Introduction

Within the opioid system, the mu opioid receptor (MOR) plays a key role in mediating the rewarding properties of natural and artificial stimuli such as food or drugs of abuse1,2. Stemming from the pioneer work of Panksepp and colleagues3, evidence from imaging, experimental psychology, and behavioral pharmacology have uncovered how MOR activation also underpins social reward and motivation in humans and animals and, consequently, modulates varieties of social behaviors (sexual and affiliative behaviors, bonding, social play, social exploration)4,5,6,7,8. Consistent with this, genetic knockout of MOR (Oprm1−/−) in mice produces severe alterations of their social repertoire, from early life to adult age9,10,11,12, further demonstrating that MOR is essential for establishing appropriate social behavior. Oprm1−/− animals were recently proposed to model autism spectrum disorder (ASD)9,13, a heterogeneous group of neurodevelopmental diseases whose diagnosis lies on the detection of two types of core symptoms: deficient social reciprocity and communication together with restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior14,15. Remarkably, not only MOR null mice recapitulate all these core symptoms but they also display multiple behavioral and physiological abnormalities frequently associated to ASD7,9,14, proving unique face validity for this model16. Moreover, these animals show several neurobiological landmarks of the disease, such as altered striatal function, decreased activation of the reward circuit in response to social stimuli or reduced oxytocin in the nucleus accumbens9,11,17,18,19,20,21 that, together with the identification of ASD patients bearing mutations in the OPRM1 gene7, demonstrate construct validity for this model. Given the key role of MOR in modulating reward, these data suggest, beyond a potential contribution of MOR dysfunction in ASD, that altered reward processes may represent a key mechanism underlying ASD etiopathogeny. Interestingly, a growing body of literature points to reward deficits in patients with ASD7,18, in agreement with the social motivation theory of autism proposing that disrupted social interest in these patients would result primarily from early deficits in their motivation for attending, enjoying and prolonging social interactions22,23.

If compromised reward processes were to account for behavioral deficits in ASD, a straightforward consequence would be that facilitating these processes in patients should represent an efficient tool to relieve autistic symptoms. To date, available pharmacological treatments for ASD mostly target associated symptoms24,25 and evidence-based behavioral interventions remain the only treatments that proved to ameliorate core symptoms26,27,28,29. Strikingly, the most widely used and longest standing intervention models, Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention (EIBI) programs, based on applied behavioral analysis (ABA), use various behavioral techniques relying on positive reinforcement as levers to shape behavior30,31,32,33,34. Concretely, EIBI programs break desirable behaviors (direct eye gaze or speech for example) down into steps and reward success at each step; conversely, these therapies may resort to punishment to discourage behaviors considered as inappropriate (tantrums for example). Directly derived from ABA-based EIBI, novel interventions have recently emerged (such as the Early Start Denver Model, enhanced Milieu teaching, Pivotal Response Treatment, parent-implemented programs), often play-based, in which the intervention is more child-directed and occurs in “naturalistic” environments, to facilitate generalization to the child’s everyday life35,36,37,38,39,40. A common feature of all these interventions remains the use of direct positive reinforcers to promote behavioral improvements, whereas punishment is mostly discarded29,36,41. Reinforcers are often edible or tangible items, such as palatable food, preferred by patients with ASD over more social reinforcement, like praise42,43. Thus, evidence-based behavioral intervention involves stimulating the reward system to increase the occurrence of appropriate behavior, particularly within the social repertoire, which is in accordance with a reward hypothesis in ASD.

Although they have demonstrated their efficacy, behavioral intervention programs show some limitations. Indeed, these programs are highly demanding to children and their families: they need to be intensive (20–40 h per week) and ought to start at the youngest possible age;44,45,46 however, their outcome remains uncertain. Therapists are seeking experimental evidence and biomarkers to identify key elements predictive of positive results for each patient. In this context, transposing the basic principles of behavioral intervention to mouse models offers a unique opportunity to decipher their neurobiological underpinnings. Here, we back-translated such intervention to Oprm1−/− mice, as a model of ASD with demonstrated reward deficiency, to assess whether associating positive reinforcement to social experience in these animals could rescue their impaired social abilities (and, eventually, other behavioral deficits), which would argue for a crucial contribution of reward processes in the beneficial effects of behavioral intervention. Moreover, we investigated gene expression in key brain regions for reward processing and social behavior in these animals to better delineate the molecular substrates of these effects.

Materials and methods

Animals, housing conditions, and breeding procedures

Male and female Oprm1+/+ and Oprm1−/− mice47 were bred in-house on an identical hybrid background: 50% 129SVPas - 50% C57BL/6 J. Oprm1+/+ and Oprm1−/− pups were bred from homozygous parents, as we previously showed that parental care has no influence on behavioral phenotype in these animals (cross-fostering experiments, see ref. 9). Homozygous parents, however, were bred from heterozygous animals, to prevent genetic derivation. This breeding scheme may have potentiated behavioral deficits in mutant animals by maintaining them together during early postnatal development. Except otherwise stated, animals were group-housed and maintained on a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 AM) at controlled temperature (21 ± 1 °C); food and water were available ad libitum. Experiments were analyzed blind to genotypes and experimental condition. All experimental procedures were conducted in accordance with the European Communities Council Directive 2010/63/EU and approved by the Comité d’Ethique pour l’Expérimentation Animale de l’ICS et de l’IGBMC (Com’Eth, 2012-033) and Comité d’Ethique en Expérimentation animale Val de Loire (C2EA-19).

Behavioral experiments

Behavioral training protocol

Equivalent numbers of naive male and female animals were used in each group. Female mice were not synchronized for estrous cycle. Experiments started when mice were 6-week old, in an attempt to mimic early intervention conditions. Caregivers in EIBI programs initially define several target behaviors to work on; here we focused on social interaction. Animals were randomly distributed across four experimental conditions before behavioral assays had started: object interaction-reinforced (OI-R), social interaction-nonreinforced (SI-NR), social interaction-reinforced (SI-R), and No Training control (NoT) (see details below and in Fig. 1a). We performed behavioral testing in three steps (time line in Fig. 1b).

Fig. 1: Experimental conditions and time line.
figure 1

a Oprm1+/+ and Oprm1−/− animals were randomly distributed across four experimental conditions: object interaction-reinforced (OI-R: food reward was offered to the animals after exposure to an unfamiliar object), social interaction-nonreinforced (SI-NR: exposure to an unfamiliar wild-type congener was not followed by food presentation), social interaction-reinforced (SI-R: social encounter was followed by the favorite food reinforcer–peanut butter or condensed milk) and no training control (NoT, animals were not manipulated between pre- and post-training behavioral assessments and did not received palatable food). b All animals undergone pre-training assays during postnatal (PN) week 6, then behavioral training for 3 weeks (except for the NoT condition) before post-training assessments on PN weeks 10 to 12. Half of the SI-NR and SI-R animals were retested for social interaction after cessation of training on PN weeks 17 and 34. Half of the OI-R, SI-NR, and SI-R animals were trained 3 more days and killed 45 min after the beginning of an ultimate social interaction session (no food reinforcement) for qRT-PCR study

Pre-training tests

To match clinical conditions48, we first evaluated which reinforcer was preferred in our animals between two highly palatable diets: condensed milk or peanut butter. This allowed us to determine the type of palatable food reinforcer to use in further experiments for each animal. Preference was measured in a Y-maze, and exploration pattern during habituation was used to assess perseverative behavior. We evaluated social abilities using the direct social interaction test (postnatal—PN–week 6, see supplemental information for detailed protocols).

Behavioral training

Behavioral training started on PN week 7 and lasted 3 weeks. In the SI-R condition (experimental group), mutant and wild-type mice interacted 5 days a week with a wild-type unfamiliar conspecific, different every day, for 5 min during the first 2 weeks, for 8 min during the 3rd week. The interactor was then removed from the arena, whereas the experimental animal remained another 5 (or 8 min) and received a  3 g unit of its favorite food reward. The amount of food was weighted before and after the test to measure consumption. In a first control group, the SI-NR group, training was performed under the same conditions except that no food reward was available in the arenas following social interaction. In SI-NR and SI-R groups, the time spent in nose contacts and their duration were measured during the course of training to assess the evolution of social behavior (days 2, 4, 8, 12, and 14). During the two last training sessions, some male mice (Oprm1+/+ and Oprm1−/−) from the SI-R group developed aggressive behavior, maybe a territorial response owing to repetitive food presentation in the arena. In a second control group, the OI-R group, food reward was offered to the animals after exposure to an unfamiliar object (different every day) instead of a mouse. The amount of food consumed was measured. Finally, in a no training control group (NoT), the animals were tested on PN weeks 6, 10, and 11 under the same conditions as in the other groups, but were not manipulated during PN weeks 7–9.

Post-training tests

Beginning on PN week 10, we performed multiple assays to assess the consequences of behavioral training. Social abilities were explored using the direct social interaction (between unfamiliar animals of the same genotype and experimental group) and three-chamber tests49. Stereotyped/perseverative behavior was assessed by scoring motor stereotypies50, monitoring alternation in a Y-maze51, and assessing anxiety-induced marble burying52. Anxiety was evaluated in the novelty suppressed feeding test53 (testing order in Figure S1). Detailed behavioral protocols are described in Supplemental information.

In cohorts dedicated to qRT-PCR analysis (half of the OI-R, SI-NR, and SI-R cohorts), animals were submitted to three additional days of behavioral training (week 12), and killed 45 min after the beginning of an additional social interaction session without food presentation (Fig. 1b). To assess the maintenance of training effects over the time, we submitted the other half of the SI-NR and SI-R cohorts to a direct social interaction test on PN weeks 17 and 24 (detailed protocol in Supplemental information).

Real-time quantitative PCR analysis

Brains were removed and placed into a brain matrix (ASI Instruments, Warren, MI, USA). Caudate putamen (CPu), nucleus accumbens (NAc), central amygdala (CeA), and ventral tegmental area/substancia nigra pars compacta (VTA/SNc) were punched out, whereas prefrontal cortex (PFC) and medial amygdala (MeA) were dissected from 1 mm-thick slices (see Figure S2). Tissues were immediately frozen on dry ice and kept at − 80 °C until use. For each structure of interest, genotype, and condition, samples were prepared from three male and three female mice and processed individually (n = 6). RNA was extracted and purified using the MIRNeasy mini-kit (Qiagen, Courtaboeuf, France). cDNA was synthetized using the first-strand Superscript II kit (Invitrogen®, Life Technologies, Saint Thomas, France). qRT-PCR was performed as previously described9. Primer sequences are displayed in Table S1.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using Statistica 9.0 software (StatSoft, Maisons-Alfort, France). All data were initially checked for normality of distribution using Kolmogorov–Smirnov’s test of normality. For all comparisons, values of p < 0.05 were considered as significant. Statistical significance in behavioral experiments was assessed using three- to four-way analysis of variance (genotype, gender, condition, and training effects) followed by Newman–Keuls post hoc test. Variance was similar between compared groups. We defined sample size (GPower 3.1) to ensure sufficient statistical power using ANOVA to detect significant effect of our parameters (effect size f = 1.80, α = 0.05, σ = 5, n = 8, power = 0.96). Significance of qRT-PCR results was assessed after transformation using a one-sample t test, as previously described9. A standard principal component analysis (PCA) was performed on behavioral and qRT-PCR data9. Loadings for each extracted principal component (PC) are quoted in Table S2. We considered the two first extracted PCs (PC1 and PC2) for schematic representation.

Results

Before behavioral training, mice lacking the MOR show perseverative behavior in the Y-maze and severe deficit in social interaction

We assessed social behavior using the direct social interaction test in mice belonging to the four experimental groups (NoT, OI-R, SI-NR, SI-R) before behavioral intervention. The sex of animals had no significant influence on these parameters. All Oprm1−/− mice, whatever the group they belonged to, displayed deficient social interaction, as evidenced by decreased time spent in nose contact (NC–genotype effect: F1,125 = 379.2, p < 0.0001), lower number of NC (F1,125 = 99.2, p < 0.0001), decreased mean duration of NC (F1,125 = 227.0, p < 0.0001), and reduced number of following episodes (F1,125 = 204.3, p < 0.0001). Moreover, mutant animals groomed more than wild-type controls (F1,125 = 21.3, p < 0.01), especially following a social contact (F1,125 = 252.3, p < 0.0001), a sign of social discomfort9,54 (Fig. 2a, statistics in Table S3). We evaluated perseverative behavior before behavioral training by recording the animal’s pattern of exploration in the Y-maze (Fig. 2b). Oprm1−/− animals from all groups displayed lower rates of spontaneous alternation than Oprm1+/+ animals (genotype: F1,125 = 26.2, p < 0.0001), consistent with an increased number of perseverative same arm returns in mutant mice (F1,125 = 23.3, p < 0.0001). Finally, we measured preference for condensed milk over peanut butter when these reinforcers were made available in the Y-maze. In this test, similar numbers of Oprm1+/+ and Oprm1−/− animals preferred one over the other reinforcer (percentage of animals preferring condensed milk: Oprm1+/+ 50.69%; Oprm1−/− 50.13%, Fig. 2c).

Fig. 2: Oprm1−/− mice display severe social interaction deficit and altered pattern of Y-maze alteration before behavioral training.
figure 2

a Before behavioral training, mutant mice (solid bars) in all groups displayed a severe deficit in social interaction when compared with wild-type animals (open bars), as evidenced by decreased time spent in nose contact (NC) with a congener of the same genotype, decreased number of NC and reduced duration of each NC, diminished number of following episodes and increased number of grooming episodes, notably after a social contact. b Oprm1−/− mice showed decreased rates of spontaneous alternation (SPA) and increased rates of same arm returns (SAR) when exploring the Y-maze, independently from training condition. c An equal number of mice preferred condensed milk and peanut butter over the other palatable food reinforcer among Oprm1+/+ and Oprm1−/− animals (OI-R and SI-R groups together). Each animal received its favorite food during training phase. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. Animal numbers per genotype and gender: NoT condition, n = 10; OI-R condition, n = 8; SI-NR condition, n = 8; SI-R condition, n = 10–11. Open stars: genotype effect (three-way ANOVA with one repeated measure—type of alternation), solid stars: genotype × condition interaction, comparison with wild-type animals trained under the same condition (three-way ANOVA followed by Newman–Keules post hoc test). One symbol: p < 0.05, two symbols: p < 0.01, three symbols: p < 0.001. NoT: no training; OI-R: object interaction-reinforced; SI-NR: social interaction-non-reinforced; SI-R: social interaction-reinforced

Palatable food intake and social interaction increased during the course of behavioral training in Oprm1 +/+ and Oprm1 −/− mice

Over the course of training, Oprm1+/+ and Oprm1−/− animals increased their intake of palatable food, with mutant mice consuming less (genotype effect: F1,64 = 10.3, p < 0.01; training effect: F14,386 = 6.2, p < 0.0001) (Figure S3a), especially females (Figure S3b). To assess the evolution of social behavior during training, we monitored (SI-NR and SI-R groups) the time spent in and number of NC, and calculated the mean duration of NC. The three parameters increased over time in both groups (training: time in NC–F4,70 = 12.1, p < 0.0001; number of NC–F4,70 = 5.4, p < 0.001; duration of NC: F4,70 = 15.4, p < 0.0001) with NC in the SI-R group being more numerous than in the SI-NR group (condition: F1,70 = 6.1, p < 0.05) than in the SI-R group (Figure S4a, statistics in Table S4). These two parameters, however, did not differ between genotypes, maybe because wild-type interactors initiated most of the social contacts during these sessions. The mean duration of NC, however, was shorter in Oprm1−/− animals than in Oprm1+/+ controls, and increased more significantly under the SI-R than the SI-NR condition (genotype: F1,70 = 13.0, p < 0.001; condition: F1,70 = 10.6, p < 0.01; training × genotype: F4,70 = 2.6, p < 0.05). Gender had little influence on these parameters (Figure S4b). Thus, daily exposure to an unfamiliar congener increased the number and duration of NC in mice, especially when this exposure was reinforced with food.

Behavioral intervention (SI-R) durably relieved social interaction deficit in Oprm1 −/− mice

We assessed social behavior in mice submitted to the four different conditions of behavioral training using two assays: the direct social interaction test and the three-chamber social preference test. In the former, Oprm1−/− mice from the NoT and OI-R groups still displayed a marked deficit in social interaction as compared to their wild-type controls, although the number of grooming episodes, including these occurring after social contact, was reduced in OI-R versus NoT mutant animals. Knockout mice from the SI-NR group showed restored number of NC as compared with their Oprm1+/+ controls, increased time spent in NC and mean duration of NC and decreased total number of grooming episodes when compared with knockout animals of the NoT group. However, the number of following episodes and grooming episodes after social contact were unchanged as compared with this control. Finally, all interaction parameters were normalized to wild-type levels in male and female Oprm1−/− mice trained under the SI-R condition (genotype × treatment: time in NC–F3,125 = 28.0, p < 0.0001; number of NC–F3,125 = 18.1, p < 0.0001; duration of NC–F3,125 = 24.6, p < 0.0001; following—F3,125 = 18.3, p < 0.0001; number of grooming episodes—F3,125 = 13.0, p < 0.01; grooming after social contact—F3,125 = 40.7, p < 0.0001; Fig. 3a, more parameters and sex effects in Figure S5, statistics in Table S5). We further tested social interaction in half of the SI-NR and SI-R cohorts 7 and 14 weeks after cessation of training and observed a persistence of the beneficial effects of behavioral training in mutants from the SI-R but not the SI-NR group, as illustrated by maintained normalization of the time in NC. Interestingly, 14 weeks after cessation of training time in NC was also higher in Oprm1+/+ mice from the SI-R group compared with SI-NR (training × genotype × condition: F3,84 = 13.6, p < 0.0001) (Fig. 3b, more parameters and sex effects in Figure S6, statistics in Table S6).

Fig. 3: Behavioral intervention (SI-R) durably relieved social interaction deficit in Oprm1−/− mice.
figure 3

a When retested after behavioral training in the direct social interaction test, Oprm1−/− animals (solid bars) from the NoT and OI-R groups displayed similar severe deficit as before training. Knockout mice from the SI-NR group showed restored number of NC as compared to their Oprm1+/+ controls (open bars) and increased time spent in NC and mean duration of NC when compared with Oprm1−/− animals of the NoT group, but their number of following episodes and grooming episodes, including after social contact, were unchanged as compared with this control. Finally, all previous interaction parameters were normalized to wild-type levels in Oprm1−/− mice trained under the SI-R condition. Genotype × condition interaction, solid stars: comparison with wild-type animals treated under the same condition; asterisks: comparison with mutant animals treated under the other conditions (three-way ANOVA followed by Newman–Keules post hoc test). b The increase in time spent in NC was maintained up to 24 weeks after complete cessation of behavioral training in knockout mice from the SI-R and not SI-NR group. Oprm1+/+ animals trained under the SI-R condition also maintained greater levels of social interaction over time than their SI-NR counterparts did. See more parameters in Figure S4. Genotype × condition interaction, Solid stars: compared with wild-type animals trained under the same condition; asterisks: Oprm1+/+ SI-R compared with Oprm1+/+ SI-NR (four-way ANOVA with week of testing as a repeated measure, followed by Newman–Keules post hoc test). c In the three-chamber test, Oprm1+/+ and Oprm1−/− mice from all experimental groups spent more time in the chamber with the mouse and made more contacts with the mouse than with the toy. Wild-type mice, however, spent significantly more time in close contact with the mouse than with the toy, each contact lasting longer with the mouse than with the toy. In contrast, Oprm1−/− mice made longer contacts with the toy and thus displayed no preference for spending time in contact with the mouse, except in the SI-R group. Consequently, preference ratio was markedly reduced in mutant mice as compared with wild-type, except for Oprm1−/− mice trained under the SI-R condition, that displayed similar social preference as Oprm1+/+ animals. Solid stars: genotype × condition interaction, comparison with wild-type animals treated under the same condition; open stars: genotype effect (four-way ANOVA with one repeated measure: stimulus—mouse versus toy followed by Newman–Keules post hoc test). Data are presented as mean ± SEM. Animal numbers per genotype and gender in a and c: NoT condition, n = 10; OI-R condition, n = 7–8; SI-NR condition, n = 8; SI-R condition, n = 10–11. Animal numbers per genotype and gender in b: SI-NR condition: n = 4; SI-R condition: n = 4–6. One symbol p < 0.05, two symbols p < 0.01, three symbols p < 0.001. M: mouse; NoT: no training; OI-R: object interaction-reinforced; SI-NR: social interaction-non-reinforced; SI-R: social interaction-reinforced; T: toy

We further assessed social behavior across experimental groups using the three-chamber test after behavioral training (Fig. 3c and S7). In this test, male and female Oprm1−/− mice from the NoT, OI-R, and SI-NR groups displayed a significant deficit in social preference, as shown by absent preference for time spent in close contact with the mouse over the toy (stimulus × genotype × condition: F3,123 = 9.7, p < 0.0001), longer duration of close contacts with the toy versus the mouse (stimulus × genotype × condition: F3,123 = 41.4, p < 0.0001) and, consequently, diminished preference ratios (genotype × condition: F3,123 = 15.5, p < 0.0001) when compared with respective Oprm1+/+ controls. In contrast, mutant animals trained under the SI-R condition preferred to spend more time in close contact with the mouse over the toy, displayed longer close contacts with the mouse and accordingly showed a fully restored preference ratio as compared with Oprm1+/+ animals from the same group (statistics in Table S5). Thus, beneficial effects of behavioral intervention in Oprm1−/− mice trained under the SI-R condition generalized to social preference measured in the three-chamber test.

Beneficial effects of behavioral intervention in mutant mice were limited to the social repertoire

We assessed whether the effects of behavioral training on social behavior would extend to other, non-social, behavioral deficits in Oprm1−/− mice. We measured spontaneous motor stereotypies in all groups after training. Mutant mice displayed more frequent grooming (genotype: F1,125 = 8.1, p < 0.01), burying (F1,125 = 5.7, p > 0.05), circling (F1,125 = 113.3, p < 0.0001), and shakes (F1,125 = 30.5, p < 0.0001) than wild-type animals independently from the experimental condition, except for grooming that was lower in the SI-NR group (condition: F3,125 = 2.9, p < 0.05). Also, burying episodes were shorter in Oprm1−/− compared with Oprm1+/+ animals through all conditions (genotype: F1,125 = 12.0, p < 0.001) (Fig. 4a). In the marble burying test, knockout mice buried more marbles than wild-type animals, with no significant effect of the experimental condition (genotype: F1,125 = 9.0, p < 0.05) (Fig. 4b). Similarly, in the Y-maze test, behavioral training failed to reduce the number of perseverative same arm entries (genotype: F1,125 = 104.1, p < 0.0001) and restore spontaneous alternation rates (F1,125 = 50.1, p < 0.0001) in mutant mice (Fig. 4c). Finally, we assessed anxiety levels following behavioral training using the novelty suppressed feeding test. In this test, Oprm1−/− mice took longer to eat on the food pellets, with mice from the OI-R, SI-NR, and SI-R condition eating faster than NoT animals. Under the OI-R condition, the latency to feed of Oprm1−/− mice was normalized to wild-type levels (genotype × condition: F3,124 = 24.3, p < 0.0001) (Fig. 4d). This latency was also returned to wild-type levels in male but not female mutant animals trained under the SI-NR and SI-R conditions (gender: F1,124 = 6.8, p < 0.01; gender × genotype × condition: F3,124 = 2.7, p < 0.05) (Figure S5). As regards food intake, Oprm1−/− mice ate less than Oprm1+/+ animals when back in home cage and so did control mice (NoT) compared with other conditions. Mutant mice ate as much as wild-type animals under the OI-R and SI-NR conditions (genotype × condition: F3,124 = 9.0, p < 0.0001; statistics in Table S5). Thus behavioral training reduced anxiety levels in male and female Oprm1−/− mice in the OI-R group. Altogether, these data indicate that such training had limited impact on off-target, non-social, behaviors.

Fig. 4: Behavioral intervention (SI-R) did not modify non-social (off-target) behaviors.
figure 4

a When tested after behavioral training, Oprm1−/− mice (solid bars), compared with wild-type animals (open bars), showed increased number of grooming and circling episodes, more frequent head shakes and shorter burying episodes. b Mutant animals buried more marbles in the marble burying test and c showed decreased rates of spontaneous alternation (SPA) and increased rates of same arm returns (SAR) when exploring a Y-maze, independently from training condition. d In the novelty suppressed feeding test, the latency to feed of mutant mice was reduced after training in the SI-NR and SI-R conditions and normalized to Oprm1+/+ levels in the OI-R group. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. Animal numbers per genotype and gender: NoT condition, n = 10; OI-R condition, n = 7–8; SI-NR condition, n = 8; SI-R condition, n = 10–11. Open stars: genotype effect; solid stars: genotype × condition interaction (three-way ANOVA followed by Newman–Keules post hoc test). Two stars: p < 0.01, three stars: p < 0.001. NoT: no training; OI-R: object interaction-reinforced; SI-NR: social interaction-non-reinforced; SI-R: social interaction-reinforced

Behavioral intervention normalized gene expression in the reward circuit of Oprm1 −/− mice

To identify molecular correlates of the behavioral improvements detected after training, we assessed in OI-R, SI-NR, and SI-R mice the expression of 12 genes across six brain regions known to play a key role in reward and social behavior: PFC, CPu, NAc, CeA, MeA, and VTA/SNc. We focused on immediate early genes and markers of plasticity (C-fos, Arc/Arg3.1, Bdnf), genes of the oxytocin/vasopressin system (Oxt, Avp, Oxtr, Avpr1a, Avpr1b) because of their key role in social behavior and evidences of altered function in Oprm1−/− mice9,11, autism gene candidates (Nlgn1, Foxp1, Crh) whose expression was dysregulated in this model9 and finally Grm4, encoding mGluR4 receptors whose activation relieves autistic symptoms9. Fold changes were calculated using expression in Oprm1+/+ mice from the OI-R group as a reference; they are presented in Table S7. Among tested genes, C-fos and Oxt particularly retained our attention (Fig. 5a). We detected a decrease in C-fos expression, a marker gene for neuronal activation, in most regions of the social/reward circuit (NAc, CPu, MeA, and VTA/SNc) of Oprm1−/− mice trained under the OI-R condition, except for the PFC and CeA where this expression was increased. Repeated interaction with a conspecific under the SI-NR condition restored C-fos transcription levels totally in the NAc and MeA and partially in the CPu and CeA, but left these levels unchanged in the PFC and VTA/SNc. SI-R training normalized C-fos mRNA levels in all brain regions but the PFC of mutant mice. As regards Oxt expression, we detected reduced levels of transcripts in the NAc, CeA, and MeA of Oprm1−/− mice from the OI-R group. Mutant mice trained under the SI-NR condition exhibited restored levels of Oxt mRNA in the NAc (even increased), partial increase in the CeA and no change in the MeA; the same animals trained under the SI-R condition exhibited normalized levels of Oxt mRNA in all these regions. Thus behavioral intervention rescued deregulated expression of C-fos and Oxt in Oprm1−/− animals, whatever their direction (up- or downregulation). Training under the SI-NR paradigm ameliorated, but only partially, this expression.

Fig. 5: Behavioral intervention normalized plasticity marker and oxytocin/vasopressin gene expression in Oprm1−/− mice.
figure 5

a In Oprm1−/− mice from the OI-R group, C-fos expression was decreased in the NAc, CPu, MeA, and VTA/SNc but increased in the PFC and CeA when compared with Oprm1+/+ trained under the same condition (used as a reference). Training under the SI-NR condition restored C-fos transcription levels totally in the NAc (even increased) and MeA and partially in the CPu and CeA, but left these levels unchanged in the PFC and VTA/SNc. SI-R training normalized C-fos mRNA levels in all brain regions but the PFC of these mice. Oprm1−/− mice from the OI-R group also displayed decreased levels of Oxt transcripts in the NAc, CeA, and MeA. When trained under the SI-NR condition, these animals exhibited restored levels of Oxt mRNA in the NAc (even increased), partial increase in the CeA and no change in the MeA; mutant mice trained under the SI-R condition instead showed normalized levels of Oxt mRNA in all these regions. Gene expression in wild-type animals from the SI-NR and SI-R are not presented for simplification (see Table S7). b Hierarchical clustering organized gene expression data in four main clusters. Cluster (A) grouped a majority of genes with upregulated expression in Oprm1−/− mice trained under the SI-NR and OI-R conditions compared with wild-type (SI-NR or SI-R) or mutant SI-R trained animals. Conversely, cluster (C) brought together genes with downregulated expression in OI-R and SI-NR trained mutant mice. Most of these downregulations were detected in the striatum and amygdala. Cluster (B) gathered genes whose expression was not significantly regulated in these regions. Gene expression in cluster (D) was either oppositely regulated between mutant mice trained under the SI-NR and OI-R conditions, not regulated in these groups, whereas increased in other groups or not regulated. This analysis revealed that gene expression pattern in Oprm1−/− mice trained under the SI-R condition was more similar to that of wild-type animals (SI-NR or SI-R groups) than that of Oprm1−/− mice trained under the SI-NR or OI-R conditions. c A PCA performed on a selection of three social interaction parameters and qRT-PCR results for 14 genes unraveled the opposition between prosocial parameters (time in social contact, following) and a social avoidance parameter (grooming after social contact) along PC1 (variables’ space, top panel). C-fos expression in the CPu, Oxt expression in the MeA and CeA, Crh mRNA levels in the NAc and Grm4 levels in the CPu cluster with the former whereas Arc in the VTA, CeA, and MeA, Oxtr in the NAc, C-fos in the CeA and Avpr1a in the CeA cluster with the latter. Projection in the subjects’ space (down panel) clearly dissociated Oprm1−/− individuals of the OI-R and SI-NR groups from Oprm1+/+ mice along PC1, with PC2 axis segregating SI-NR from OI-R populations. Remarkably, individual Oprm1−/− mice from the SI-R group clustered with Oprm1+/+ animals, showing that, when focusing on this particular set of behavioral and gene expression data, SI-R training normalized Oprm1−/− features. Gene expression data are expressed as fold change versus the Oprm1+/+ OI-R group (median ± SEM). Behavioral data are presented as mean ± SEM. Animal numbers per genotype, gender, and training condition: n = 4. One star p < 0.05, two stars p < 0.01, three stars p < 0.001. CeA: central nucleus of the amygdala; CPu: caudate putamen; MeA: medial nucleus of the amygdala; NAc: nucleus accumbens; OI-R: object interaction-reinforced; PFC: prefrontal cortex; SI-NR: social interaction-non-reinforced; SI-R: social interaction-reinforced; VTA/SNc: ventral tegmental area/substantia nigra pars compacta. See social interaction parameters before qRT-PCR experiment in Figure S8; principal components extracted in PCA analysis are displayed in Table S2

We performed a cluster analysis of all qRT-PCR data to visualize patterns of gene expression depending on genotype and experimental condition (Fig. 5b). Hierarchical clustering organized gene expression data in four main clusters. Cluster (A) grouped a majority of genes with upregulated expression in Oprm1−/− mice trained under the SI-NR and OI-R conditions compared with wild-type (SI-NR or SI-R) or mutant SI-R trained animals. Remarkable genes in this cluster were Arc, markedly upregulated in all regions studied for OI-R and SI-NR trained Oprm1−/− mice, Oxtr (coding oxytocin receptors), upregulated in the amygdala of mice from the OI-R and SI-NR groups and Crh, upregulated in the PFC, CPu, and VTA/SNc of mutant animals. Conversely, cluster (C) brought together genes with downregulated expression in OI-R and SI-NR trained mutant mice. Most of these downregulations were detected in the striatum and amygdala (such as C-fos, Oxt, Crh, Bdnf, Grm4, Avpr1a, or Avpr1b). Cluster (B) gathered genes whose expression was not significantly regulated in these regions. Gene expression in cluster (D) was either oppositely regulated between mutant mice trained under the SI-NR and OI-R conditions (C-fos and Oxt in the NAc), not regulated in these groups while increased in other groups (Avp in the NAc, Bdnf in the NAc and CPu) or not regulated. Globally, this analysis unraveled that gene expression pattern in knockout animals trained under the SI-R condition was more similar to that of wild-type animals (SI-NR or SI-R groups) than that of Oprm1−/− mice trained under the SI-NR or OI-R conditions.

Finally, we performed a PCA including social interaction parameters and gene expression data in different regions to assess correlations between these different outputs (Fig. 5c and S8, Tables S2 and S7). We selected three behavioral parameters and 14 qRT-PCR results that together accounted for 81.5% of the variance in this sample (variables’ space, Fig. 5c, top). We found that the time spent in NC and number of following episodes, two measures of prosocial behavior, clustered together and with C-fos expression in the CPu, Oxt expression in the MeA and CeA, Crh mRNA levels in the NAc and Grm4 levels in the CPu. This first cluster was opposed along the first principal component (PC1) to a second centered on the number of grooming episodes occurring immediately after social contact, an index of social avoidance, and gathering expression of Arc in the VTA, CeA, and MeA, Oxtr in the NAc, C-fos in the CeA and Avpr1a in the CeA. Oxt and C-fos expression in the NAc and C-fos levels in the MeA correlated mostly with PC2. Projection in the subjects’ space (Fig. 5c, bottom) dissociated Oprm1−/− individuals of the OI-R and SI-NR groups from Oprm1+/+ mice, with PC2 axis segregating SI-NR from OI-R populations. Remarkably, and consistent with the results of clustering analysis, data from individual Oprm1−/− mice from the SI-R group clustered with those of Oprm1+/+ animals (OI-R, SI-NR, and SI-R condition), showing that, when focusing on this particular set of behavioral and gene expression data, SI-R training (behavioral intervention) normalized Oprm1−/− features.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first report of a successful back-translation of ABA-based EIBI to a murine model of autism. We used mice lacking MOR, which display blunted reward processing1,9,13, a predictor of positive outcome for behavioral intervention in children with ASD55,56, and demonstrated that associating a positive reinforcement to daily encounters with an unfamiliar wild-type congener is sufficient to durably restore direct social interaction, social preference in the three-chamber test and gene expression in the reward/social circuit of these animals, similarly in males and females.

Transposing complex intervention programs such as ABA-based EIBI to a rodent model is a true challenge, and our experimental paradigm shows some methodological limitations. First, EIBI utilize various elaborated paradigms to modify target behaviors in patients with autism30,31,32,33,34, whereas here we used appetitive conditioning solely. However, most of the techniques used in behavioral interventions involve positive reinforcement and thus stimulate the reward system, as we did in the present experiments. As a positive reinforcer, we used food reward, as commonly done in the first stages of EIBI43. Of note, mutant animals, especially females, consumed less-palatable food than Oprm1+/+ mice during the course of training, suggesting reduced motivation for food, as previously described57. These results are in agreement with a key role of MOR in mediating the hedonic “liking” for food58,59. Oprm1−/− mice, however, can learn an operant task to obtain food57,60. In our study, they modified their behavior when exposed to palatable food, further showing that they can use food reward as a reinforcer. Second, EIBI therapists usually reinforce each occurrence of the target behavior immediately (fixed ratio 1) and depending on its quality (differential reinforcement)61,62. Here we instead used a trace conditioning procedure, palatable food being presented with a delay after social encounter, and provided the same amount of food whatever the quality of previous social interaction. Although this paradigm likely made the association between food reward and social experience more difficult to acquire, improved social interaction in Oprm1−/− animals from the SI-R group after training demonstrates that these animals indeed made such association. Importantly, food reward was not able to rescue social interaction when not associated to social experience (OI-R condition), showing that behavioral improvements under the SI-R condition were not due to reward exposure only. Moreover, experiencing social encounter with a wild-type stranger every day without receiving a reward (SI-NR) partially restored social interaction in mutants, in line with clinical reports showing lower symptom severity in patients with ASD interacting with typical peers, notably at school63. These improvements, though, rapidly faded when daily encounters ceased. In contrast, social enrichment produced partial but persistent beneficial effects on social abilities in Oprm1−/− mice when provided from neonatal age64. Earlier training under the SI-NR condition may thus be required to obtain stable social improvements in these animals. Despite the above limitations, our behavioral intervention (SI-R) paradigm successfully allowed us to modify social behavior in Oprm1−/− mice by using positive reinforcement, in agreement with our primary hypothesis.

Remarkably, our transposition of behavioral intervention successfully reproduced several key features of EIBI in MOR null mice. As shown in the clinics29,45, the effects of intervention were long lasting: they were still detectable 14 weeks after complete cessation of intervention. Such long-lasting beneficial effects of SI-NR training were observed independently from the sex of the animals. Clinical studies reporting positive effects of EIBI include both male and female patients; due to limited numbers of girls, however, these studies do not comment on sex effects29,45,65. Interestingly, behavioral intervention (SI-R condition) also showed beneficial effects in Oprm1+/+ mice, by preserving high levels of social interaction in aging (24 weeks) animals. This result further demonstrates the key role played by reward in sustaining social behavior7,66. Furthermore, Oprm1−/− mice of the SI-R group showed only modest improvements in off-target, non-social, behaviors, namely motor stereotypies or anxiety. Based on clinical data, we could have expected some reduction of stereotyped behaviors;29 however, our paradigm may not have been comprehensive and/or intensive enough to verify this. Of note, knockout mice from the OI-R group showed reduced anxiety in the novelty suppressed feeding test, likely a consequence of associating daily novel object exploration with a reward: we thus involuntarily trained these animals to display less anxiety in this particular context.

Last, behavioral intervention modified neural substrates in Oprm1−/− mice, as shown by regulated gene expression. We analyzed the expression pattern of a small collection of genes across six brain regions within the reward/social circuit and revealed that back-translating EIBI in mice was able to normalize this pattern, in correlation with behavioral markers of social abilities. As regards neuronal activity, we detected reduced C-fos mRNA level following social interaction in the reward/social circuit (NAc, CPu, MeA, and VTA/SNc), as opposed to increased expression in executive control and anxiety-related PFC and CeA, in knockout mice from the OI-R group, extending previous observations9. These results likely reflect modified connectivity within reward/aversion pathways in these animals17 and are coherent with their reduced reward sensitivity, social interest, and increased anxiety9,13. They also match imaging data in ASD patients showing decreased activity in the reward circuit in response to social stimuli7,67,68 and increased activity in the amygdala under anxiogenic social conditions69. Remarkably, SI-R training normalized C-fos expression in all these regions but the PFC. This result suggests that SI-R paradigm was able to reattribute rewarding properties to social interaction and to decrease anxiety in this context. Now regarding plasticity gene markers, we report for the first time excessive widespread Arc expression in the brain of Oprm1−/− mice following social interaction. In the CeA, MeA, and VTA/SNc, increased Arc levels were tightly correlated with a behavioral marker of social avoidance, grooming after social contact. Arc codes for activity-regulated cytoskeletal-associated protein (Arc), involved in regulating synaptic plasticity, cellular signaling, glutamate neurotransmission, and spine growth70,71. Intriguingly, levels of Arc protein were found increased in the brains of two other mouse models of ASD72,73 as well as in the blood of patients with autism74. Deficient Arc expression, however, may also be detrimental to social behavior, as shown by impaired sociability and schizophrenia-related phenotype in mice with invalidated Arc gene75 as well as genetic association between mutations in Arc and schizophrenia in humans76,77,78. Together, these data support the hypothesis of a functional connection between Arc and neurodevelopmental diseases with impaired social abilities, namely autism and schizophrenia79. Remarkably, SI-R but not SI-NR trained Oprm1−/− mice displayed normalized Arc expression in the CeA, MeA, and VTA/SNc, indicating that plastic events occurred in these regions that likely contributed to improve social abilities. Still related to plasticity, SI-R training induced or restored the expression of Bdnf in the NAc and CeA, respectively, of Oprm1−/− mice. Together, these results match data from imaging studies showing EIBI-induced brain plasticity in autistic patients80, specifically in the reward circuit for subjects who display initial hypoactivation in these regions55.

Now focusing on genes from the oxytocin/vasopressin system, behavioral training had also major effects on their expression in Oprm1−/− mice. First, we confirmed our previous observation of decreased levels of Oxt mRNA, coding for oxytocin, in the NAc of Oprm1−/− mice (OI-R group)9 and extended it to the CeA and MeA, where they were correlated with prosocial behavioral parameters. Accordingly, transcripts for the oxytocin receptor (Oxtr) were found increased in the same regions, matching binding data11. The detection of Oxt transcripts (as well as Avp mRNAs, coding for vasopressin) in brain regions outside the hypothalamus, where oxytocinergic (and vasopressinergic) neurons are localized81, might appear surprising: it implies that these mRNAs where transported to projection sites of oxytocinergic neurons. In agreement with this, Oxt mRNAs have recently been evidenced in distal projections of human stem cell-derived neurons82, suggesting that local transcription may play an important role in oxytocin (and possibly vasopressin) neurotransmission. On the vasopressin side, expression of Avpr1a, coding V1AR receptors, in mutants, was decreased in the NAc and MeA but increased in the CeA. All these data point to major alterations of the oxytocin/vasopressin system in MOR null mice that likely contributed to their autistic-like phenotype, as shown in other animal models and in patients83,84,85,86. Singularly, behavioral intervention brought oxytocin/vasopressin gene expression back to wild-type levels or even higher for Oxt and Avp in the NAc. Thus, beneficial effects of intervention on social behavior in mutants likely involved restored oxytocin/vasopressin function, a key neurobiological substrate for social reward87. Pharmacological manipulations of this system, using either oxytocin receptor agonists (including oxytocin) or V1AR receptor antagonists, can restore social abilities in animal models of ASD85, including Oprm1−/− mice11 and in patients with autism88,89. Oxytocin treatment in ASD patients increases NAc connectivity90 that was shown decreased notably as a function of OXTR risk-allele dosage:19 these data further highlight the connection between oxytocin activity and reward processing. Interestingly, our results bring additional arguments for combining pharmacological approaches targeting oxytocin with behavioral therapy91,92, by revealing shared neurobiological mechanisms.

Finally, behavioral intervention normalized (except in PFC) the expression of Crh, coding for corticotropin-releasing factor that possibly contributes to increased anxiety levels in ASD93,94, and partially restored striatal expression of Grm4, coding for the mGluR4 receptor, which activation rescues ASD symptoms in Oprm1−/− mice9. These results indicate that the neurobiological underpinnings of therapy-induced behavioral improvements spread well over plasticity markers and the oxytocin/vasopressin system, and will require further investigation.

In conclusion, we demonstrate that reattributing a rewarding value to social experience in the Oprm1−/− mouse model of ASD durably rescues deficient social abilities and modify neurobiological substrates in the reward circuitry, in accordance with the Social Motivation Theory of autism95,96. This occurred despite defective reward processing in mice lacking MOR, suggesting that palatable food pairing was able to rescue social reward in these animals, maybe by restoring oxytocin/vasopressin function. These results strongly suggest that the main key to success in EIBI programs is to restore social reward in patients with ASD. They also point to the therapeutic potential of pharmacological treatments stimulating reward processes to relieve ASD symptoms, an idea that is now emerging from clinical and imaging studies21. Such pharmacotherapy could target the oxytocin/vasopressin system as well as other key substrates for reward processing and efficiently complement EIBI programs by reducing their demand and the variability of their outcomes.