Introduction

With the consistent improvement in living standards, human life expectancy has increased. Some studies have shown that the prevalence of dementia is also appears to be gradually increasing1,2. Dementia is a complex neurological disorder with an irreversible and nonlinear development process. Dementia does not have only a variety of complex causes, but also will brings with many other health problems. Currently, dementia is the fourth leading cause of death following cancer, heart disease, and cerebrovascular disease in the elderly3,4. According to the International Association of Alzheimer’s Disease International Statistics Annual Report 2011, ~36 million people worldwide currently experience dementia, and the incidence is increasing at a rate of 1 patient every 7 s. It is estimated that the number of individuals experiencing senile dementia will increase to 66 million by 2030 and reach 115 million by 20505. Studies have shown that reducing the modifiable risk factors of dementia may contribute to the prevention and control of dementia6,7; hence, identifying its possible risk factors is significantly important.

In recent years, there has been a growing interest in physical exercise as a non-pharmacological treatment for dementia8,9. Certainly, some studies have reported that regular physical exercise can favorably affect the physical and cognitive functions of patients with dementia10,11. Conversely, physical inactivity and a sedentary lifestyle are considered the risk factors of dementia. Studies assessing the association between sedentary behavior and the occurrence of dementia have increased in recent years. However, until now, there have been no consistent conclusions regarding the association between sedentary behavior and the risk of dementia. To the best of our knowledge, the risk of dementia and sedentary lifestyle has not yet been evaluated by a meta-analysis, which is a generally accepted statistical tool for combining results to produce a more precise estimation of associations in different studies12,13. Therefore, we conducted a meta-analysis of 18 cohort studies to evaluate the association between sedentary behavior and the risk of dementia. Considering the significant health and economic burden of dementia, the results of our study may provide additional practical and valuable treatments for dementia prevention.

Materials and methods

Approval by institutional review boards is not required for this systematic review of previously published de-identified data.

Literature search strategy

We conducted this meta-analysis in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses14 and the checklist of items in the Meta-Analysis Of Observational Studies in Epidemiology15. We performed a comprehensive search on PubMed, Embase, and Web of Science databases from their inception to March 2018 for cohort studies published in peer-reviewed journals describing an association between sedentary behavior and the risk of dementia. We used the following keywords to identify relevant citations: “sedentary” or “sedentariness” or “long time sitting” in combination with “dementia” or “Alzheimer’s disease” or “AD” or “cogniti”. Only articles published in the English language were considered. In addition, reference lists of the retrieved original articles and relevant review articles were also comprehensively examined to identify further pertinent studies.

Study selection

Studies meeting the following criteria were included in the meta-analysis: (1) the study design was cohort; (2) sedentary behavior was the exposure variable and the outcome was the incidence of dementia; (3) the study reported the relative risks (RRs) with the corresponding 95% confidence intervals (CIs) of dementia associated with sedentary behavior or included the data needed for their calculation. Animal studies, clinical trials, reviews, letters, and commentaries were excluded. Only studies with detailed information on both sedentary behavior and the incidence of dementia were included.

Data extraction and quality assessment

Two authors (S.Y. and W.F.) independently extracted data in a standardized fashion. The following data were included: first author, publication year, country (state), age, sample size, cases, adjusted factors, and adjusted RR with 95% CI. Discrepancies were resolved by a discussion with a third author (L.Z.). Adjusted RRs were selected over the unadjusted risk estimates. In cases where multiple risk estimates were reported in the same study, for example, when the study introduced risk estimates for 3 different years, those risk estimates were included as separate risk estimates.

Quality assessment was performed according to the Newcastle-Ottawa quality assessment scale16, which is a validated scale for nonrandomized studies in meta-analyses. This scale assigns a maximum of nine points to each study: four for the selection of participants and measurement of exposure, two for comparability of cohorts or cases and controls based on the design or analysis, and three for the assessment of outcomes and adequacy of follow-up. We assigned scores of 0–3, 3.5–6, and 6.5–9 for low-, moderate-, and high-quality studies, respectively. When the studies had several adjustment models, we extracted those that reflected the maximum extent of adjustment for potentially confounding variables. Disagreement between the two authors’ independent data analysis was resolved through a review by the third author.

Statistical analyses

The RR was considered as the common measure of the association between sedentary behavior and the risk of dementia. When RRs were reported separately for subgroups by the different sedentary times in one study, the fixed-effects model was used to combine the results of the subgroups and calculate a common RR for the main analysis12. In addition, the random-effects model was used to calculate an overall pooled RR for the main analysis.

Heterogeneity was identified according to Cochran’s Q test, with P < 0.10 indicating heterogeneity. The I2 statistics measured the percentage of total variation through studies due to heterogeneity rather than chance. It was calculated according to the formula by Higgins17. I2 was used to quantify the heterogeneity, with 25%, 50%, and 75% indicating low, moderate, and high degrees of heterogeneity, respectively.

Subgroup analyses were performed to determine the possible influence of factors such as state or region and weather. The concomitant effects of age, sex, and educational level were controlled in some studies. The Begg’s rank correlation and the Egger’s linear regression tests were used to assess potential publication bias18,19. Moreover, the Duval and Tweedie’s nonparametric trim-and-fill method were used to adjust potential publication bias20. All analyses were performed using the Stata statistical software (version 12.0; College Station, TX, USA), and all tests were two-sided with a significance level of 0.05.

Results

Literature search

A total of 3617 articles were searched from the electronic databases, and 2215 articles were removed because of duplication. Another 1402 unqualified records were excluded after browsing the titles or abstracts. Among these, 1269 papers reported irrelevant studies, which did not analyze the association between sedentary behavior and the risk of developing dementia, 88 were non-cohort studies, and 27 articles offered insufficient information. After retrieving and reviewing the full text, we determined that 18 cohort studies met our inclusion criteria21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38. The study selection procedure is presented in the flow chart in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow chart of study identification.

Characteristics of the included studies

Table 1 shows the main characteristics of the 18 cohort studies included in the systematic review. The 18 cohort studies were published between 1984 and 2018. Most of these studies (11) were conducted in Europe (RR = 1.37, 95% CI = 1.14–1.65), four in America (RR = 1.52, 95% CI = 1.30–1.77), two in Asia (RR = 0.76, 95% CI = 0.52–1.11), and one in Australia (RR = 0.98, 95% CI = 0.89–1.08). The sample size of these studies ranged from 33 to 490 with a total of 250,063 participants. Seventeen studies were published after 2000, and only one study was published before 2000. The quality assessment scores ranged from 6 to 9, with an average score of 7 points, representing satisfactory quality of the studies.

Table 1 Main characteristics of the included studies involving sedentary behavior and the risk of dementia.

Results of meta-analysis

Association between sedentary behavior and the risk of dementia

Figure 2 shows the results from the random-effects model combining the RRs for dementia in relation to sedentary behavior. Eighteen studies investigated the association between sedentary behavior and risk of dementia. The pooled RR of dementia for sedentary behavior was 1.30 (95% CI, 1.12–1.51), with a substantial heterogeneity across studies (P = 0.000, I2 = 66.9%).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Forest plot of sedentary behavior associated with dementia.

Results of subgroup and sensitivity analyses

Subgroup analyses

Table 2 shows the results of subgroup analyses. To assess the stability of the primary estimates outcomes and identify the potential resources of heterogeneity, we performed subgroup analyses. Subgroup analyses by state, controlling for the concomitant effects of age, sex, and educational level, were performed to assess the increased risk of dementia as a result of a sedentary behavior. In general, these subgroup analyses showed no statistically significant difference since the pooled results of all subgroups showed a positive and statistically significant association between sedentary behavior and the risk of dementia.

Table 2 Results of subgroup analyses about sedentary behavior and the risk of dementia.

Sensitivity analyses

We performed sensitivity analyses to determine the potential sources of heterogeneity in the association between sedentary behavior and risk of dementia, examine the influence of various exclusions on the combined RR, and assess the robustness of all results. We compared the fixed-effect and random-effect models, but found no significant difference in the pooled RRs between the two (fixed-effects model pooled RR = 1.16 [95% CI, 1.09–1.24], random-effects model pooled RR = 1.30 [95% CI, 1.12–1.51]). In addition, each study was excluded in turn, and the results of the remaining studies were pooled together. The pooled RR did not significantly change, ranging from 1.27 (95% CI, 1.10–1.47) to 1.34 (95% CI, 1.16–1.56), and the heterogeneity was detected with an I2 = 65.4% and 66.3%, respectively.

Publication bias

Visual inspection of the funnel plot showed significant asymmetry (Fig. 3). The Egger and Begg test indicated no evidence of publication bias (Egger, P = 0.677; Begg, P = 0.074). The trim-and-fill method was used to evaluate the impact of any potential publication bias, and the results showed that two potentially missing studies would be required to obtain the funnel plot symmetry for the association between sedentary behavior and the risk of dementia (Fig. 4). After using the trim-and-fill method, the corrected RR was 1.27 (95% CI, 1.10–.47; random-effects model, P = 0.000), suggesting that the pooled RR did not significantly change by the correction for potential publication bias.

Fig. 3: Funnel plot for studies of sedentary behavior and dementia.
figure 3

The horizontal line represents summary effect estimates, and two slashes lines are pseudo 95% CIs.

Fig. 4: Filled funnel plot of RR from studies that investigated the association between sedentary behavior and the risk of dementia.
figure 4

The horizontal line represents summary effect estimates, and two slashes lines are pseudo 95% CIs.

Discussion

With the rapid development of information technology, people are able to receive essential information without leaving their homes. Artificial intelligence and mobile terminals (such as mobile phones and laptops) enable individuals to interact with other individuals worldwide in a static or sedentary manner. Therefore, sedentary lifestyle has been considered to be significantly prevalent in the current society, specifically for office workers, students, and drivers. Previous reviews have suggested that sedentary behavior was associated with lower cognitive performance, although the attributable risk of sedentary time to all-cause dementia incidence is unclear39,40. However, the meta-analysis of 18 cohort studies involving 250,063 participants and 2269 patients confirmed the positive association between sedentary behavior and the risk of dementia. Compared with individuals who had not been exposed to sedentary behavior, individuals who were exposed to sedentary behavior had a 30% higher risk of experiencing dementia. Sedentary behavior was associated with several chronic diseases that were also associated with cognitive impairment and risk of dementia41,42. Previous data suggested that prolonged sedentary time could impair glucose and lipid metabolism43,44,45, which were recognized as the risk factors for cognitive decline and all-cause dementia46,47. In addition, inflammation was also identified as a potential risk factor for dementia48,49. Meanwhile, sedentary behavior might conversely induce or aggravate individual inflammation50,51,52. Finally, exercise was considered a protective factor for dementia53. However, physical activity declines with age, while sedentary behavior increases with age54,55. Voss and his colleagues found that increases in sedentary behavior and sedentary time were significantly observed in individuals who were about to retire and were continuously observed in adults after the age of 60 years, and individuals aged over 80 years are often involved in sedentary behavior, with an average sedentary time of 9 h per day56, eliminating the benefits of physical exercises on individual’s cognitive health.

Considering that a substantial heterogeneity was observed in the included studies, we further performed a subgroup analysis to determine the potential sources of heterogeneity. Based on the result of the subgroup analyses, we found that the pooled odds ratios of the studies conducted in Asia and Australia showed no statistically significant association between sedentary behavior and the risk of dementia. The probable explanation was that only two studies from Asia and one from Australia were included in this meta-analysis. Their sample sizes were small and lacked representation, thereby potentially leading to bias in the results57, indicating the need for additional studies in Asia and Australia to determine significant robust results. When the analysis was stratified to control the concomitant effects of age, sex, and educational level, we found that the summarized results from the original studies without adjusting for age, sex, and educational level showed no statistically significant difference in the association between sedentary behavior and risk of dementia (P > 0.05). On the contrary, if the original studies were controlled for age, sex, and educational level, the merged RRs were all statistically significant (P < 0.05), although the difference between the pooled results in each subgroup was not statistically significant (P > 0.05). Specifically, aging was an independent suggested risk factor for developing dementia58,59. Our subgroup analysis suggested that age might be a confounding factor in the association between sedentary lifestyle and risk of dementia, consistent with the results when sex and educational level were adjusted. To generalize the findings, more studies determining the association between age, sex, and educational level and sedentary lifestyle and their synergistic effects on the risk of dementia were required.

To assess the consistency and robustness of the results from the primary analysis and determine the potential sources of heterogeneity in the association between sedentary behavior and the risk of dementia, we performed a sensitivity analysis. However, the pooled results showed only minor changes ranging from 1.27 (95% CI, 1.10–1.47) to 1.34 (95% CI, 1.16–1.56) after excluding each study in turn from the analysis, highlighting the robustness and reliability of the primary result. On the contrary, publication bias was observed in the included studies, but the primary result did not significantly change after using the “trim-and-fill” method, significantly confirming the robustness of our findings.

This meta-analysis has several strengths. First, the study design of all the studies in our analysis was cohort, which was considered as a stronger measure when demonstrating causation and identifying risk factors than other observational study designs60. Second, consistent results from the sensitivity analysis indicated that our findings were reliable and robust, although heterogeneity was observed among the studies. Third, when several RRs were introduced separately in terms of the different sedentary times, we combined the results of these subgroups and calculated a common result using the fixed-effects model. Therefore, we could pool the outcomes regarding the association between the risk of dementia and sedentary behavior with non-sedentary behavior.

On the contrary, this meta-analysis has several limitations that may affect the interpretation of the results in our analysis. First, the heterogeneity of the included studies was significant (I2 > 60%) and was observed throughout the analyses. However, we determined the potential sources of heterogeneity by performing subgroup analyses and sensitivity analysis. Second, considering the limited information provided in original studies, a dose–response analysis was not performed to provide further evidence supporting the association between sedentary behavior and the risk of dementia. Third, the definition of “sedentary behavior” was inconsistent, which might induce bias in effect size estimates. Fourth, the outcome we were interested in was all-cause dementia. Considering that the information related to the subtypes of dementia was insufficient, we did not analyze the effects of sedentary lifestyle on specific dementia subtypes, such as vascular dementia and senile dementia.

Conclusion

The results of this meta-analysis of cohort studies with the most up-to-date evidence suggested that sedentary behavior was significantly associated with an increased risk of dementia. The positive association remained consistent throughout the analysis. Considering the increasing prevalence of sedentary behavior in modern society, our findings have been significant for policy makers and health education institutions. Considering the insufficient information on the dose–response association of sedentary time and the risk of developing dementia, well-designed studies with adequate analyses of sedentary time and the risk of dementia are required to confirm our findings.