractography are described in Supplementary Methods (p. 3). In brief, preprocessing included correction for susceptibility-induced off-resonance field, motion, and eddy current distortions. We dissected the bilateral fronto-striatal tract, anterior thalamic projection, and three branches of the SLF, as previously described [25, 35]. For each tract we extracted a metric indicative of the size of the tract, i.e. the volume, and a metric reflecting its microstructural organization, i.e. the hindrance modulated orientational anisotropy (HMOA) [36]. We selected this metric, as it represents a compact measure of fiber diffusion properties in SD tractography as FA does in conventional diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) but can be more reliably used in regions with complex fiber organization, such as those crossed by the SLF [36]. Finally, we used volume to calculate the Lateralization Indices (LIs) for each pair of the SLF branches, according to the formula: (Right volume−Left volume)/(Right volume + Left volume). Positive values indicate a rightward asymmetry, whereas negative values reflect a leftward asymmetry. We selected this metric because there is evidence that the pattern of lateralization of the SLF branches is associated with attention performance in neurotypical adults [25]. In line with the Human Brain Mapping Committee on Best Practice in Data Analysis and Sharing (COBIDAS) guidelines on data sharing (https://www.humanbrainmapping.org/files/2016/COBIDASreport.pdf), de-identified raw diffusion imaging data will be made publicly available at http://www.bcblab.com upon publication.
Statistical analysis
Categorical approach
We used SPSS (v26, IBM) to confirm the normality of tract metrics and conduct the statistical analyses. Taking a categorical approach, the association between tract metrics and treatment response was tested using logistic regressions, in which the independent variable was one tract metric (volume, LI, and HMOA for the ten selected tracts) and the dependent variable was treatment response (as classified by either BAARS-IV score or k-mean clustering). As a descriptive measure of the association with treatment response, we calculated the area under the curve (AUC) from the corresponding receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves. We also performed 10-fold cross-validation to estimate the strength of the identified factors associated with treatment response in new data. Family-wise error correction for multiple comparisons (total number of tracts) was performed with a permutation test [37]. We chose this method because we observed relevant correlations between the tracts of interest (Table S7). Clinico-demographic characteristics that could be potential confounders (age, MPH dose, weight, baseline severity, years of previous treatment, and handedness) were afterwards included as covariates.
To better understand the association between tract metrics found significant by logistic regression and treatment response, we conducted these two secondary analyses.
Comparison with controls
Independent-sample t-tests (two-sided) were performed to determine whether responders and non-responders differed from controls in the tract metrics revealed as significant by logistic regression.
Dimensional approach
We used linear regressions to examine whether the association between tract metrics and treatment response was driven by improvement on either inattentive and/or hyperactive/impulsive symptoms (i.e., reduction of BAARS-IV Inattention or Hyperactivity/Impulsivity scores at follow-up as compared to baseline).
Machine learning
Finally, we explored whether combining our strongest imaging metric with clinical and neuropsychological data through machine learning provided a more comprehensive profile of factors associated with treatment response (classification based on k-mean clustering). We included clinico-demographic and neuropsychological variables, as previously suggested [14, 15]. The former included age, ADHD presentation, baseline severity, medication-naïve status, MPH dose at follow-up, treatment adherence, and use of illicit drugs. Neuropsychological variables included handedness, total IQ, and measures of inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity as extracted from the CPT during the Qb test, both under placebo and under an acute dose of MPH. Please see Supplementary Methods (p. 3) for the complete list of variables extracted. We created different models using Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator (LASSO) regressions. These are conceptually very similar to standard multiple logistic regressions, except that they can accept more independent variables, even if collinear, because they automatically select a limited set of them. The lasso regression has a regularization parameter to select more or fewer independent variables. To find the optimal regularization parameter, we conducted 10-fold cross-validation and chose the parameter with the minimum cross-validation error. Due to our relatively small sample size, we used the whole sample to both fit the lasso regression and assess its accuracy. Potential limitations of this approach are presented in the discussion. Before conducting the lasso regression, we used multiple imputations to estimate the missing values [38], creating several imputed datasets. Specifically, we imputed 20 datasets per run [39], and we ran the process 10 times, so we obtained 200 models. Among these models, those including a higher number of variables may achieve a higher balanced accuracy (BAC) but are also more likely to overfit. Hence, to limit overfitting while achieving optimal accuracy, we selected the model with the least number of variables that produced a BAC above 80%, as previously recommended [40].
Results
Sample
60 male adults with ADHD completed the study (see Fig. S1 for Flow Diagram). They were mostly White British (71%), had a mean (±SD) age of 28 (±7) years and a full-scale IQ of 109 (±12); the majority was right-handed (78%) and medication-naïve (77%). Data on individuals previously exposed to medication is reported in Table S2. Controls were matched for sex, age, and IQ, and were mostly right-handed (90%). Concerta XL was titrated up to 54 mg in most participants, but the dose had to be reduced to 36 mg in 24% of cases and to 18 mg in 7% of cases due to side effects. Only two participants increased the dose. We considered dose at follow-up among the potential confounding factors in our statistical analysis (see the “Methods” section). At follow-up, 42 participants were identified as responders and 18 as non-responders based on their BAARS-IV score. K-mean clustering identified a group with high and a group with low average improvement on BAARS-IV and Qb scores, which were respectively labeled as responders (N = 26) and non-responders (N = 34) (Tables S3, S4). With either classification, responders, and non-responders did not significantly differ in ethnicity, age, total IQ, handedness, clinical presentation, and MHP dose (Table S5). According to the classification based on a 30% BAARS-IV improvement cut-off, there was no significant difference in baseline symptom severity between groups; however, we found a significant difference when using the classification based on k-mean clustering (Table S5). We considered baseline severity among the potential confounding factors in our statistical analysis (see the “Methods” section). Drug screening and MPH assay results are reported in Supplementary results (p. 6).
Statistical analysis
Categorical approach
We analyzed whether tract metrics were associated with treatment response by using logistic regression. When we tested the previously published definition of response, i.e., symptom reduction of at least 30%, we observed that a smaller left SLF I volume was associated with non-response only at a trend level (Wald’s X2(1,60) = 3.502, p = 0.061). In contrast, when we used the data-driven classification responders/non-responders based on k-mean clustering, we found that ADHD participants with a smaller left SLF I volume, increased right-lateralization of the SLF I, or greater left anterior thalamic HMOA had a significantly lower probability of being responders to two months of MPH treatment (Table 1). No associations were detected for the fronto-striatal tract. All logistic regression results are reported in Table S6. ROC curves are presented in Fig. 1. AUCs, specificity, and sensitivity at optimal cut-off are reported in Table 1.
Plots for the SLF I Volume, SLF I lateralization index, and left anterior thalamic HMOA. The left SLF I volume was the strongest associate of treatment response. AT anterior thalamic, AUC area under the curve, HMOA hindrance modulated orientational anisotropy, LI lateralization index, ROC receiver operating characteristic, SLF superior longitudinal fasciculus, Vol volume.
The regression including the left SLF I volume survived family-wise correction for multiple comparisons (number of tracts = 10) (Table S8). ROC curve analysis revealed that the left SLF I volume was moderately associated with treatment response (AUC = 70.9%). The 10-fold cross-validation showed that 70% of the new observations kept the association between the left SLF I volume and treatment response (Supplementary results, p. 10). Further, the association between the left SLF I volume and treatment response remained statistically significant after controlling for handedness, years of previous treatment, and MPH dose (all p < 0.01), and when controlling for age, baseline severity, and weight (respectively, p = 0.011, p = 0.012, and p = 0.019).
We then investigated the relationship between the three significant tract metrics and treatment response (as classified by k-mean clustering) in the following two secondary analyses. Given the exploratory aim of these analyses, and for completeness, we also carried out these analyses for the two metrics that did not survive correction for multiple comparisons (SLF I lateralization index and left anterior thalamic HMOA), although caution should be used when interpreting them.
Comparison with controls
Considering the definition of treatment response based on k-mean clustering, non-responders significantly differed from controls in the left SLF I volume (t(52) = −2.172, p = 0.034), whereas responders did not (t(44) = 0.881, p = 0.383). As detailed in Supplementary results (p. 10), non-responders also differed from controls in the SLF I lateralization index, whereas responders and controls did not significantly differ in both the SLF lateralization index and the left anterior thalamic HMOA. That is, the anatomy of individuals with ADHD who responded to treatment did not significantly differ from that of controls in all three metrics (Fig. 2).
Panels A and B, respectively, show the tractography reconstruction of the SLF branches and the left anterior thalamic radiation. As displayed in panel C, participants with ADHD with smaller left SLF I volume and greater right-lateralization of the SLF I had a lower probability of being responders to two months of treatment. Notably, responders did not differ from controls in these tract metrics. Further, participants with ADHD with greater left anterior thalamic HMOA had a lower probability of being responders to two months of treatment. Again, responders did not differ from controls in this tract metric. AT anterior thalamic, HMOA hindrance modulated orientational anisotropy, SLF superior longitudinal fasciculus.
Dimensional approach
When we used linear regression to test whether the association between tract metrics and treatment response was driven by improvement on either inattentive and/or hyperactive/impulsive symptoms, we observed that the left SLF I volume was positively associated with the individual degree of improvement on both inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive symptoms (p = 0.011 and 0.003, respectively). Further, the left anterior thalamic HMOA was negatively associated with improvement on inattentive symptoms (Table 2 and Fig. 3).
The left SLF I volume was associated with the degree of symptomatic improvement on both inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive symptoms. The left anterior thalamic HMOA was only associated with the improvement on inattentive symptoms. AT anterior thalamic, HMOA hindrance modulated orientational anisotropy, SLF superior longitudinal fasciculus, vol volume.
Machine learning
Finally, lasso regression produced 200 treatment response models with an average BAC of 96.6%. To limit overfitting while achieving optimal accuracy, we selected the model with the least number of variables that showed at least 80% BAC [40]. The chosen model achieved 82.5% BAC (as compared to 73.7% for the left SLF I volume alone) and included 10 variables: left SLF I volume, baseline severity of inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive symptoms, medication-naïve status, MPH dose, handedness (categorical and dimensional score), total IQ, Qb impulsivity score after an acute dose of MPH, and baseline Longest Passivity (i.e., the maximum number of consecutive omission errors at the CPT as measured by the Qb test). The exponentiated regression coefficients (odds ratios) for each included variable are reported in Table S9.
Discussion
For the first time, we examined the relationship between the anatomy of specific brain networks and response to MPH in adults with ADHD. Our results suggest that a smaller left SLF I, which corresponds to the dorsal attentive network (DAN) [41], was associated at a group level with a significantly lower probability of being responders to two months of MPH treatment. Secondary analyses indicated that the association between the left SLF I volume and treatment response was driven by improvement on both inattentive and hyperactive/impulsive symptoms and that only non-responders significantly differed from controls in this tract metric. Overall, these novel findings suggest that a smaller left DAN may contribute to the pathophysiology of adult ADHD and partly explain variation in treatment response.
The fact that the SLF I is related to treatment response is likely explained by its role in supporting core brain functions implicated in ADHD, such as attention, working memory, and motor control [25, 26, 41,42,43]. Further, prior diffusion imaging studies identified alterations in the SLF of participants with ADHD as compared to controls, which were associated with symptom severity and neuropsychological deficits [44,45,46,47,48,49]. Only a few imaging studies reported differences in fronto-parietal networks between responders and non-responders and were all in children [22, 23], although a recent small study reported improvement of visuo-spatial attention under MPH in children with a dorso-ventral gradient of the FA of the SLF [50]. We added to these prior findings as we observed that only the SLF I was associated with treatment response in adults, and that only non-responders significantly differed in this metric as compared to controls. Our results suggest that some anatomical alterations may be more evident in non-responders and may contribute to their poor treatment response.
Unfortunately, tractography does not allow the direct investigation of the biological mechanisms through which a smaller left SLF I might influence treatment response. A smaller tract may be related to altered white matter (e.g. myelination) or gray matter (e.g. reduced number or size of axons) [36], both of which have been previously suggested as pathophysiological mechanisms in ADHD [23, 51,52,53]. Ultimately, a smaller tract may affect conduction speed and thus brain function [54, 55]. We might speculate that an asymmetrical representation of the SLF I in the two hemispheres may condition an imbalance between hemispheric contributions during the voluntary control of attention and may lead to less efficient suppression of the right-lateralized ventral attentive network (VAN), which in turn may enhance distractibility [56]. This imbalance might also potentially hinder MPH modulatory activity on these networks. We therefore encourage future studies to investigate the role of asymmetry of attentive networks in ADHD pathophysiology. Although the underlying mechanisms are unknown, our results are in line with animal studies suggesting that therapeutic doses of stimulants mainly affect catecholamine levels at a cortical level [57, 58]; and with prior imaging studies reporting stimulant-related functional changes in frontoparietal networks in adults with ADHD [59,60,61].
Nevertheless, as supported by our machine learning approach, other individual characteristics, such as clinical and cognitive profiles, likely also play a role in treatment response [14, 15]. For instance, we observed that a lower total IQ and more severe inattention and impulsivity levels at the CPT (at baseline or under an acute dose of MPH, respectively) were associated with worse response. This was also observed in some previous reports [16, 17], although not all confirmed the potential association of IQ with outcome [14]. As prior studies suggested that executive attention might play an important role in intelligence, acting as a bottleneck on task performance [62], it is possible to speculate that poorer attentive functions may be associated with both a lower total IQ and lower responsiveness to MPH. However, further studies are needed to clarify how general cognitive levels may be linked to treatment response. Further, left-handedness appeared to be indirectly associated with poorer response. Prior studies reported conflicting results on handedness and ADHD, with findings varying from normal to higher rates of left-handed subjects among participants with ADHD [63, 64]. We found a higher proportion of left-handed subjects in our ADHD group as compared to controls and this, together with the neuropsychological findings from the studies cited above, may further support the suggestion that response to stimulants is affected by the brain anatomical and functional organization on which MPH acts. Finally, clinical characteristics such as baseline symptom severity, medication-naïve status, and MPH dose may also be important—in agreement with previous clinical studies [14, 65, 66]. Taken together, these findings suggest that, given the heterogeneity of ADHD, a profile that combines information by integrating data from multiple sources may be more effective in identifying factors associated with treatment response.
Overall, this study is one of the largest diffusion imaging studies in adult ADHD and benefitted from the longitudinal design and the use of an advanced tractography method; however, limitations should be taken into consideration. We included a small percentage of participants previously treated with ADHD medication. Exposure to medication has been suggested to have a ‘normalizing’ effect on brain structure by some prior studies [67], although more recent reports suggest that this may not be the case [68]. Our findings suggest that the observed ‘normalizing’ effect of long-term treatment on brain anatomy may be at least in part dependent on a selection bias, as responders to treatment have fewer pre-treatment brain alterations. Further, this study was designed as an open trial, as we were unable to carry out a randomized controlled trial due to ethical constraints—as this would mean withholding what is an effective treatment. Nonetheless this is unlikely to have affected our results because virtual dissections were performed using a semi-automated procedure and blind to-group; and the symptom rating scale was integrated with objective measures of treatment response, such as the Qb scores. Finally, we only recruited males, because the prevalence of ADHD has been reported to be more than double in males as compared to females [69], and there is preliminary evidence of sex differences in brain connectivity [70, 71]. However, it is not known whether sex differences in brain connectivity may relate to sex differences in treatment response. Thus, to avoid potential sex-related confounding on our connectivity analyses, we only included males, but we encourage future studies to extend our analyses to the female ADHD population. Similarly, we only included participants without comorbid conditions because there is evidence that differences in brain connectivity and treatment response may exist between individuals with/without comorbidities [72]. For instance, recent meta-analyses highlighted both shared and specific connectivity alterations in individuals with ADHD and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) [73, 74]; and individuals with both conditions have lower response rates to stimulants [75]. Considering that this is the first study investigating the association between anatomical connectivity and treatment response in adult ADHD, we wanted to avoid potential comorbidity-related confounding. However, our results should be validated in clinical samples including individuals with comorbidities.
Investigating factors associated with treatment response represents a first step towards the identification of potential predictors of treatment response [13]. This is of relevance because, although at a group level MPH has been proven to be effective in ameliorating symptoms, there is individual variability in response, and this affects outcomes [8]. Further, the choice of medication in clinical practice is based on a trial-and-error process. Thus, there is increasing recognition that we need to better understand the biological basis of treatment response and (where possible) at an individual level [13]. However, to be tested as potential predictors according to the principles of precision psychiatry, the anatomical factors associated with treatment response that we report should first be replicated in independent samples and, preferably, confirmed by meta-analyses [76]. Further, although machine learning approaches, like the one we used in our exploratory analysis, have been deemed to be suitable to combine variables at multiple interacting levels, and we performed 10-fold cross-validation as a means of ‘internal’ validation, we did not have an independent sample for external validation, which might have led to inflated accuracy. Therefore, our results need to be validated in an external sample—and of sufficient size to ensure stable and generalizable accuracy estimates [76]. Only once these steps have been completed, can the utility of our proposed model be tested in pragmatic randomized trials [13, 77]. Nevertheless, identifying factors associated with treatment response is a necessary and valuable first step.
In conclusion, we provided novel evidence that the anatomy of the DAN may partly explain variation in treatment response in adult ADHD. If replicated, these findings may help identify predictors of treatment response and guide the development of new treatments.
References
Sibley MH, Mitchell JT, Becker SP. Method of adult diagnosis influences estimated persistence of childhood ADHD: a systematic review of longitudinal studies. Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3:1157–65.
Kessler RC, Adler L, Barkley R, Biederman J, Conners CK, Demler O, et al. The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:716–23.
Seixas M, Weiss M, Muller U. Systematic review of national and international guidelines on attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Psychopharmacol. 2012;26:753–65.
Faraone SV. The pharmacology of amphetamine and methylphenidate: relevance to the neurobiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and other psychiatric comorbidities. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;87:255–70.
Rubia K, Alegria AA, Cubillo AI, Smith AB, Brammer MJ, Radua J. Effects of stimulants on brain function in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;76:616–28.
Pereira-Sanchez V, Franco AR, Vieira D, de Castro-Manglano P, Soutullo C, Milham MP, et al. Systematic review: medication effects on brain intrinsic functional connectivity in patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2021;60:222–35.
Biederman J, Mick E, Surman C, Doyle R, Hammerness P, Harpold T, et al. A randomized, placebo-controlled trial of OROS methylphenidate in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:829–35.
Cortese S, Adamo N, Del Giovane C, Mohr-Jensen C, Hayes AJ, Carucci S, et al. Comparative efficacy and tolerability of medications for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in children, adolescents, and adults: a systematic review and network meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. 2018;5:727–38.
Schrantee A, Tamminga HG, Bouziane C, Bottelier MA, Bron EE, Mutsaerts HJ, et al. Age-dependent effects of methylphenidate on the human dopaminergic system in young vs adult patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a Randomized Clinical Trial. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73:955–62.
Bhattacharya SE, Shumsky JS, Waterhouse BD. Attention enhancing effects of methylphenidate are age-dependent. Exp Gerontol. 2015;61:1–7.
Chang Z, Ghirardi L, Quinn PD, Asherson P, D’Onofrio BM, Larsson H. Risks and benefits of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder medication on behavioral and neuropsychiatric outcomes: a qualitative review of pharmacoepidemiology studies using linked prescription databases. Biol Psychiatry. 2019;86:335–43.
Pitts M, Mangle L, Asherson P. Impairments, diagnosis and treatments associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in UK adults: results from the lifetime impairment survey. Arch Psychiatr Nurs. 2015;29:56–63.
Cortese S. Setting the foundations of developmental precision psychiatry for ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. 2021;178:677–9.
Retz W, Retz-Junginger P. Prediction of methylphenidate treatment outcome in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2014;264:S35–43.
Hermens DF, Rowe DL, Gordon E, Williams LM. Integrative neuroscience approach to predict ADHD stimulant response. Expert Rev Neurother. 2006;6:753–63.
Vallejo-Valdivielso M, de Castro-Manglano P, Diez-Suarez A, Marin-Mendez JJ, Soutullo CA. Clinical and neuropsychological predictors of methylphenidate response in children and adolescents with ADHD: a naturalistic follow-up study in a Spanish sample. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health. 2019;15:160–71.
Hermens DF, Cooper NJ, Kohn M, Clarke S, Gordon E. Predicting stimulant medication response in ADHD: evidence from an integrated profile of neuropsychological, psychophysiological and clinical factors. J Integr Neurosci. 2005;4:107–21.
Kambeitz J, Romanos M, Ettinger U. Meta-analysis of the association between dopamine transporter genotype and response to methylphenidate treatment in ADHD. Pharmacogenom J. 2014;14:77–84.
la Fougere C, Krause J, Krause KH, Josef Gildehaus F, Hacker M, Koch W, et al. Value of 99mTc-TRODAT-1 SPECT to predict clinical response to methylphenidate treatment in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Nucl Med Commun. 2006;27:733–7.
Krause J, la Fougere C, Krause KH, Ackenheil M, Dresel SH. Influence of striatal dopamine transporter availability on the response to methylphenidate in adult patients with ADHD. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2005;255:428–31.
Norman LJ, Sudre G, Bouyssi-Kobar M, Sharp W, Shaw P. A longitudinal study of resting-state connectivity and response to psychostimulant treatment in ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. 2021;178:744–51.
An L, Cao XH, Cao QJ, Sun L, Yang L, Zou QH, et al. Methylphenidate normalizes resting-state brain dysfunction in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38:1287–95.
Moreno A, Duno L, Hoekzema E, Picado M, Martin LM, Fauquet J, et al. Striatal volume deficits in children with ADHD who present a poor response to methylphenidate. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;23:805–12.
van Ewijk H, Heslenfeld DJ, Zwiers MP, Buitelaar JK, Oosterlaan J. Diffusion tensor imaging in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36:1093–106.
Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell’Acqua F, Forkel SJ, Simmons A, Vergani F, Murphy DG, et al. A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:1245–6.
Parlatini V, Radua J, Dell’Acqua F, Leslie A, Simmons A, Murphy DG, et al. Functional segregation and integration within fronto-parietal networks. Neuroimage. 2017;146:367–75.
Kim JW, Sharma V, Ryan ND. Predicting Methylphenidate Response in ADHD Using Machine Learning Approaches. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2015;18:pyv052.
Huys QJ, Maia TV, Frank MJ. Computational psychiatry as a bridge from neuroscience to clinical applications. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19:404–13.
Wechsler D. Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation, 1999.
Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113.
Barkley RA. Barkley adult ADHD rating scale-IV (BAARS-IV), ix. New York: Guilford Press; 2011. 150 pp.
Dolgin E. FDA clearance paves way for computerized ADHD monitoring. Nat Med. 2014;20:454–5.
Jain AK. Data clustering: 50 years beyond K-means. Pattern Recognit Lett. 2010;31:651–66.
Sanefuji M, Craig M, Parlatini V, Mehta MA, Murphy DG, Catani M, et al. Double-dissociation between the mechanism leading to impulsivity and inattention in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: a resting-state functional connectivity study. Cortex. 2017;86:290–302.
Rojkova K, Volle E, Urbanski M, Humbert F, Dell’Acqua F, Thiebaut de Schotten M. Atlasing the frontal lobe connections and their variability due to age and education: a spherical deconvolution tractography study. Brain Struct Funct. 2016;221:1751–66.
Dell’Acqua F, Simmons A, Williams SC, Catani M. Can spherical deconvolution provide more information than fiber orientations? Hindrance modulated orientational anisotropy, a true-tract specific index to characterize white matter diffusion. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013;34:2464–83.
Nichols T, Hayasaka S. Controlling the familywise error rate in functional neuroimaging: a comparative review. Stat Methods Med Res. 2003;12:419–46.
Lu K, Jiang L, Tsiatis AA. Multiple imputation approaches for the analysis of dichotomized responses in longitudinal studies with missing data. Biometrics. 2010;66:1202–8.
Graham JW, Olchowski AE, Gilreath TD. How many imputations are really needed? Some practical clarifications of multiple imputation theory. Prev Sci. 2007;8:206–13.
Thome J, Ehlis AC, Fallgatter AJ, Krauel K, Lange KW, Riederer P, et al. Biomarkers for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). A consensus report of the WFSBP task force on biological markers and the World Federation of ADHD. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2012;13:379–400.
Alves PN, Forkel SJ, Corbetta M, Thiebaut de Schotten M. The subcortical and neurochemical organization of the ventral and dorsal attention networks. Commun Biol. 2022;5:1343.
Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:201–15.
Ni AM, Bowes BS, Ruff DA, Cohen MR. Methylphenidate as a causal test of translational and basic neural coding hypotheses. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2022;119:e2120529119.
Chiang HL, Yang LK, Chen YJ, Hsu YC, Lo YC, Tseng WI, et al. Altered white-matter tract property in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience. 2022;487:78–87.
Hyde C, Sciberras E, Efron D, Fuelscher I, Silk T. Reduced fine motor competence in children with ADHD is associated with atypical microstructural organization within the superior longitudinal fasciculus. Brain Imaging Behav. 2021;15:727–37.
van Ewijk H, Heslenfeld DJ, Zwiers MP, Faraone SV, Luman M, Hartman CA, et al. Different mechanisms of white matter abnormalities in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a diffusion tensor imaging study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53:790–9 e3.
Wolfers T, Onnink AM, Zwiers MP, Arias-Vasquez A, Hoogman M, Mostert JC, et al. Lower white matter microstructure in the superior longitudinal fasciculus is associated with increased response time variability in adults with attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2015;40:344–51.
Douglas PK, Gutman B, Anderson A, Larios C, Lawrence KE, Narr K, et al. Hemispheric brain asymmetry differences in youths with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuroimage Clin. 2018;18:744–52.
Chiang HL, Hsu YC, Shang CY, Tseng WI, Gau SS. White matter endophenotype candidates for ADHD: a diffusion imaging tractography study with sibling design. Psychol Med. 2020;50:1203–13.
Mazzetti C, Gonzales Damatac C, Sprooten E, Ter Huurne N, Buitelaar JK, Jensen O. Dorsal-to-ventral imbalance in the superior longitudinal fasciculus mediates methylphenidate’s effect on beta oscillations in ADHD. Psychophysiology. 2022;59:e14008.
Silk TJ, Vance A, Rinehart N, Bradshaw JL, Cunnington R. Structural development of the basal ganglia in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Psychiatry Res. 2009;172:220–5.
Nagel BJ, Bathula D, Herting M, Schmitt C, Kroenke CD, Fair D, et al. Altered white matter microstructure in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50:283–92.
Shaw P, Lerch J, Greenstein D, Sharp W, Clasen L, Evans A, et al. Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and clinical outcome in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:540–9.
Cragg BG, Thomas PK. The relationships between conduction velocity and the diameter and internodal length of peripheral nerve fibres. J Physiol. 1957;136:606–14.
Waxman SG, Bennett MV. Relative conduction velocities of small myelinated and non-myelinated fibres in the central nervous system. Nat N Biol. 1972;238:217–9.
Corbetta M, Patel G, Shulman GL. The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron. 2008;58:306–24.
Berridge CW, Devilbiss DM, Andrzejewski ME, Arnsten AF, Kelley AE, Schmeichel B, et al. Methylphenidate preferentially increases catecholamine neurotransmission within the prefrontal cortex at low doses that enhance cognitive function. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:1111–20.
Hannestad J, Gallezot JD, Planeta-Wilson B, Lin SF, Williams WA, van Dyck CH, et al. Clinically relevant doses of methylphenidate significantly occupy norepinephrine transporters in humans in vivo. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;68:854–60.
Cubillo A, Rubia K. Structural and functional brain imaging in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Expert Rev Neurother. 2010;10:603–20.
Cary RP, Ray S, Grayson DS, Painter J, Carpenter S, Maron L, et al. Network structure among brain systems in adult ADHD is uniquely modified by stimulant administration. Cereb Cortex. 2017;27:3970–9.
Picon FA, Sato JR, Anes M, Vedolin LM, Mazzola AA, Valentini BB, et al. Methylphenidate alters functional connectivity of default mode network in drug-naive male adults with ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2020;24:447–55.
Conway ARA, Kovacs K, Hao H, Rosales KP, Snijder JP. Individual differences in attention and intelligence: a united cognitive/psychometric approach. J Intell. 2021;9:34.
Ghanizadeh A. Lack of association of handedness with inattention and hyperactivity symptoms in ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2013;17:302–7.
Niederhofer H. Hand preference in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Percept Mot Skills. 2005;101:808–10.
Jensen PS, Arnold LE, Swanson JM, Vitiello B, Abikoff HB, Greenhill LL, et al. 3-year follow-up of the NIMH MTA study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46:989–1002.
Pliszka S, Issues AWGoQ. Practice parameter for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46:894–21.
Nakao T, Radua J, Rubia K, Mataix-Cols D. Gray matter volume abnormalities in ADHD: voxel-based meta-analysis exploring the effects of age and stimulant medication. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168:1154–63.
Greven CU, Bralten J, Mennes M, O’Dwyer L, van Hulzen KJ, Rommelse N, et al. Developmentally stable whole-brain volume reductions and developmentally sensitive caudate and putamen volume alterations in those with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected siblings. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72:490–9.
Polanczyk G, de Lima MS, Horta BL, Biederman J, Rohde LA. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:942–8.
King JB, Yurgelun-Todd D, Stoeckel A, DiMuzio JM, Lopez-Larson MP. Sex differences in white matter integrity in youths with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a pilot study. Front Neurosci. 2015;9:232.
Tung YH, Lin HY, Chen CL, Shang CY, Yang LY, Hsu YC, et al. Whole brain white matter tract deviation and idiosyncrasy from normative development in autism and ADHD and unaffected siblings link with dimensions of psychopathology and cognition. Am J Psychiatry. 2021;178:730–43.
Parlatini V, Itahashi T, Lee Y, Liu S, Nguyen TT, Aoki YY, Forkel SJ, Catani M, Rubia K, Zhou JH, Murphy DG, Cortese S. White matter alterations in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): a systematic review of 129 diffusion imaging studies with meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry. 2023. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-023-02173-1.
Zhao Y, Yang L, Gong G, Cao Q, Liu J. Identify aberrant white matter microstructure in ASD, ADHD and other neurodevelopmental disorders: A meta-analysis of diffusion tensor imaging studies. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2022;113:110477.
Di X, Azeez A, Li X, Haque E, Biswal BB. Disrupted focal white matter integrity in autism spectrum disorder: a voxel-based meta-analysis of diffusion tensor imaging studies. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2018;82:242–8.
Aishworiya R, Valica T, Hagerman R, Restrepo B. An update on psychopharmacological treatment of autism spectrum disorder. Neurotherapeutics. 2022;19:248–62.
Fusar-Poli P, Hijazi Z, Stahl D, Steyerberg EW. The Science of Prognosis in Psychiatry: A Review. JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75:1289–97.
Chekroud AM, Bondar J, Delgadillo J, Doherty G, Wasil A, Fokkema M, et al. The promise of machine learning in predicting treatment outcomes in psychiatry. World Psychiatry. 2021;20:154–70.
Acknowledgements
We thank all the participants who took part in the study, the Adult ADHD Clinic and Pharmacy Team at the Maudsley Hospital; and appreciate the contribution of the QbTech Company (https://www.qbtech.com), who provided the materials to carry out the Qb test. We would like to thank the NATBRAINLAB (http://www.natbrainlab.co.uk) and the Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, King’s College London, for helpful discussion.
Funding
This research project was funded by Shire (project IST-ALB-000217) and included support from the NIHR Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre. Further, the results leading to this publication have received funding from the Innovative Medicines Initiative 2 Joint Undertaking under grant agreement No 777394 for the project AIMS-2-TRIALS. This Joint Undertaking receives support from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme and EFPIA and AUTISM SPEAKS, Autistica, SFARI. Dr. Michel Thiebaut de Schotten was supported by the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under the European Research Council (ERC) Consolidator Grant “DISCONNECTOME” (ERC-2018-COG 818521), and the University of Bordeaux’s IdEx “Investments for the Future” program RRI “IMPACT,” which received financial support from the French government. Prof. Marco Catani was the recipient of a Wellcome Trust Investigator Award (103759/Z/14/Z). Prof. Declan Murphy was funded by two IMI initiatives (EU AIMS and AIMS-2-TRIALS) under grant agreements nos.115300 and 777394.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
All authors made substantial contributions to the study. VP was responsible for the recruitment of participants, data acquisition and analysis, and writing up of the manuscript. JR contributed to the data analysis and writing of the manuscript. ASF contributed to the data analysis and writing of the manuscript. RW, SM, and MS contributed to the recruitment and/or testing of participants and revision of the manuscript. FDA supervised the acquisition and processing of the imaging data and revised the manuscript. MC contributed to the study design and the recruitment of participants. MTDS was responsible for the study design and critical revision of the manuscript; DM was responsible for the study design and critical revision of the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The design of the study, data collection and analysis, the writing of the manuscript or the decision to publish were not influenced by the funding bodies. Any views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the funders. The other authors declare no competing interest.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Parlatini, V., Radua, J., Solanes Font, A. et al. Poor response to methylphenidate is associated with a smaller dorsal attentive network in adult Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Transl Psychiatry 13, 303 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-023-02598-w
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-023-02598-w