Abstract
The presence of a partner can attenuate physiological fear responses, a phenomenon known as social buffering. However, not all individuals are equally sociable. Here we investigated whether social buffering of fear is shaped by sensitivity to social anxiety (social concern) and whether these effects are different in females and males. We collected skin conductance responses (SCRs) and affect ratings of female and male participants when they experienced aversive and neutral sounds alone (alone treatment) or in the presence of an unknown person of the same gender (social treatment). Individual differences in social concern were assessed based on a well-established questionnaire. Our results showed that social concern had a stronger effect on social buffering in females than in males. The lower females scored on social concern, the stronger the SCRs reduction in the social compared to the alone treatment. The effect of social concern on social buffering of fear in females disappeared if participants were paired with a virtual agent instead of a real person. Together, these results showed that social buffering of human fear is shaped by gender and social concern. In females, the presence of virtual agents can buffer fear, irrespective of individual differences in social concern. These findings specify factors that shape the social modulation of human fear, and thus might be relevant for the treatment of anxiety disorders.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Excessive fear responses are characteristic of anxiety disorders and pave the way for a variety of other psychological and psychiatric problems. There is evidence from animal models that autonomic [1, 2] and neural [2,3,4,5] fear responses are reduced in the presence of a non-fearful conspecific, a phenomenon known as social buffering [6,7,8]. Adapting the paradigms that were used to study social buffering in animals, recent studies have shown similar social buffering of autonomic fear or stress responses in humans [9,10,11], in particular in individuals who score higher on trait [12, 13] or state [9, 14] anxiety.
Social buffering effects in animals and humans are assumed to occur, because the presence of a conspecific or another individual signals the availability of resources to cope with imposed threat, and thus is perceived as a safety cue [11, 15, 16]. On the neurobiological level, the presence of a conspecific has been shown to reduce stress-induced changes in neural circuitries [2, 11, 17, 18] and synapses [3, 4].
However, not all humans are equally sociable. In some individuals, the presence of another person induces concern and anxiety, paralleled by an increase in autonomic fear and hormonal stress responses [19,20,21]. The sensitivity to anxiety in social situations has been defined as an independent dimension of human anxiety, known as social concern [22,23,24,25]. Social concern is characterized by worries about publicly observable anxious reactions such as trembling, blushing, or sweating in front of others [22, 26]. As a result, for individuals scoring high on social concern, the presence of another individual may impose a threat, instead of a safety cue that may trigger social buffering.
There is recent evidence suggesting that the virtual presence of another person can reduce responses to social stress, if participants believe that the virtual agent was steered by a real person [27, 28]. Based on these findings, it is possible that similar to a real person the presence of a virtual person can be perceived as a safety cue and thus elicits social buffering effects, and is irrespective of individual social concern.
Social anxiety is known to manifest differently in females and in males. In females, social anxiety tends to be generalized, i.e., to occur in a variety of different social contexts [29, 30]. In contrast, males are mainly concerned and anxious in situations related to mating, indicated by increased physiological fear responses in the presence of a potential partner [31]. Given that other individuals are seen as a threat in many different social contexts in females and only in mating contexts in males, high sensitivity to social anxiety may have a stronger effect on social buffering in females than in males.
Taken together, there is evidence that the effects of social buffering can attenuate physiological responses in humans, and thus are potentially relevant for the diagnosis and treatment of anxiety disorders. However, it remains unclear as to whether and how social buffering of human fear is shaped by social concern, whether the effects of social concern are different in females and males, and whether and how social buffering effects differ in the presence of a real and a virtual person.
To address these questions, we first tested how social concern influences the social buffering of fear in the presence of an unknown person in females and males (real-life; Study 1). Second, we investigated the effect of social concern on social buffering in the presence of a virtual agent (virtual reality, VR; Study 2). The agent was an exact virtual representation (created based on a body scan) of the real partner in Study 1 and was placed in a virtual environment that was identical to the testing room used in the real-life study (Fig. 1A). It was clear to the participants that the agent was not steered by a real person.
In more detail, in the real-life and the VR studies, participants were presented with the same aversive, fear-inducing or neutral sounds, and their autonomic responses (skin conductance responses, SCRs) and subjective ratings were measured. Participants experienced the sounds alone (real-life/VR alone treatment groups), or in the presence of an unknown person of the same gender and ethnicity, or a virtual representation of that person (real-life/VR social treatment groups). The real or virtual partners faced away from the participant and her or his screen and fixated on the opposite wall (Fig. 1A). This manipulation was important to minimize social interaction and the effect of social evaluation. Hence, the partner was merely physically present, without direct social interaction, similar to studies that investigated social buffering in animals. The gender of the partner (confederate) matched the gender of the participants to avoid the potential complications inherent in mixed-gender pairings and consistent with same-sex setting used by most animal studies [32, 33].
We assessed individual differences in social concern (worries regarding social censure or rejection because of publicly observable reactions) [22, 23] using the Anxiety Sensitivity Index (ASI-3; 22). The ASI-3 is a well-established questionnaire that allows for assessing social concern in parallel with other anxiety dimensions, i.e., physical and cognitive concern (see below for details). The parallel assessment of different anxiety dimensions is important, because it allows us to evaluate the effect of social concern, compared to physical and cognitive concern.
In more detail, the social concern scale assesses concern regarding observable reactions in the presence of others. Individuals indicate whether they are concerned to appear nervous (“It is important for me not to appear nervous.”), tremble (“When I tremble in the presence of others, I fear what people might think of me.”), blush (“It scares me when I blush in front of people.”), sweat (“When I begin to sweat in a social situation, I fear people will think negatively of me.”) or faint (“I think it would be horrible for me to faint in public.”) in the presence of another person. Accordingly, social concern can be elicited by the mere presence of another individual.
Guided by previous studies [9, 34], we assumed that the real or virtual presence of another person might reduce the subjective and autonomic fear responses, indicating a social buffering effect. In this case, the SCRs and subjective ratings to aversive sounds should be significantly smaller in the social compared to the alone treatment groups. Given that individuals scoring high on social concern tend to fear observable reactions in the presence of others (e.g., blushing, trembling etc), high social concern scores should counteract social buffering. The higher the social concern, the smaller the reduction in SCRs and subjective ratings to aversive sounds in the social compared to the alone treatment groups. Inspired by findings showing more generalized social anxiety effects in females [29,30,31], we hypothesized that this effect might be stronger in females than in males, reflected by a significant interaction between gender (female/male), treatment (alone/social) and social concern. Motivated by studies showing a reduction of social stress during virtual compared to real-life social interactions [35], we expected that the effect of social concern on social buffering should be significantly stronger in the presence of a real person compared to the presence of a virtual representation (agent) of this same person.
Methods and materials
Real-life study 1
Participants
The sample size required to detect differential effects between treatment groups and the modulating effect of individual differences (here social concern) was estimated based on a power calculation (threshold of 80% sensitivity (1 − beta = 0.80); α = 0.05; medium effect size [36]) of our previous study [9] in which we used a similar design as in the current study. The results yielded a required sample size smaller than 26 in each group. In total, 147 healthy Caucasian participants (72 females) participated in this study. The participants had no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders and were pre-screened to exclude depressive symptoms [37]. All participants gave written informed consent before the study. Participants were randomly assigned to an alone treatment group or a social treatment group. The participants in the social treatment group were paired with a person of the same gender and ethnicity, i.e., a Caucasian female or male student who was unknown to all the participants, to avoid the potential complications of gender-mixed and ethnicity-mixed pairings of participants and confederates. The confederate was asked to face away from the participant and her or his screen and to fixate on the opposite wall to minimize social interaction and the effect of social evaluation. Participants in the alone treatment group completed the experiment in absence of another person (Fig. 1A). The data of 13 participants had to be excluded: four due to technical problems, six because of extensive artefacts in the SCRs signal that could not be corrected, and three because of a lack of SCRs during the entire data acquisition. We analyzed the data of 134 participants: 31 females in the alone treatment (Mage = 23.74, SDage = 0.40), 35 females in social treatment (Mage = 25.51, SDage = 0.85), 34 males in alone treatment (Mage = 24.76, SDage = 0.72) and 34 males in social treatment (Mage = 27.21, SDage = 0.86). The data of the female sample were part of an analysis reported elsewhere [9]. However, none of the analyses in the current manuscript have been previously reported. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University Hospital of Wurzburg (142/18), and all procedures were conducted in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975 and its later amendments.
Scales
Before the study, all participants were screened for depression [37] and filled in questionnaires measuring different dimensions of anxiety sensitivity [22], perceived social support (Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support, MSPSS; [38]), positive and negative affect (Positive and Negative Affect Schedule, PANAS; [39]) and state and trait anxiety (State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, STAI; [40]). After the study, participants of the social treatment groups rated statements regarding their impressions of the other person (the confederate) on a well-established scale [41], ranging from 1 (not at all) to 9 (very much).
Stimuli and paradigm
During the test, all participants were presented with aversive and neutral sounds via headphones. The sounds were taken from the International Affective Digital Sounds system (IADS; [42]). Previous studies showed that the presentation of the aversive sounds can induce fear, which was significantly stronger than the neutral sounds [9, 43]. The type of the sound was indicated by a preceding image (cue) presented on a black screen: a white circle was the cue for aversive sounds whereas a white square indicated neutral sounds. The sounds and corresponding images were presented in a pseudo-randomized order. Each trial started with a fixation cross, which was replaced by one of the cues. After an interval varying from 6 to 25 s (mean interval = 16.2 s), the respective sound (4 s) was presented. Then participants rated their feeling while hearing the sound on a continuous scale from −4 (very bad) to +4 (very good) that was presented for 4 s (Fig. 1B). The cursor of the rating scale was placed at a random position in each trial to keep participants’ attention and minimize rating error. There were three blocks of 18 trials with half of them including aversive sounds and the remaining half of them including neutral sounds. Before the main experiment, all participants performed four practice trials: two containing aversive sounds and two containing neutral sounds to familiarize themselves to the task.
Recording of SCRs
The SCRs were continuously collected during the main part of the experiment by a Brainamp ExG MR amplifier (Brain Products GmbH), and recorded by Vision Recorder software (Brain Products GmbH). Two Ag/AgCl electrodes filled with non-hydrating gel were attached to the thenar and hypothenar of participants’ nondominant palm. The sample rate was 1000 Hz. The constant voltage across the two electrodes was 0.5 V [9].
Data analysis
The SCRs were pre-processed using the EEGLAB toolbox [44]. Low-pass filtering (1 Hz) was performed on the raw data to remove high-frequency noise. The cue and the sound onset were defined as 0 s. For each trial, an epoch during the cue presentation (time window −1 to 6 s), and an epoch during sound presentation period (time window −1 to 10 s) were defined and separately segmented. The responses during the 1 s before the stimulus were used for baseline correction. Based on the grand-averaged wave, the SCRs value was characterized as the area under the curve per second in the time window from 2.5 to 6 s after stimulus onset, a method that has been recommended if discrete SCRs are hard to discern in one experimental condition (here discrete SCRs in response to the neutral cues/sounds were hard to detect) [9, 45,46,47].
Given that the cue-related SCRs did not differ between genders and studies (Tables S1, S2) and did not influence the observed social buffering effects (Tables S3–S6), our main analyses focused on SCRs elicited by the sound presentation. SCRs were transformed by log (x + 1) to correct for skewness.
We conducted linear mixed models with trial by trial affect ratings and SCRs as the dependent variable to investigate the buffering effect (LMM, “lme4” package of the R-software; [48]). We tested all fixed effects by setting a different “baseline” value for each participant (random intercept) to resolve non-independence of SCRs/affect ratings for each participant. The variables were included into the model based on our primary hypotheses. When testing the higher order interaction, all lower order terms were included in the regression equation [49]. All continuous predictors had been normalized to avoid potential multicollinearity [49]. To control for unspecific effects of state anxiety, depression and negative emotions before the experiment, the individual scores of the depression scale [23], PANAS-negative emotion scale [24], and the STAI-state-pre anxiety [10] were included as covariates in all LMMs. There was a significant overall decline in SCRs to aversive sounds in the second compared to the first half of the experiment. However, this habituation effect did not affect the observed social modulation effects (Tables S7, S8).
Effect sizes reported for LMM results are based on the marginal R2 (R2m) which estimates the proportion of variance explained by the fixed factors [50], calculated with the R package “MuMIn” [51]. Effect sizes reported for post-hoc and simple effect tests are based on estimated marginal means (EMM) [52], calculated with the R package “emmeans”. The Kenward–roger method was used for freedom degree estimation.
VR study 2
Participants
Sixty-one healthy Caucasian females participated in the VR study. The inclusion criteria were the same as in Study 1. Participants were randomly assigned to the alone and social treatment groups. The data of ten participants had to be excluded: one due to technical problems, one because of extensive artefacts in the SCRs signal that could not be corrected, six because of lack of SCRs, and two because of problems conducting the ratings. In total, we analyzed the data of 51 participants: 25 females in the alone treatment group (Mage = 23.29, SDage = 0.42) and 26 females in social treatment group (Mage = 23.98, SDage = 0.57). As Study 1, Study 2 was approved by the ethics committee of the University Hospital of Wurzburg. All participants gave written informed consent before the study. All procedures were conducted in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975 and its later amendments.
Scales
Participants completed the same scales as in Study 1. In addition, we used a German version of the Networked Minds Measure of Social Presence (NMMSP; [53]) to test how participants perceived the presence of the agent. It has two sub-dimensions, measuring perceived co-presence and psycho-behavioral accessibility. The co-presence scale measures the degree to which the presence of the agent is processed on a low-level sensory level (e.g., I was often aware of my partner in the room). The psycho-behavioral interaction scale measures the degree of perceived social interactions with the virtual agent (e.g., I was able to communicate my intentions clearly to my partner) on a scale from ‘1’ (not at all) to ‘7’ (very much).
Stimuli and paradigm
The stimuli and the paradigm were same as in Study 1 (Fig. 1B), except that participants performed the study in a virtual environment. They wore a VR headset (HTC Vive Pro). The resolution of the goggles was 2880 × 1600 pixels. The virtual environment was a precise representation of the testing room used during data collection in Study 1 (Fig. 1A), which was created with Unreal Engine 4.24.3. The agent was created based on a high-resolution body scan of the female confederate that was present in Study 1, following a human avatar generation pipeline by Achenbach et al. [54]. The confederate had given consent to publish the avatar picture for scientific use. As the confederate, the virtual agent faced away from the participant (Fig. 1A) and performed small humanoid movements (e.g., slightly moving the arm on the arm rest of the chair), but did not interact with the participant. The affect ratings were conducted by pressing the button on the controller.
Recording of SCRs and data analyses
Recording and pre-processing of the SCRs was identical to Study 1.
To investigate differences in social buffering effects between real and virtual social presence and the modulating effect of social concern, we combined the female data in the real-life (female data in Study 1) and the data in the VR (Study 2). Study (real-life/VR) was included as a between-subject variable. The statistics of SCRs were identical to Study 1.
Results
Study 1: Testing the effect of social concern on social buffering in the presence of a real person
Scales
The Cronbach’s α coefficients of scales ranged from 0.60 to 0.92 (see Table 1 for details). The average scores on ASI-3, MSPSS, ADS, STAI, and PANAS were comparable between the four groups (female-alone; female-social; male-alone; male-social) (Table 1). The only difference was found in impression ratings for the present partner that were more positive in the female compared to the male sample (Table 1).
Aversive sounds elicited a more negative affect and larger SCRs than neutral sounds
As a manipulation check, we compared the affect ratings and SCRs to aversive and neutral sounds in the alone treatment groups. The results showed significantly more negative affect ratings for aversive compared to neutral sounds in females and males, with a stronger effect in females, B = −0.56, p < 0.001 (females: aversive sounds, M = −1.97, SE = 0.11, neutral sounds, M = 1.33, SE = 0.11, t(3443) = −56.28, p < 0.0001, EMM = −3.31, SE = 0.059; males: aversive sounds, M = −1.69, SE = 0.11, neutral sounds, M = 1.05, SE = 0.11, t(3443) = −48.91, p < 0.0001, EMM = −2.74, SE = 0.056).
Moreover, SCRs to aversive sounds were significantly higher than neutral sounds with a stronger effect in females, B = 0.083, p < 0.001 (females: aversive sounds, M = 0.15, SE = 0.010, neutral sounds, M = −0.0063, SE = 0.010, t(3420) = 18.41, p < 0.001, EMM = 0.16, SE = 0.0084; males: aversive sounds, M = 0.084, SE = 0.010, neutral sounds, M = 0.012, SE = 0.010, t(3420) = 8.97, p < 0.001, EMM = 0.072, SE = 0.0080).
The SCRs to aversive sounds were significantly related to participants’ affect ratings with a larger effect in females, B = 0.024, p < 0.001 (females: B = −0.057, 95% CI = [−0.063, −0.052], males: B = −0.033, 95% CI = [−0.039, −0.027]). The higher the SCRs to the aversive sounds, the more negative participants’ affect ratings.
Social buffering in females, but not in males
First, we tested if the presence of a real partner had an effect on the ratings that participants made after hearing the sounds. An LMM with affect ratings to aversive sounds as the dependent variable and treatment (alone/social), gender (female/male) and treatment × gender as the predictors showed no significant effects, treatment (B = 0.35, p = 0.16), gender (B = 0.32, p = 0.21), treatment × gender (B = −0.21, p = 0.56); R2m = 0.028.
Second, we performed the same model with SCRs to aversive sounds as the dependent variable. The results showed significant main effects of treatment (B = −0.064, p = 0.0017) and gender (B = −0.058, p = 0.006), and a significant treatment × gender interaction (B = 0.059, p = 0.042) (Fig. 2A; Table 2); R2m = 0.019. Clarifying the interaction effect, simple effect analyses showed significantly larger SCRs in the alone treatment group compared to the social treatment group in females (alone: M = 0.14, SE = 0.015, social: M = 0.077, SE = 0.014, t(126) = 3.13, p = 0.0022, EMM = 0.064, SE = 0.021), but not in males (alone: M = 0.084, SE = 0.014, social: M = 0.078, SE = 0.014, t(126) = 0.29, p = 0.77, EMM = 0.0058, SE = 0.020). Together, these SCRs results indicate a significant social buffering effect in the female sample, but not in the male sample.
Given the gender differences in impression ratings (Table 1), it is possible that females showed stronger social buffering because they perceived their partner more positively than males. In this case, the individual differences in impression ratings should significantly predict the individual differences in SCRs in the social treatment groups. Note that this analysis can only be conducted on the social treatment groups, because participants in the alone treatment groups did not have a partner and thus could not provide impression ratings. An LMM with SCRs to aversive sounds as the dependent variable and gender, impression rating (the sum value), and gender × impression rating as the predictors showed no significant main effects of gender (B = 0.0035, p = 0.87) and impression rating (B = 0.0098, p = 0.47), and no significant gender × impression rating interaction (B = −0.012, p = 0.54); R2m = 0.0089. According to this result, gender differences in impression ratings are unlikely to account for the observed gender difference in social buffering.
Differential effects of social concern on social buffering in females and males
To investigate the effect of social concern on social buffering in females and males, we conducted an LMM with social concern, gender (female/male), treatment (social/alone) and their interaction terms as the predictors and SCRs to aversive sounds as the dependent variable. The results showed significant main effects of gender (B = −0.056, p = 0.007) and treatment (B = −0.064, p = 0.001), significant two-way interactions between social concern and gender (B = 0.051, p = 0.011), between treatment and gender (B = 0.059, p = 0.036), and between treatment and social concern (B = 0.044, p = 0.027), and a significant three-way interaction between social concern, treatment and gender (B = −0.077, p = 0.007), R2m = 0.034 (Fig. 2B and Table 3).
To unpack the three-way interaction, we conducted an LMM separately for females and males. For females, the result showed a significant main effect of treatment (B = −0.064, p = 0.010), and a marginally significant social concern × treatment effect (B = 0.046, p = 0.066); R2m = 0.028. As shown in Fig. 2B, in the social treatment group, females showed a reduction in SCRs to aversive sounds with decreasing social concern (B = 0.036, p = 0.014), while there was no such an effect in the alone treatment group (B = −0.025, p = 0.22). In males we found a significant main effect of social concern (B = 0.034, p = 0.0017), and a significant social concern × treatment interaction (B = −0.030, p = 0.045); R2m = 0.036. In males, the interaction effect was driven by a positive relationship between social concern and SCRs to aversive sounds in the alone treatment group (B = 0.030, p = 0.0094), while there was no such effect in the social treatment group (B = 0.0045, p = 0.68).
To test if the observed effects are specific for social concern, we conducted the same analyses with the two other anxiety dimensions of the ASI, i.e., physical concern and cognitive concern. The respective LMMs revealed no significant three-way interactions (physical concern × treatment × gender: B = −0.0075, p = 0.80, cognitive concern × treatment × gender: B = 0.021, p = 0.50; Tables S9 and S10).
Study 2: Testing the effect of social concern on social buffering in the presence of a virtual person
Study 1 revealed that females showed a reduction in fear-related SCRs in the presence of another person, but only if they scored low on social concern. However, it is unclear if social concern counteracts social buffering in females in general, or just in the presence of a real person. If social concern reduces social buffering only in the presence of a real person, the presence of a virtual person should induce social buffering in females irrespective of social concern. Study 2 tested this assumption by investigating the effect of social concern on social buffering of fear in females in the presence of a virtual person (agent).
Scales
The questionnaire scores of the female participants in Studies 1 and 2 were comparable (Table 4). The Cronbach’s α coefficients of these questionnaires ranged from 0.70 to 0.93 (see Table 4 for details). The results of the NMMSP questionnaire [53] indicated that the virtual agent induced a moderate level of perceived co-presence (M = 3.54, SE = 0.15) and a low level of perceived social interactions (M = 1.39, SE = 0.013). The impression ratings for the real partner were more positive than for the virtual agent (Table 4).
Aversive sounds elicited a more negative affect and larger SCRs than neutral sounds
As in Study 1, participants showed more negative ratings to aversive sounds compared to neutral sounds (aversive sounds, M = −2.23, SE = 0.15, neutral sounds, M = 1.93, SE = 0.15, t (2966) = −6.90, p < 0.001, EMM = −4.16, SE = 0.068). They also showed larger SCRs to aversive sounds compared to neutral sounds (aversive sounds, M = 0.086, SE = 0.013, neutral sounds, M = 0.0011, SE = 0.013, t (2942) = 8.43, p < 0.001, EMM = 0.085, SE = 0.010). Moreover, as in Study 1, there was a significant negative relationship between SCRs and affect ratings (B = −0.032, 95% CI = [−0.038 −0.026]), indicating that an increase in SCRs reflected a more negative emotional state.
Social buffering of fear in females in the presence of a real and a virtual person
We investigated if the mere presence of a virtual agent had a similar buffering effect on fear responses as the mere presence of a real person. First, we performed an LMM with affect ratings to aversive sounds as the dependent variable and treatment (alone/social), study (real-life/VR) and their interaction treatment × study as the predictors. The results revealed no significant effects, treatment (alone/social) (B = 0.34, p = 0.19), study (real-life/VR) (B = −0.10, p = 0.73), treatment × study (B = −0.032, p = 0.94), R2m = 0.053.
Second, we tested the same model with SCRs to aversive sounds as the dependent variable. The results showed significant main effects of study (B = −0.058, p = 0.021) and treatment (B = −0.065, p = 0.003), but no significant study × treatment interaction (B = 0.025, p = 0.44) (Fig. 3A; Table 5A); R2m = 0.030, indicating that the buffering effect was comparable between the real-life and the VR study. Conducting an LMM separately for the VR study revealed a significant main effect of treatment (B = −0.040, p = 0.033) (Table 5B); R2m = 0.035. Post-hoc analysis showed larger SCRs in the alone treatment (M = 0.086, SE = 0.013) compared to the social treatment group (M = 0.046, SE = 0.013), EMM = 0.040, SE = 0.019. These results indicate a significant social buffering effect in the VR study, which was comparable to the results of female participants in the real-life study.
Given the differences in impression regarding the real partner and the virtual agent (Table 4), we tested whether impression ratings significantly predicted individual differences in SCRs in the social treatment groups. An LMM with SCRs to aversive sounds as the dependent variable and study (real-life/VR), impression rating, and study × impression rating as the predictors showed no significant main effects of study (B = 0.0058, p = 0.82) and impression rating (B = 0.013, p = 0.39), and no significant study × impression rating interaction (B = 0.024, p = 0.32); R2m = 0.029, indicating that differences in impression ratings did not predict the SCRs in the social treatment groups.
Virtual social buffering occurs independently of social concern
Next, we tested if the social buffering of fear in the presence of a virtual person is shaped by social concern. The LMM with social concern, treatment (social/alone), study (real-life/VR) and their interaction items as the predictors, SCRs to aversive sounds as the dependent variable showed significant main effects of treatment (B = −0.061, p = 0.005) and study (B = −0.059, p = 0.018), a significant two-way interaction between social concern and treatment (B = 0.049, p = 0.035), and a significant three-way interaction between social concern, treatment and study (B = −0.070, p = 0.032), R2m = 0.039 (Fig. 3B; Table 6). This indicates that the reduction in SCRs in the social treatment group decreased with social concern in the real-life study, while there was no such effect in the VR study (B = −0.029, p = 0.086; Fig. 3B). According to these results, social concern did not modulate the buffering effect in the VR study.
In addition, we conducted the same analysis on the other ASI-3 subscales: physical concern and cognitive concern. Both LMMs on physical concern and on cognitive concern showed no significant effects (all p > 0.05, Tables S11 and S12), indicating no modulating effect of cognitive concern and physical concern.
Discussion
The results of our first study revealed an increased social buffering effect in females (Fig. 2A) that is mainly driven by those participants that scored low on social concern (Fig. 2B). In males, social concern increased the SCRs to aversive sounds in the alone treatment group (Fig. 2B). However, a similar increase in SCRs in the male sample was also observed with increasing physical concern (Fig. S1), indicating that in the male sample, social concern had no specific effect. Importantly, the differential effects of social concern on social buffering in females and males were found although both samples had a comparable average social concern score (Table 1). This means that females scored as high (or low) on social concern as males, but, in contrast to males, individual social concern affected the individual extent of female-social buffering.
Previous studies have shown that increases in trait and state anxiety are related to increasing physiological responses, and a stronger attenuation of these responses in the presence of others, i.e., stronger social buffering effects [9, 12, 13]. Trait anxiety assessed by the STAI [40] covers the individual disposition to respond in an anxious way, and STAI-state anxiety reflects a transitory emotion characterized by physiological arousal and consciously perceived feeling of apprehension, dread, and tension [40]. Social concern as measured by the social concern scale of the ASI [22] reflects the fear of publicly observable anxious reactions, and thus targets a more specific anxiety dimension than the STAI. To account for the potential effects of state and trait anxiety, the individual STAI state and trait scores were added as control variables in analyses testing social concern effect. According to our results, social concern scores predicted a reduction of the social buffering effect in real-life study, even if individual differences in state and trait anxiety were taken into account (Tables S13, S14), highlighting the relevance of social concern for explaining individual differences in social buffering in females.
The differential effect of social concern in females and males supports previous evidence, suggesting that females are affected by social anxiety in a large variety of social situations [29, 30], while males are specifically affected in mating contexts, i.e., typically in the presence of a female [31]. Building upon these previous results, our findings showed that individual differences in social concern can account for gender differences in social buffering.
That said, our study showed significant social buffering in female participants in the presence of a female partner, raising the question whether the observed social buffering effect was driven by the presence of a female partner. Impression ratings were more positive for the female partner compared to the male partner, but did not account for the observed gender effect in social buffering. Given that the gender-related differences in impression ratings was the only difference between the social treatments with a female and a male partner, it is unlikely that the observed social buffering effects were mainly driven by the gender of the partner. However, future studies should systematically test for the effect of the partner’s sex by conducting the same social treatment conditions with partners from the opposite sex (i.e., to assess social buffering in females in the presence of a male partner and social buffering in males in the presence of a female partner).
When experiencing the aversive sound alone, females showed higher SCRs compared to males (Fig. 2A), consistent with previous findings indicating that females respond more strongly to aversive events than males [55,56,57]. However, this gender difference in SCRs in the alone treatment groups cannot account for the differential effect of social concern on SCRs in the social treatment groups between females and males in Study 1, and between real and virtual social presence (comparison of Study 1 and Study 2).
In more detail, the results of Study 2 showed that females also benefit from the presence of a virtual person, indicated by a reduction of fear-related SCRs in the presence of a virtual agent (Fig. 3A). In contrast to social buffering in the presence of a real person, the virtual social buffering effect occurred independently of individual social concern scores (Fig. 3B), indicating that a virtual agent can be a safety cue for females with high social concern. The finding of social buffering effects in the VR environment is in line with previous studies showing a reduction of fear responses [28] or social stress [27] if participants believed that the agent was controlled by a real person, such as a romantic partner [28].
Extending these studies, our results showed social buffering of fear in females in the mere presence of a virtual person, i.e., an agent that was facing away and that was not controlled by a human agent. According to their ratings on the NMMSP questionnaire [53], our participants perceived the virtual agent as present, but not as a social interaction partner. Despite this rather minimalistic virtual social treatment, we observed social buffering in females irrespective of individual differences in social concern. This finding is interesting in two ways. First, it bolsters the claim that at least in females the mere presence of another individual can buffer autonomic fear responses [9]. Second, our findings indicate that females scoring high on social concern can benefit from virtual social presence, although they show reduced social buffering in the presence of a real person.
In our study, we investigated social buffering of human fear in females and males in the presence of a real person and in the presence of a virtual representation of this person, using an identical paradigm. Based on this systematic approach, we could directly compare how social concern affects the social buffering of human fear across genders, and in different environments (real-life vs VR). On the theoretical level, our findings provide novel insights into individual differences that shape the social modulation of autonomic human fear responses. Our finding that the mere virtual presence of another person can attenuate autonomic fear responses in females independently of individual differences in social concern is of potential practical relevance for the treatment of anxiety disorders. In future studies, it would be interesting to contrast these effects with the social presence on the processing of positive stimuli.
Data availability
All Data and scripts of both studies are provided at https://osf.io/zcu67/?view_only=0d4449de234d45e99adfea1c18f22b26.
References
Fuzzo F, Matsumoto J, Kiyokawa Y, Takeuchi Y, Ono T, Nishijo H. Social buffering suppresses fear-associated activation of the lateral amygdala in male rats: behavioral and neurophysiological evidence. Front Neurosci. 2015;9:99.
Kiyokawa Y, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y. Two types of social buffering differentially mitigate conditioned fear responses. Eur J Neurosci 2007;26:3606–13.
Sterley TL, Baimoukhametova D, Fuzesi T, Zurek AA, Daviu N, Rasiah NP, et al. Social transmission and buffering of synaptic changes after stress. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21:393–403.
Loewen SP, Baimoukhametova DV, Bains JS. Sex-specific vasopressin signaling buffers stress-dependent synaptic changes in female mice. J Neurosci. 2020;40:8842–52.
Smith AS, Wang Z. Hypothalamic oxytocin mediates social buffering of the stress response. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;76:281–8.
Majumdar S, Lungwitz EA, Andrews KD, Chambers JE, Truitt WA. Animal Models to Investigate Social Support Induced Anxiety Reductions. In: Sangha S, Foti D, editors. Neurobiology of Abnormal Emotion and Motivated Behaviors. London: Academic Press; 2018. p. 224–40.
Kiyokawa Y, Hennessy MB. Comparative studies of social buffering: a consideration of approaches, terminology, and pitfalls. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;86:131–41.
Levy DR, Yizhar O. Stress and sociability. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21:304–6.
Qi Y, Herrmann MJ, Bell L, Fackler A, Han S, Deckert J, et al. The mere physical presence of another person reduces human autonomic responses to aversive sounds. Proc Biol Sci. 2020;287:20192241.
Pan Y, Olsson A, Golkar A. Social safety learning: shared safety abolishes the recovery of learned threat. Behav Res Ther. 2020;135:103733.
Eisenberger NI, Master SL, Inagaki TK, Taylor SE, Shirinyan D, Lieberman MD, et al. Attachment figures activate a safety signal-related neural region and reduce pain experience. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108:11721–6.
Hyde LW, Gorka A, Manuck SB, Hariri AR. Perceived social support moderates the link between threat-related amygdala reactivity and trait anxiety. Neuropsychologia 2011;49:651–6.
von Mohr M, Krahé C, Beck B, Fotopoulou A. The social buffering of pain by affective touch: a laser-evoked potential study in romantic couples. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2018;13:1121–30.
Ditzen B, Heinrichs M. Psychobiology of social support: the social dimension of stress buffering. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2014;32:149–62.
Cohen S, Wills TA. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychol Bull. 1985;98:310.
Hornstein EA, Eisenberger NI. A Social Safety Net: Developing a Model of Social-Support Figures as Prepared Safety Stimuli. Curr Directions Psychol Sci. 2018;27:25–31.
Hostinar CE, Sullivan RM, Gunnar MR. Psychobiological mechanisms underlying the social buffering of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis: A review of animal models and human studies across development. Psychol Bull. 2014;140:256.
Faustino AI, Tacão-Monteiro A, Oliveira RF. Mechanisms of social buffering of fear in zebrafish. Sci Rep. 2017;7:44329.
Spence SH, Rapee RM. The etiology of social anxiety disorder: an evidence-based model. Behav Res Ther. 2016;86:50–67.
Gu R, Ao X, Mo L, Zhang D. Neural correlates of negative expectancy and impaired social feedback processing in social anxiety. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2020;15:285–91.
Stegmann Y, Ahrens L, Pauli P, Keil A, Wieser MJ. Social aversive generalization learning sharpens the tuning of visuocortical neurons to facial identity cues. Elife. 2020;e55204:9.
Taylor S, Zvolensky MJ, Cox BJ, Deacon B, Heimberg RG, Ledley DR, et al. Robust dimensions of anxiety sensitivity: development and initial validation of the Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3. Psychol Assess. 2007;19:176–88.
Reiss S, Peterson RA, Gursky DM, McNally RJ. Anxiety sensitivity, anxiety frequency and the prediction of fearfulness. Behav Res Ther. 1986;24:1–8.
Reiss S. Expectancy model of fear, anxiety, and panic. Clin Psychol Rev. 1991;11:141–53.
McNally RJ. Anxiety sensitivity and panic disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52:938–46.
Taylor S. Anxiety sensitivity: Theory, research, and treatment of the fear of anxiety. Mahwah, N.J.: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1999.
Kothgassner OD, Goreis A, Kafka JX, Kaufmann M, Atteneder K, Beutl L, et al. Virtual social support buffers stress response: an experimental comparison of real-life and virtual support prior to a social stressor. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2019;63:57–65.
Kane HS, McCall C, Collins NL, Blascovich J. Mere presence is not enough: responsive support in a virtual world. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2012;48:37–44.
Asher M, Aderka IM. Gender differences in social anxiety disorder. J Clin Psychol. 2018;74:1730–41.
Asher M, Asnaani A, Aderka IM. Gender differences in social anxiety disorder: a review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2017;56:1–12.
Xu Y, Schneier F, Heimberg RG, Princisvalle K, Liebowitz MR, Wang S, et al. Gender differences in social anxiety disorder: results from the national epidemiologic sample on alcohol and related conditions. J Anxiety Disord. 2012;26:12–9.
Gutzeit VA, Ahuna K, Santos TL, Cunningham AM, Sadsad Rooney M, Munoz, et al. Optogenetic reactivation of prefrontal social neural ensembles mimics social buffering of fear. Neuropsychopharmacology 2020;45:1068–77.
Ishii A, Kiyokawa Y, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y. Social buffering ameliorates conditioned fear responses in female rats. Horm Behav. 2016;81:53–8.
Reddan MC, Young H, Falkner J, Lopez-Sola M, Wager TD. Touch and social support influence interpersonal synchrony and pain. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2020;15:1064–75.
Owens ME, Beidel DC. Can Virtual Reality Effectively Elicit Distress Associated with Social Anxiety Disorder? J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2014;37:296–305.
Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull. 1992;112:155–9.
Hautzinger M. Allgemeine Depressions-Skala: ADS. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz; 1993.
Zimet GD, Dahlem NW, Zimet SG, Farley GK. The multidimensional scale of perceived social support. J Personal Assess. 1988;52:30–41.
Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Personal Soc Psychol. 1988;54:1063.
Spielberger CD, Gorusch RL, Lushene RE. State-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consult. Psychol. Press; 1971.
Hein G, Engelmann JB, Vollberg MC, Tobler PN. How learning shapes the empathic brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2016;113:80–5.
Bradley M, Lang PJ. The International Affective Digitized Sounds (IADS): Stimuli, Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings. University of Florida: NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention; 1999.
Herrmann MJ, Boehme S, Becker MP, Tupak SV, Guhn A, Schmidt B, et al. Phasic and sustained brain responses in the amygdala and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis during threat anticipation. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37:1091–102.
Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. 2004;134:9–21.
Naqvi NH, Bechara A. Skin conductance: a psychophysiological approach to the study of decision making. In: Senior C, Russell T, Gazzaniga MS, editors. Methods in mind. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press; 2006. p. 103–22.
Figner B, Murphy RO. Using skin conductance in judgment and decision making research. In: Schulte-Mecklenbeck M, Kuehberger A, Ranyard R, editors. A handbook of process tracing methods for decision research. New York: Psychology Press; 2011. p. 163–84.
Damasio AR, Grabowski TJ, Bechara A, Damasio H, Ponto LL, Parvizi J, et al. Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:1049–56.
Bates D, Machler M, Bolker BM, Walker SC. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;67:1–48.
Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, Aiken LS. Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Psychology Press; 2014.
Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H, O’Hara RB. A general and simple method for obtainingR2from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods Ecol Evol. 2013;4:133–42.
Barton K, Barton MK. Package ‘MuMIn’. Version. 2015;1:18.
Lenth R, Singmann H, Love J, Buerkner P, Herve M. Emmeans: estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. R Package Version. 2018;1:3.
Biocca F, Harms C, Gregg J. The networked minds measure of social presence: Pilot test of the factor structure and concurrent validity. 4th annual international workshop; Philadelphia, PA2001. p. 1–9.
Achenbach J, Waltemate T, Latoschik ME, Botsch M. Fast generation of realistic virtual humans. Proceedings of the 23rd ACM Symposium on Virtual Reality Software and Technology; Gothenburg, Sweden 2017. p. 1–10.
Day AL, Livingstone HA. Gender differences in perceptions of stressors and utilization of social support among university students. Can J Behav Sci. 2003;35:73.
Ali N, Nitschke JP, Cooperman C, Baldwin MW, Pruessner JC. Systematic manipulations of the biological stress systems result in sex-specific compensatory stress responses and negative mood outcomes. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020:1–11.
McLean CP, Anderson ER. Brave men and timid women? A review of the gender differences in fear and anxiety. Clin Psychol Rev. 2009;29:496–505.
Acknowledgements
We thank Tomonori Nakayama for help with data analysis. This work was supported by the German Research Foundation [HE 4566/5-1 to GH], the Sino-German (CSC-DAAD) Postdoc Scholarship Program [No. 201804910929 to YQ], and the National Natural Science Foundation of China [32000733 to YQ]. This publication was supported by the Open Access Publication Fund of the University of Wuerzburg.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Conceptualization: YQ, DB, PK, MEL, JD, GH; Methodology: YQ, DB, PK, MJH, GH; Formal Analysis: YQ, MJH; Investigation: YQ, DB, PK; Writing of the Original Draft: YQ, GH; Writing—Review & Editing: YQ, DB, PK, MJH, MEL, JD, GH; Funding Acquisition: YQ, GH. All authors gave final approval for publication.
Corresponding authors
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
JD is an investigator in an EU-funded study by P1Vital and corecipient of a grant of the Bavarian state government to BioVariance. All other co-authors reported no biomedical financial interests or potential competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Qi, Y., Bruch, D., Krop, P. et al. Social buffering of human fear is shaped by gender, social concern, and the presence of real vs virtual agents. Transl Psychiatry 11, 641 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-021-01761-5
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-021-01761-5
This article is cited by
-
Differential effects of everyday-life social support on chronic pain
BMC Neurology (2024)
-
The effects of everyday-life social interactions on anxiety-related autonomic responses differ between men and women
Scientific Reports (2023)