Introduction

Symbiotic microbes are increasingly recognised as key modulators of host phenotypes. This is particularly true for the mammalian gut microbiota, whose metabolism is intimately entwined with that of the host. Among their many roles in host physiology, mammalian gut microbes modulate host energy metabolism [1, 2], regulate fat accumulation and thermal homoeostasis [3], and provide protection against pathogenic infection [4, 5]. They are also in constant dialogue with the host immune system, activating innate immune responses and tuning acquired immune responses to distinguish enemies from allies [6,7,8]. As such, alterations to these microbial communities can have significant impacts on host health and have been associated with major metabolic and immune-related health conditions in humans [1, 9, 10].

Despite gut microbiota’swell-established role in host biology, we know surprisingly little about the forces that shape microbiota composition within and between individuals in nature. Community composition is notoriously variable among individuals, and is affected by a number of processes that can be viewed within a metacommunity framework [11]: transmission processes (microbial dispersal) first determine which microbes colonise an individual host. Subsequently, aspects of the nutritional and immunological environment inside the host (e.g. host diet, genetics), as well as ecological interactions with resident microbes, selectively filter colonising microbes that can persist and thrive. In mammals, the microbiota is initially established through maternal transmission at birth [12], with community composition then further shaped by transmission from family members and the broader environment [13,14,15] as well as selective processes within the host [16].

A key question is to what extent ongoing transmission throughout life shapes the microbiota. Accumulating evidence suggests the gut microbiota is affected by a host’s environment, such as diet [17, 18] and contact with soil [15, 19, 20]. The microbiota can also be shaped by a host’s social environment, since a special form of microbial transmission can occur through social contact. Intimate social contact, such as the many forms of prosocial touch common in mammals (e.g. grooming, licking, huddling), may function as an important transmission route for microbes. This is particularly true for microbes not easily transmitted via the environment, including strict anaerobes and non-spore-forming bacteria [21]. Moreover, if less transmissible microbes are more likely to positively impact host fitness [22], social interactions could constitute a key pathway (alongside vertical transmission) by which symbionts of high functional significance are transmitted in mammals. Laboratory rodent studies have repeatedly shown that cohousing drives convergence in microbiota composition [23,24,25], indicating that social interaction and close proximity facilitate microbial transmission under captive conditions.

In highly social group-living mammals, the host social environment seems to have important effects on the gut microbiota. Social group membership has been shown to predict gut microbiota composition in several species of primates [26,27,28,29,30,31] and other group-living mammals [32,33,34]. Social group effects also occur in humans, as unrelated individuals living in the same household were found to have a more similar microbiota than relatives living in different households [35]. However, the mechanisms underlying these observations remain unclear, and may include not only direct social transmission but also shared environmental exposures like diet. In some cases, social group effects on the microbiota have been found while controlling for kinship or shared diet, supporting the idea that social transmission homogenises the gut microbiota. For example, sifakas (Propithecus verrauxii) were found to have a social group-specific gut microbiota composition that was not explained by diet similarity, or habitat overlap, nor genetic relatedness [28]. Further support comes from individuals observed to switch social groups, for example immigrant male baboons [36], whose microbiota composition converged on that of their new social group.

Some evidence also suggests social interactions affect microbiota similarity at a dyadic level, within groups or populations. Several primate studies have shown the intensity of social interaction between group members to predict similarity in their microbiota [26,27,28, 30]. Baboons that groomed each other more were found to share more gut microbes, and these shared bacteria were enriched in anaerobic and non-spore-forming taxa [26]. Similar patterns were found in humans, with couples who reported having a “physically close relationship” sharing more gut microbes than less close couples or friends [37]. However, socially interacting primates often experience strong overlap in their environments, and thus it remains difficult to distinguish social transmission from shared environmental exposures [21]. Species that are not group-living (sensu Wilson, [38]) arguably provide more powerful systems in which to clearly distinguish effects of social interaction from confounding shared environmental exposures, as social interactions are more limited in time and space. However, the role of social transmission in shaping the microbiota in such species has yet to be explored.

Here, we use wild mice as a model system (wood mice, Apodemus sylvaticus) to assess how social interactions shape gut microbiota similarity among sympatric individuals, in comparison to effects of host kinship, spatial proximity, and other factors. These mice are not group-living, but can be considered a semi-social species, with the propensity to co-nest in underground burrows varying seasonally and among individuals [39, 40]. Individuals have stable, partially overlapping home ranges, and yet vary in their level of social contact, making them a particularly suitable species in which to study social transmission. Social behaviours that might facilitate gut microbe transmission in wood mice include contact behaviours such as allogrooming [41], huddling [42], nest-sharing [40] or aggressive interactions [41]. Transmission could also occur through non-contact social behaviours, such as scent-marking and investigation of faecal cues [43, 44], or possibly coprophagy as seen in other mouse species, though coprophagy has not been documented in wood mice to date. Using a tracking system based on passive radio-frequency identification (RFID) tags, we intensively followed a population of mice for one year and used social network analyses to test two specific hypotheses about social transmission of microbiota. First, we test the prediction that if social interactions drive microbial transmission, dyadic microbiota similarity will be positively predicted by proximity in the social network, independent of other potential confounders. Second, individuals that are more connected in the social network are predicted to have higher microbiota diversity, as they are exposed to more extensive social transmission.

Materials and methods

Field data collection

Data were collected over a one-year period (Nov 2014–Dec 2015) from a wild population of wood mice (Apodemus sylvaticus) in a 2.47 ha mixed woodland plot (Nash’s Copse) at Imperial College’s Silwood Park campus, UK (Fig. S1A). Live traps were set for one night every 2–4 weeks in an alternating checkerboard design, to ensure even coverage. At first capture, all mice were injected subcutaneously with a passive integrated transponder tag (PIT-tag) for permanent identification. At each trapping, demographic data on captured animals was recorded and samples for gut microbiota analysis and mouse genotyping collected (see Appendix 1 in Supplementary material).

Data on rodent space use and social associations was collected in parallel to trapping using a set of nine custom-built PIT-tag loggers (described in [45] and Appendix 2; Figs. S1), distributed across the trapping grid. Loggers consisted of a box with entrance tubes, that recorded the time-stamped presence of any rodent that entered. Loggers were rotated systematically around the plot throughout the study period, using a sampling design that ensured even spatial coverage, with each 100 m2 grid cell covered on average 5.49 (SD 1.61) times (Fig. S1C). Between logging nights, loggers were thoroughly cleaned with 70% ethanol (see Appendix 2). After data cleaning and filtering (Fig S2),  83 of the 93 mice tagged during study period were present in the logger data.

Kinship analysis

To derive estimates of host genetic relatedness, ear tissue samples were used to genotype all captured mice at eleven microsatellite loci (Tables S1 and S2; detailed in [39]) and build a pedigree in COLONY 2.0.6.5 [46]. Full details of genotyping methods and pedigree reconstruction are provided in Appendix 3. After sample failures, genetic relatedness could be inferred for 70 of the 83 monitored mice.

Constructing social networks

All analyses were conducted in R version 3.6.1 (R-Core-Team 2019). To capture patterns of spatiotemporal coincidence among wood mice, social networks were constructed from logger data using the package asnipe [47] and plotted using igraph [48]. Individual mice were nodes, and edges described the number of instances two individuals were observed  associated, i.e. observed at the same logger during the same night (12 h period, 6 pm to 6 am), within a specific time window of each other. To measure association strength, we used an adjusted version of the Simple Ratio Index (SRI), that accounted for variable overlap in individual lifespans (i.e. time between first and last logger observation) [49], hereafter “Adjusted SRI”. Adjusted SRI is defined as follows for two individuals, A and B:

$$I = \frac{X}{{\left[ {X + y_{AB} + y_A + y_B} \right]}}$$

where X is the number of instances (night-location combinations) in which A and B were observed associated (at the same location within a specified time window of each other), yAB is the number of instances in which A and B were both observed, but not associated, yA and yB are the number of instances in which both were known to be alive but only A or B were observed respectively. By taking lifespan overlap into account we could incorporate data from all 83 individuals across the entire year into one static social network.

To examine how the definition of social association might affect social network-microbiota relationships, we constructed a series of networks using increasingly intimate definitions of social association, by applying a sliding time window criterion of variable length to define social association (i.e what counts as X in the formula), from 12 h (as above) down to a 2 min period (12 h, 4 h, 1 h, 30 min, 10 min, 2 min). We also calculated a parallel set of networks with binary social association indices (BI), where ‘1’ indicated the dyad were observed associated at least once, and ‘0’ indicated they were not.

Gut microbiota characterisation

The gut microbiota was successfully characterised from 239 faecal samples belonging to 75 individual wood mice (covering 90% of the monitored mice, mean = 3.2 samples/mouse, range = 1–9). Full details of library preparation, sequencing and bioinformatics are given in Supplementary Material (Appendix 4; Figs. S3S5). Briefly, microbiota profiling involved amplicon sequencing of the 16S rRNA gene (V4-region). Sequence data were processed through the DADA2 pipeline v1.6.0 [50], to infer amplicon sequence variants (ASVs) and taxonomy assigned using the GreenGenes Database (Consortium 13.8). Using package phyloseq [51], ASV-counts were normalised to proportional abundance within each sample [52] and singleton ASVs as well as those belonging to non-gut microbial taxa (Cyanobacteria, Mitochondria) were removed. Lastly, we used package iNEXT [53] to estimate asymptotic richness and Shannon diversity for each sample.

Statistical analyses

To describe compositional microbiota variation, package vegan [54] was used to calculate Jaccard distances and Bray–Curtis dissimilarities among samples (Fig. S6). We used the Jaccard Index (1-Jaccard distance, the proportion of shared ASVs between sample pairs) as our primary measure of microbiota similarity, as we considered this metric most relevant for investigating microbial transmission among hosts. However, for robust inference, we repeated key analyses using Bray–Curtis dissimilarity (an abundance-weighted metric less sensitive to potential sequencing artefacts), and repeated analyses on both Jaccard Index and Bray–Curtis dissimilarity with a rarefied dataset, to confirm beta diversity results were not affected by read depth variation among samples.

General predictors of gut microbiota composition

We performed permutational analysis of variance (PERMANOVA) in vegan to (1) test the repeatability of gut microbiota composition among individuals sampled multiple times, (2) identify non-social effects on the microbiota to be controlled for in subsequent analyses and (3) estimate how much individual variation was independent of these covariates. We tested effects of time (month, as a factor), host age (juvenile/adult), sex, plot region, habitat type, and individual identity on Jaccard distance. Plot region and habitat type for each individual were defined from logger data, as the most common logger territory (no.1–9) and habitat type (rhododendron, open woodland/bluebell, bamboo or mixed; Fig. S1A) they were detected in.

Associations between social association strength and microbiota similarity

To test whether dyadic microbiota similarity was predicted by social association strength, we performed Bayesian regression models in package brms [55]. These models are well-suited for this as they permit random effect structures able to account for the types of dependence inherent to dyadic data, and repeat sampling of individuals [56]. We constructed brms models that included all dyadic sample comparisons except within-individual comparisons. Microbiota similarity (Jaccard Index) was used as the response, with social association strength (adjusted SRI, or BI index) as the main predictor. As the Jaccard Index is a proportion, a logit link function was used. To control for potential confounding variables as far as possible, we fitted several dyadic covariates: spatial distance between hosts, sampling interval (time in days between samples taken), kinship, sex and age similarity (0/1 for different/same). Spatial distance was calculated as the distance between individuals’ mean spatial coordinates from logger records (minimum 34 logger records per mouse). All covariates either naturally ranged from 0 to 1 or were scaled to do so, to make model estimates for all terms comparable. To control for non-independence in the dataset arising from a dyadic response variable and repeat samples per mouse, both the model intercept and slope (social association strength effect) were allowed to vary as defined by two random effects: (i) a multi-membership random effect capturing the individuals in each dyad (Individual A + Individual B) and (ii) a multi-membership random effect capturing the samples in each dyad (Sample A + Sample B).

To test for sex-dependence in the effect of social association (e.g. arising from specific sexual behaviours) on the microbiota, the main model (12 h edge definition) was also run including dyad sex category (male-male, male-female or female-female) and its interaction with social association strength. In this model, only a multi-membership random intercept was fitted (not a random slope) to help ensure there was enough power to estimate the interaction effect. Finally, to check our results were robust to the chosen statistical approach, we confirmed key results with two alternative statistical modelling frameworks: (1) MCMCglmm, an alternative R package for Bayesian regression [57] and (2) a matrix permutation-based method common in social network analyses, Multiple Regression Quadratic Assignment procedure (MRQAP; [58]), with a data subset including one randomly selected sample per individual (Appendix 5; Fig. S7).

Social network position and microbiota diversity

We hypothesised that an individual’s social network position might affect gut microbiota (alpha) diversity. Depending on the transmission ecology, different types of network position might best predict diversity. To explore this, we calculated six different metrics of network position, that capture different aspects of social connectedness (Fig. 1). If the sheer amount of social interaction or number of social partners can diversify the microbiota, we expect diversity to be predicted by measures of general network centrality (Fig. 1). Alternatively, if diversity is driven by the distinctness of transmission sources, and if this is reflected in their social distance, we expect diversity to be predicted by measures of bridge-type centrality (Fig. 1). To test the relationship between each centrality measure and gut microbiota diversity, we used Bayesian regression models in MCMCglmm with either asymptotic ASV richness or asymptotic Shannon diversity as the response. We first explored how several covariates predicted diversity: host age, sex, sampling month (as a factor), plot region, habitat type, read count, and PCR plate (4-level factor), and simplified models to include only covariates with p < 0.1. We then added into this model one of our six measures of social centrality (Fig. 1), derived from either the 12 h or 2 min network. Individual identity and PCR plate were fitted as a random factors. A node permutation test was used to verify that significant effects were not driven by network structure. The observed posterior mean estimates for network position were compared with those derived from 1000 models in which network positions were randomised across individuals.

Fig. 1: Six measures of network centrality and interpretation of a positive relationship with microbiota diversity.
figure 1

Images depict focal individuals (red circles) whose social interactions (lines) with other individuals (black circles) give them a high value of each centrality metric.

Identifying which bacterial taxa associate with social interaction

To identify candidate socially transmitted bacterial taxa, we tested how each bacterial family affected the strength of correlation between social association strength and microbiota similarity. We recalculated the Jaccard Index excluding each bacterial family in turn, then compared (both 12 h and 2 min) social network effect sizes and credible intervals from MCMCglmm models using these indices (full model details in Appendix 5).

Results

Factors predicting gut microbiota composition

The mean Jaccard Index across the whole population of mice was 0.17 (sd = 0.6). In a marginal PERMANOVA on data from repeat-sampled mice, individual identity explained 33% compositional variation in the microbiota, while temporal fluctuations (month) explained 6%, with similar results for both Jaccard Index and Bray–Curtis dissimilarity (Table S3, See Fig. S8 for more thorough description of the temporal fluctuations). When other individual-level attributes were included (age, sex, plot region and habitat type), 27% variation in microbiota composition remained attributable to individual identity (Table S4), indicating the microbiota showed consistent individual variation that was not explained by measured host factors. No other variables predicted microbiota composition, except for a weak effect of habitat type (marginal PERMANOVA on data with one sample per individual, Table S5). Among the subset of hosts (70 of 75) with kinship information, kinship and microbiota similarity (Jaccard Index) were unrelated (Mantel test: r = 0.001, p = 0.520).

Wood mouse social structure

The wood mouse social network showed marked variation in edge weights (social association strength) but no clear clustering (Fig. 2). Social association strength did not differ significantly among female-female, female-male and male-male pairs (Table S6). Global network density declined as increasingly intimate edge definitions were used (Fig. 2). The correlation among social networks with different edge definitions also decayed as the difference in time windows increased (Table S7). As expected, social association strength was to an extent predicted by spatial proximity in all networks (MRQAP p < 0.001, Table S6), though this spatial effect weakened as more intimate edge definitions were used (Fig. S9, Table S6). Even in the least intimate (12 h) social network, mice clearly did not solely associate with their nearest neighbours, as distances to the closest social partner (mean 25.6 m; sd = 15.3 m) were on average over three times greater than those to the nearest neighbour (mean = 8.4 m; sd = 5.5 m). Some strong social associations were observed between individuals whose mean spatial locations were over 60 m apart (Fig. 2E–H). As such, the social structure of this population was only partially determined by spatial location, and this spatial influence on social contact was weakest in the 2 min network.

Fig. 2: Wild wood mouse social networks with different edge definitions (2min-12h), plotted in either social space (A–D) or geographical space (E–H).
figure 2

In A–D networks are plotted using a standard weighted spring layout that minimises the sum of edge lengths and overlap across the network (igraph, [43]), and in B–H mice are positioned at their mean spatial coordinates recorded from logger data, superimposed on a habitat map of the study area. Background colours reflect habitat types (dark green = rhododendron, light green = bamboo, blue = bluebell, white = open woodland). Red and blue circles represent female and male mice respectively, and line thickness is proportional to social association strength.

Social association strength predicts microbiota similarity

Among pairs of individuals, the strength of social association strongly and positively predicted similarity in gut microbiota composition (in 12 h network: posterior mean 0.78, CI = 0.34–1.24; Fig. 3). Specifically, the proportion of ASVs shared within dyads (Jaccard Index) was positively predicted by their social association strength in all networks, even when controlling for effects of sex, age, kinship, sampling interval, and spatial distance (Table S8). Other variables also predicted microbiota similarity, including the spatial distance between hosts (posterior mean −0.08, CI = −0.12 to −0.04) and the time interval over which they were sampled (posterior mean −0.46, CI = −0.48 to −0.43), but the size of these effects was consistently smaller than that of social association strength (Fig. 3, Table S8). Consistent results were obtained with models using alternative statistical frameworks, when using Bray–Curtis dissimilarity, and when using indices derived from a rarefied microbiota ASV dataset (Appendix 5; Tables S9 and S10). Even binary social networks predicted microbiota similarity (Table S11), albeit less strongly than association strength.

Fig. 3: Social association strength predicts gut microbiota similarity more strongly than spatial distance, kinship and other effects.
figure 3

Effect size estimates (points) and their 95% credible intervals (coloured lines) are plotted from Bayesian regression (brms) models with pairwise microbiota similarity among hosts (Jaccard Index) as the response. Where credible intervals do not overlap zero, a variable significantly predicts microbiota similarity. Social association strength in the 12 h network has a strong positive effect on microbiota similarity, that is larger than that of other variables.

The relationship between social association strength and microbiota similarity became stronger as networks with increasingly intimate edge definitions were analysed (Fig. 4A), while spatial and temporal effects remained comparable across networks (Table S8). As such, the effect of social association increased from 1.7 times as large as the next strongest (sampling interval) effect in the 12 h network, to over 13 times as strong in the most intimate (2 min) network. Accordingly, the mean predicted Jaccard Index among mice with a weak (0 ≤ Adjusted SRI < 0.1) vs. strong (SRI > 0.9) level of social association increased modestly from 0.23 to 0.30 in models using the 12 h network, but approximately doubled from 0.29 to 0.59 in models using the 2 min network (Fig. 4C). Since more intimate networks also had fewer edges (i.e. lower density, Fig. 2), we also tested whether variation in network density alone might drive this trend. To do this, we ran a set of null models (described fully in Appendix 6) in which the least intimate (12 h) network was thinned to have the same number of edges as seen in each real network. In contrast to the real networks, social network effect sizes remained relatively constant in null models using artificially thinned networks (Fig. 4B).

Fig. 4: Social association strength predicts microbiota similarity more strongly in networks that use a more intimate edge definition.
figure 4

A The effect of social association strength on microbiota similarity (Jaccard Index) is stronger in networks with more intimate edge definitions. Social network effect sizes (estimated slope of the relationship between social association strength and microbiota similarity, the Jaccard Index) and their 95% credible intervals are plotted from Bayesian regression (brms) models that included the same covariates shown in Fig. 3. B Differences in effect size across networks are not due to variation in network density, as effect size did not change in null models where the 12 h network was artificially thinned by removal of the weakest edges to have the same density as each real network of differing edge definition. C The distribution of predicted microbiota similarity (Jaccard Index) values in pairs of mice who were observed socially associated either never (white, SRI = 0), rarely (light grey, 0 < SRI ≤ 0.1) or often (SRI > 0.9, dark grey) in either 12 h or 2 min social networks (columns).

Sex-dependent effects of social association on microbiota similarity

We further found that the effect of social association strength on microbiota similarity depended on the sex of interacting individuals. In a model including an interaction between social association strength and dyadic sex combination, social association strength predicted microbiota similarity strongly in male-male pairs (posterior mean 0.28, CI = 0.01–0.56; Table S12) and male-female pairs (posterior mean 0.30, CI = 0.04–0.56) but not significantly in female-female pairs (posterior mean 0.10, CI −0.15 to 0.35; Fig. 5, Table S12).

Fig. 5: Social association strength predicts microbiota similarity only among dyads involving males.
figure 5

Estimated social network effects on the microbiota (slope of the relationship between social association strength and Jaccard Index) and 95% credible intervals are plotted from a Bayesian regression (brms) model using the 12 h social network that included an interaction term between social association strength and dyad sex-category (male-male, male-female or female-female). Females are depicted in red and males in blue respectively. Social association strength has a significant positive association with microbiota similarity in dyads involving males, but not in female-only dyads.

Social network position and microbiota diversity

Both microbiota diversity metrics (richness and Shannon diversity) were predicted by plot region, habitat type, and month (Table S13). Both diversity estimates were also associated with PCR plate, and richness was also predicted by read count. Four measures of network position positively predicted gut microbiota richness: degree and information centrality predicted richness in both 12 h and 2 min networks, and betweenness and bridge propensity additionally predicted richness in the 2 min network (Table 1). No measures of network position predicted Shannon diversity when controlling for covariates (Table S14).

Table 1 Social network centrality metrics predict individual gut microbiota richness.

Identifying bacterial taxa that drive social network effects

The social network effect we identified did not depend entirely on any single bacterial family, since it remained statistically significant in all models where a single bacterial family was excluded (Fig. 6). For some of the more diverse bacterial families, effect size did shift slightly when they were excluded, but not in a way that directly related to their diversity. Excluding the family S24-7 made the social network effect somewhat weaker and almost nonsignificant when using the most intimate (2 min) edge definition (taking the p.MCMC-value from p < 0.001 to p = 0.012), a pattern that was similar but weaker in the 12 h network. Conversely, excluding Lachnospiraceae, the most diverse family, if anything slightly strengthened the social network effect in both networks (Fig. 6). Excluding Lactobacillaceae also slightly weakened the social network effect size, but only when using the less intimate (12 h) edge definition.

Fig. 6: The influence of specific bacterial families on social network effect size.
figure 6

Social network effect sizes (slope of the relationship between social association strength and microbiota similarity, Jaccard Index) and 95% credible intervals are plotted from 146 Bayesian regression (MCMCglmm) models, in which a single bacterial family was excluded from the calculation of microbiota similarity. Effects are plotted against the species richness of each dropped family (logged number of ASVs, y-axis). Results are shown from models using A the least intimate, 12-h network and B the most intimate, 2-min network.

Discussion

Recent studies have shown that the social environment can strongly affect gut microbiota composition in group-living species, such as primates living in large groups [26, 29] or smaller family units [27, 28, 30]. Here, we provide the first evidence for similar effects in a non-group-living species. The social network of wood mice showed no clear clustering, as those of group-living species do. Yet, the social network strongly predicted similarity among individuals in gut microbiota composition, and this effect was far stronger than effects of spatial or temporal proximity, kinship, and similarity in other host attributes (age, sex). In short, mice who were observed at the same location within the same short timeframe, shared more gut bacterial taxa than mice who were observed together less often. This social effect was sufficiently strong that mice who were observed together even once shared more bacterial taxa than mice who were never observed together. This co-occurrence can be seen as a proxy for a more nuanced social relationship, perhaps involving close physical contact behaviours like allogrooming or huddling, that serve as routes for social microbiota transmission.

Social effects on the microbiota can result from social partners having more similar environmental exposures, and previous studies have struggled to separate such influences from the effect of social transmission. Here, several findings suggest the social effect we see is likely driven by social transmission rather than shared exposures. First, we find a strong social network effect even when controlling for host spatial and temporal proximity as well as kinship, reducing the likelihood it is driven by shared traits or exposure to microbes from common environmental sources, such as diet or soil. Second, more intimate definitions of social association (mice co-occurring within a two-minute period, rather than simply during the same night) predicted microbiota similarity more strongly, suggesting close interaction between hosts is important in driving the effect. Finally, the strength of the social network effect varied according to which bacterial families were included in the analysis, in ways that are consistent with a social transmission explanation. When members of the anaerobic, non-spore-forming bacterial family S24-7 (Bacteroidales, Muribaculaceae; [59]) were excluded, the social network effect weakened. Conversely, when members of the spore-forming family Lachnospiraceae were excluded (which are able to survive outside the host and have been found in soil; [60, 61]), the social network effect became slightly stronger. These observations suggest that microbial transmission during close host contact is an important driver of the social effect, allowing hosts to share microbes that cannot persist in the external environment. Previous work in hominids has also shown high host fidelity and even cospeciation with the host among members of the Bacteroidales, while Lachnospiraceae members showed low host fidelity and frequent host switches [62]. Taken together, these findings are consistent with the idea that microbes unable to persist outside the host are more reliant on transmission by close contact (e.g. social behaviour or birth), and perhaps in part because of this, may evolve increased host specificity.

Besides shared environmental exposures, another factor that could have contributed to the social effect we observe here is “artificial” transmission of microbes at the logger boxes we used to monitor mouse behaviour. While impossible to rule out entirely, we think logger contamination is unlikely to have played a significant role in generating the social effects observed here, for several reasons. First, logger boxes were thoroughly sterilised between logging nights (see Appendix 2). Second, our data suggested mice did not spend long periods of time in logger boxes (mean minutes logged per mouse per night when observed was 3.8 +/− sd 2.1), nesting inside loggers was never observed, and faecal pellets inside were also rare. Finally, and importantly, the observed effect of social association on the microbiota was sex-dependent, which is inconsistent with transmission occurring solely at loggers. Specifically, social association strengths derived from logger co-occurrence data were similar in magnitude for all pair types, yet only significantly predicted microbiota similarity for dyads involving males, but not female-female pairs. This implies that the effect of social association on microbiome similarity is driven by behaviours outside logger boxes that are differentially expressed between the sexes.

Indeed, our finding of a sex-dependent effect indicates the link between social interactions and the gut microbiota might be more nuanced than previously thought. It suggests that behaviours which vary in type, frequency or strength according to the sex of social partners are involved in gut microbial transmission. In wood mice, home range overlap is much greater among male-female and male-male dyads than among female-female dyads [39, 63] and observations in captivity suggest allogrooming may be more common between males and females [41]. Limited data has also suggested that co-nesting may be more common for male-female than same-sex pairs [40]. Female wood mice are therefore expected to socially interact with one another less often, and female-female links in our social networks may reflect actual social contact to a lesser extent than male-female and male-male links. In line with our findings, a recent study found that interactions involving males were more important for the transmission of a herpesvirus pathogen in wood mice [64]. It is therefore possible that in this species, the spread of infectious agents more broadly is dominated by interactions involving males. Our findings seem to constitute a mirror image of the common trend in primates, where female-female social bonds are often physically closer than male-male bonds [65], and where social interactions among females have been shown to predict microbiota similarity more strongly than those among males [66, 67]. In pair-bonding species like humans, the strongest microbiota-homogenising effects of social interaction may occur in close sexual relationships [37]. Interestingly, in wood mice (which do not pair-bond), we find no evidence that male-female associations predict microbiota similarity more strongly than male-male associations. This might be because sexual relationships are not well-captured by our measure of social association, or because other social behaviours prevalent among males are more important in transmission of gut microbes than behaviours specific to mixed-sex pairs.

In addition to social contact homogenising the gut microbiota, we also found that the diversity of an individual’s microbiota is predicted by their position in the social network. Individuals with a central position in the social network, particularly with many contacts or in positions that bridged different parts of the network, carried more bacterial taxa in their gut. Of all network metrics, the strongest predictor of microbiota richness was the number of others an individual was connected to in the network (their degree). Similar trends were previously reported in sifakas [28] and chimpanzees [29], and humans self-reporting more social relationships also had greater gut microbial diversity [67]. However, effects in the opposite direction have also been found. In barn-swallows, the extent of same-sex social interaction was negatively correlated with microbiota diversity [68] and in red-bellied lemurs, the most sociable individuals had the lowest gut microbiota diversity [27]. Perhaps a more careful consideration of social connectedness patterns may help in understanding how sociability might shape microbiota diversity. For example, the sheer amount of social interaction (the definition of sociability in [27]) might be less important in diversifying the microbiota than the number of transmission sources (the definition of sociability in [28]). We find that social interactions predict both alpha- and beta-diversity of the gut microbiota – social network position predicted community richness, and social partners had more similar community compositions. Metacommunity theory predicts that connectivity among local communities (hosts) is critical to explaining overall patterns of diversity. On average, dispersal (microbial transmission through host social interaction) is expected to diversify local communities up to a point, by providing novel colonists and rescuing rare species from extinction, but then cease to be enriching as high dispersal begins to homogenise communities and the best competitors at a regional scale come to dominate and exclude others [69]. In other words, local diversity is expected to be maximal at intermediate average levels of dispersal [70]. If social connectivity is uneven among hosts (as is common in social networks, including ours), a metacommunity could also maintain both diversifying flux and a level of local community uniqueness, that allows competing microbial species to coexist within the metacommunity. In such a network, hosts that interact with many others, especially those likely to harbour distinct microbes, may experience the most diversifying effects of social transmission, compared to those interacting with the same or similar individuals. Consistent with this idea, we found that hosts interacting with others from different parts of the network (with high bridge-type centrality) had more diverse microbiotas, while this was not true for highly connected individuals with more interconnected partners (i.e. with high eigenvector centrality).

Overall, our findings suggest the social environment is an underestimated force shaping the gut microbiota among free-living animals. An important future question then is what role this “social microbiome” [21] plays in host fitness. Besides the pathogenic challenges arising from social contact, which have been acknowledged for some time [71,72,73] there may also be benefits. Our results suggest social transmission affects microbiota attributes that have potential relevance for host health: microbiota diversity, similarity among interacting individuals, and transmission of bacteria that cannot readily persist outside the host. While exact relationships between microbiota diversity and beneficial functions remain poorly understood [74, 75], a diverse microbiota might bring benefits in terms of resisting pathogenic infection [11, 76] or increasing metabolic capacity [1, 77]. Immunological benefits may also result from microbiota similarity among closely interacting individuals. Since symbiotic microbes can be pathogenic in an unaccustomed individual [78, 79], sharing a set of familiar microbes with social partners might help maintain diversity, while minimising the threat of opportunist pathogens [20, 80]. Lastly, if anaerobic, non-spore-forming microbes are less likely to be harmful [22] and more likely to be beneficial, social interactions may facilitate the sharing of functionally important, and perhaps more host-specialist symbionts, such as members of the Bacteroidales [62, 81]. Since such benefits of social behaviour could be present even without any others (e.g. benefits of cooperative behaviour), it is possible that the social transmission of gut microbes could represent an underappreciated force in the early evolution of sociality.