Abstract
The infralimbic (IL) division of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) is a crucial site for the extinction of conditioned fear memories in rodents. Recent work suggests that neuronal plasticity in the IL that occurs during (or soon after) fear conditioning enables subsequent IL-dependent extinction learning. We therefore hypothesized that pharmacological activation of the IL after fear conditioning would promote the extinction of conditioned fear. To test this hypothesis, we characterized the effects of post-conditioning infusions of the GABAA receptor antagonist, picrotoxin, into the IL on the extinction of auditory conditioned freezing in male and female rats. In four experiments, we found that picrotoxin injections performed immediately, 24 h, or 13 days after fear conditioning reduced conditioned freezing to the auditory conditioned stimulus (CS) during both extinction training and extinction retrieval; this effect was observed up to two weeks after picrotoxin infusions. Interestingly, inhibiting protein synthesis inhibition in the IL immediately after fear conditioning prevented the inhibition of freezing by picrotoxin injected 24 h later. Our data suggest that the IL encodes an inhibitory memory during the consolidation of fear conditioning that is necessary for future fear suppression.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Animals rely on fear memories for survival. However, it is equally important for animals to suppress fear after danger has passed. Impairments in the extinction of fear memories is a key feature of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) [1, 2], but the precise reasons for why some memories are resistant to extinction are unclear. Several studies in humans and rodents show that the medial prefrontal cortex is crucial for extinction after Pavlovian fear conditioning [3,4,5,6]. In rodents, the infralimbic cortex (IL) is essential for the extinction of fear memories [3, 4, 7]. Pharmacological manipulations of the IL during extinction training [4, 5, 8, 9] or during the post-extinction consolidation period [8, 10,11,12] influence the acquisition and retrieval of extinction memories.
In addition to its role in extinction learning, recent data suggest that the IL might have a role in other inhibitory learning processes. For example, IL inactivation reduces latent inhibition of fear, whereas stimulation facilitates the extinction of appetitive conditioned responses [13, 14]. This suggests that the IL participates in many forms of inhibition, including inhibitory memories that might be encoded during fear conditioning itself. Interestingly, previous work by the first author has shown that pharmacological inactivation or protein synthesis inhibition in the IL immediately after contextual fear conditioning impairs the subsequent extinction of fear [15]. Similarly, it was also found that silencing the IL several hours after fear conditioning leads to later extinction impairments [16]. This raises the intriguing possibility that the IL is engaged during (or soon after) Pavlovian fear conditioning to encode an inhibitory memory that supports the later extinction of the excitatory fear memory. Consistent with this, there is evidence that aversive avoidance learning increases IL c-fos expression [17] and IL inactivation impairs the consolidation of avoidance [18, 19]. Additionally, Pavlovian fear conditioning increases c-fos expression in the IL [20, 21]. Post-conditioning activation of the IL might represent the encoding of inhibitory engrams in the IL that are required for fear extinction.
Several studies have shown that pharmacological activation of the IL during extinction training facilitates long-term extinction memory [4, 5, 8, 9]. We hypothesized that pharmacological activation of the IL immediately after fear conditioning would also facilitate the subsequent extinction of fear memory. Here we characterized the effects of post-conditioning IL pharmacological stimulation and protein synthesis inhibition on subsequent extinction learning.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
In this study, we used 133 male and female Long-Evans rats obtained from Envigo (200–225 g on arrival). Rats were individually housed in cages within a temperature- and humidity-controlled vivarium and kept on a 14:10 light: dark cycle (lights on at 7 AM). Experiments were performed during the light phase of the cycle. Rats had ad libitum access to standard rodent chow and water. Before behavioral testing, rats were handled for 5 days (~30 sec/rat/day). All experimental procedures were conducted in accordance with the US National Institutes of Health (NIH) Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by the Texas A&M University Institutional Animals Care and Use Committee (IACUC).
Surgery
Rats were anesthetized with isoflurane (5% for induction and 1–2% for maintenance) and secured in a stereotaxic apparatus (Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA) for IL cannulation. Animals received a 10 mg/ml/kg injection of Rimadyl (Zoetis), the incision area on the scalp was shaved, povidone-iodine pads were used for antisepsis, ophthalmic ointment was applied to the eyes, and lidocaine was injected subcutaneously beneath the scalp. An incision was made using a scalpel blade and the scalp was retracted using hemostats. The skull was leveled to place bregma and lambda in the same horizontal plane, and holes were drilled in the skull for placement of the cannula and two jeweler’s screws, which were affixed near the lambdoid suture.
Stainless steel guide cannulae (26 gauge, 9-mm from base of the pedestal; Plastics One or RWD) were implanted using the following coordinates [mm relative to either bregma (AP and ML) or skull surface (DV)]: AP = + 2.65, ML = ± 3.2, DV = −6.3 to −6.5 at a 30° angle. After the cannulae were lowered, dental cement was applied to the skull surface to secure the cannulae to the skull. After surgeries, topical antibiotics (Triple Antibiotic Plus, G&W Laboratories) were applied to the skin surrounding the headcap. Dummy cannulae (33 gauge, extending 1 mm beyond the cannula tip) were inserted into the guide cannulae to prevent clogging. Animals had at least one week for recovery between surgery and behavioral testing.
Drugs and infusion procedures
For drug infusions, animals were transported to the infusion room and stainless-steel injectors (33 gauge) connected to Hamilton syringes mounted on infusion pumps were inserted into the guides. Picrotoxin (PIC 200 ng/µL) was dissolved in 0.5% ethanol-saline and was used to stimulate the IL via disinhibition since it is a GABAA receptor antagonist [22]. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF; 2.5 µg/µl) was dissolved in saline and used to promote plasticity in the IL [23, 24]. Anisomycin (ANISO; 100 µg/µl) was dissolved in 5 N HCl and PBS (pH was corrected using NaOH) and was used to inhibit protein synthesis in the IL [15, 25, 26]. Injections (0.15 µl/min; 0.3 µl total) lasted two minutes and injectors were left in place for one more minute to allow diffusion. The three drugs used in the study have relatively short half-lives (<2 hr), and were not given immediately before the behavioral procedures; all behavioral tests were performed in a drug-free state.
Behavioral apparatus and procedures
All experiments were conducted in 16 standard rodent conditioning chambers (30 × 24 × 21 cm; Med Associates, St Albans, VT), housed inside sound-attenuating cabinets. The chambers had two aluminum sidewalls, a Plexiglas rear, ceiling, and door, and a grid floor. The grid floor consisted of 19 stainless steel bars that were connected to a shock source, and a solid-state grid scrambler for foot shock delivery (Med Associates). Each conditioning chamber was equipped with a loudspeaker for delivering auditory stimuli, a ventilation fan, and a light. Locomotor activity was transduced into an electrical signal by a load cell under the floor of the chamber to automatically measure freezing. Approximately one week after surgeries, rats underwent auditory fear conditioning in context A (chamber lights on, fans off, cabinet doors closed, 3% acetic acid, white transport boxes), which consisted of five tone-footshock pairings after a 3 min baseline period. The tones (CS) were 10 s, 80 dB, 2 kHz; the shocks (US) were 1 mA, 2 s, the intertrial interval (ITI) was 70 s long and animals remained in the chamber for an additional minute after the last pairing. Context exposure consisted of a 10 min stimulus-free period in the extinction context (context B; chamber lights off, fans on, cabinet doors open, 70% ethanol, black floors covering the grid floor, and black transport boxes with bedding). Extinction and extinction retrieval consisted of a 3-min stimulus-free baseline period followed by 45 CS-alone presentations (ITI = 40 s) followed by a 3 min post-trial period.
Histological procedures
Rats were overdosed with sodium pentobarbital (100 mg/kg) and perfused transcardially with 0.1 M PBS followed by 10% formalin. Brains were extracted from the skull and post-fixed in a 10% formalin solution for 24 h followed by a 30% sucrose solution where they remained for a minimum of 48 h. After the brains were fixed, coronal sections (30μm thickness) were made on a cryostat (−20 °C), mounted on subbed microscope slides, and stained with thionin (0.25%) to visualize cannula placements. Only rats with bilateral IL cannula placements were included in the analysis.
Statistical analysis
Data were analyzed with conventional parametric statistics (Prism 9.0; GraphPad Software). Freezing data for the pre-CS baseline (BL) period and for each trial (CS + ITI averages) are shown for conditioning and extinction sessions. Baseline freezing levels for the extinction retrieval sessions were minimal and were not shown. To index extinction retrieval, the first 5 trials of the retrieval session were averaged and compared across groups. When appropriate, Fisher’s LSD post hoc tests were used for pairwise means comparisons. Group sizes were based on previous work from the laboratory. No significant sex differences were observed, so animals from both sexes were collapsed for all analyses (but are distinguished in bar graphs).
Results
Experiment 1: Pharmacological stimulation of the infralimbic cortex immediately after fear conditioning suppresses freezing and facilitates extinction retrieval
To determine whether and how IL activation during fear memory consolidation affects fear retrieval and extinction learning, we infused animals with either VEH or PIC immediately after auditory fear conditioning. As shown in Fig. 1B (left panel), all groups showed similar levels of freezing during the fear conditioning session and showed systematic increases in freezing across the session. This was confirmed in a two-way ANOVA, which revealed a main effect of the conditioning trial (F5, 100 = 21.0, p < 0.01), but no other main effects or interactions (Fs < 0.31, ps > 0.66).
Two days after fear conditioning, extinction was performed. As shown in Fig. 1B (middle panel), there were no group differences in baseline freezing (t18 = 0.190, p = 0.850). However, once the CS was delivered the PIC group exhibited lower freezing than controls throughout the extinction session. A two-way ANOVA performed for the entire session revealed the main effects of the trial (F9, 180 = 6.76, p < 0.01) and drug treatment (F1, 20 = 8.35, p < 0.01), with an interaction between the factors (F9, 180 = 2.75, p < 0.01). Although PIC reduced freezing during the first block of extinction trials, it did not affect freezing during the first CS (Fig. S1). This suggests that PIC affected the extinction, rather than retrieval, of the fear memory. Despite showing lower levels of freezing at the outset of extinction training, VEH- and PIC-treated rats came to exhibit similar levels of freezing by the end of the extinction session.
One day after extinction, animals were returned to the extinction context and tested for extinction retrieval. As shown in Fig. 1B (right panel), the PIC group showed lower levels of freezing than controls during the 5-trial retrieval test (t20 = 3.74, p < 0.01). Hence, PIC treatment immediately after fear conditioning not only decreased freezing during the extinction session but also facilitated extinction retrieval the following day.
Experiment 2: Pharmacological stimulation of the infralimbic cortex 24 h after fear conditioning suppresses freezing and facilitates extinction retrieval
Previous work has shown that BDNF infusions into the IL up to 24 h after fear conditioning can cause a reduction in conditioned freezing similar to what we observed with immediate post-conditioning infusions of PIC [24]. Here we explored whether PIC infusions into the IL 24 h after fear conditioning would also suppress freezing; we included a BDNF group as a positive control. As shown in Fig. 2B (left panel), all groups increased freezing similarly across the conditioning session. This was confirmed by a two-way ANOVA, which revealed a main effect of the conditioning trial (F5, 115 = 36.3, p < 0.01).
Twenty-four hours after conditioning, animals received intra-IL infusions of VEH, PIC or BDNF and underwent extinction 48 h later. As shown in Fig. 2B (middle panel), there were no significant differences between groups (F2,23 = 1.55; p = 0.23) during the baseline period. However, animals that received infusions of PIC or BDNF exhibited lower freezing levels than VEH-treated animals once extinction trials were delivered. PIC two-way repeated measures ANOVA revealed significant main effects of the trial (F9, 207 = 4.05, p < 0.01), treatment (F2,23 = 3.51, p = 0.05), and a significant interaction between these factors (F18,127 = 3.47, p < 0.01) on freezing behavior during the extinction session. Similar to the first experiment, PIC did not affect freezing during the first CS (Fig. S1) and all rats reached similar levels of freezing by the end of the extinction session.
On the following day, animals were subjected to an extinction retrieval test. As shown in Fig. 2B (right panel), both drug-treated groups showed lower levels of freezing than the control group. A one-way ANOVA revealed a significant effect of treatment (F2,23 = 5.28, p = 0.01) and post-hoc comparisons showed that both drug-treated groups had lower levels of freezing than the VEH group (ps < 0.03). Hence, PIC or BDNF infusions made 24 h after conditioning (far outside of the classical consolidation time window) reduce the expression of conditional freezing during extinction and retrieval.
Experiment 3: The effects of picrotoxin infusions in the infralimbic cortex are long-lasting
In the previous experiments, the interval between picrotoxin treatment and extinction training was never longer than two days. Therefore, it is possible that the disruption in freezing we observed were caused by an acute effect of the picrotoxin treatment. To investigate this possibility, we explored whether PIC infusions into the IL either 24 h or 13 days after fear conditioning would suppress freezing and facilitate extinction retrieval (Fig. 3A).
As shown in Fig. 3B (left panel), all groups acquired conditioned freezing similarly during the conditioning session. An ANOVA revealed a main effect of trial (F5, 162 = 20.40, p < 0.01), but no other main effects or interactions (Fs < 0.9, ps > 0.40). After receiving VEH or PIC infusions 1 or 13 days after conditioning, animals underwent extinction training 14 days after conditioning. As shown in Fig. 3B (middle panel), baseline freezing before CS onset was similar in all groups (F2,17 = 1.49, p = 0.24). However, once the extinction session commenced, PIC infusions reduced freezing. This was confirmed by a two-way ANOVA, which revealed significant main effects of trial (F9,270 = 3.14, p < 0.01) and drug treatment (F2, 270 = 31.55, p < 0.01); the interaction of these factors was not significant (F18,270 = 1.28, p = 0.20). Furthermore, post hoc comparisons revealed that the two PIC groups had lower freezing when compared to the control group (ps < 0.01) but were not significantly different from each other (p = 0.95). Similar to the first experiment, PIC did not affect freezing during the first CS (Figure S1).
One day after extinction, animals underwent a retrieval test. As shown in Fig. 3B (right panel), both PIC-treated groups had lower freezing levels than controls during the retrieval test. This was confirmed by a one-way ANOVA (F2,27 = 4.55, p = 0.02). Post-hoc tests revealed significant differences between the two treated groups when compared to the VEH group (ps < 0.05); the two drug-treated groups did not differ from each other (p = 0.94). Overall, these results reveal that intra-IL PIC infusion performed 24 h after conditioning produces a long-lasting (13 days) decrement in subsequent conditional freezing during extinction training. Moreover, fear memories remain susceptible to intra-IL PIC treatment for at least 13 days after conditioning.
Experiment 4: Protein synthesis inhibition in the infralimbic cortex during fear memory consolidation blocks the effects of picrotoxin
The previous experiment suggests that pharmacological stimulation of the IL outside of the consolidation time-window can reduce fear expression during extinction. However, a recent study suggests that protein synthesis inhibition in the IL during consolidation can impair subsequent extinction of fear [15]. Therefore, we performed an experiment combining protein synthesis inhibition immediately after learning and pharmacological stimulation 24 h after learning (Fig. 4A).
As shown in Fig. 4B (left panel), all groups showed similar increases in freezing levels during the conditioning session. This was confirmed by a three-way ANOVA, which showed a main effect of trial (F5, 225 = 50.77, p < 0.01), but no other main effects or interactions (Fs < 1.54, ps > 0.22). Immediately after fear conditioning, animals received intra-IL infusions of anisomycin (ANISO) or VEH, on the next day were treated with intra-IL injections of PIC or VEH and were extinguished two days later. As shown in Fig. 4B (middle panel), all groups had similar levels of freezing during the baseline period of the extinction session (Fs < 0.29, ps > 0.59). However, once extinction commenced the VEH-PIC group exhibited lower levels of freezing than all other groups. This was confirmed in a three-way ANOVA that revealed a main effect of the trial (F9, 405 = 21.2, p < 0.01) and significant interactions between the two drug treatments (F1, 45 = 4.46, p = 0.04), as well as a significant three-way interaction (F9, 324 = 3.081, p < 0.01). Additionally, post hoc tests revealed significant differences between the VEH-PICRO group when compared with all other groups for extinction blocks 1 to 5 (ps < 0.03). Similar to the first experiment, PIC did not affect freezing during the first CS (Figure S1).
The next day animals returned to the extinction context for a retrieval test. As shown in Fig. 4B (right panel), no differences were observed between groups. This was confirmed by a two-way ANOVA which showed no significant main effects or interactions (Fs < 1.418, ps > 0.24). This contrasts with what was observed in other experiments, in which PIC treated animals exhibited less freezing during retrieval when compared to controls. The reason for this difference is not clear, though it is noteworthy that the data variability was higher in this experiment. Ultimately, these data suggest that protein synthesis in the IL during the consolidation of fear is required for the suppression of conditioned freezing by IL picrotoxin infusions.
Discussion
In this study, we show that IL picrotoxin infusions from minutes to days after auditory fear conditioning attenuate conditional freezing and facilitate fear extinction retrieval. Although the effects of pharmacological stimulation of the IL are not restricted to the classic consolidation time-window, protein synthesis inhibition in the IL immediately after fear conditioning attenuated the effects of picrotoxin infusions performed 24 h later. This suggests that protein synthesis in the IL soon after fear conditioning contributes to subsequent extinction learning. Interestingly, we found that fear memories are susceptible to IL picrotoxin up to 13 days after fear conditioning. This reveals that post-training picrotoxin modulates IL processes involved in extinction learning long after cellular consolidation of the fear memory is complete. Ultimately, the present study reveals that fear conditioning recruits the IL to enable subsequent fear extinction.
It is possible that the effects we observed were due to a disruption of fear memory consolidation by IL picrotoxin infusions. However, this is unlikely because freezing to the initial CS in each of the four experiments was spared (Figure S1). The additional analysis shown in Fig. S1 shows that during the first extinction trial, the rats in the PIC-treated groups exhibited less freezing than controls during the ITI, but not during the CS. The divergence of freezing during the ITI, but not the CS, is interesting. One possibility is that the CS retrieves a contextual fear memory that contributes to ITI freezing in controls. Picrotoxin may undermine cross-modal and configural CS-context associations and attenuated ITI freezing.
Early lesion studies indicate that the IL is not necessary for fear memory formation or retrieval per se [3, 4]. Moreover, animals that received both anisomycin immediately after fear conditioning and picrotoxin 24 h later did not show a suppression of freezing. This suggests that picrotoxin did not disrupt fear memory consolidation, but rather augmented an inhibitory memory that was established shortly after fear conditioning. In all four experiments, this resulted in a reduction in conditioned freezing during the extinction training session, and lower conditioned freezing during retrieval (except for Experiment 4). Unexpectedly, enhanced extinction retrieval was not observed in Experiment 4. Methodological differences regarding the timing and number of drug manipulations differences in conditioned freezing levels and increased variability may have masked this effect.
Several studies have demonstrated that IL manipulations performed during extinction training can bidirectionally modulate extinction retrieval the next day [4, 5, 9, 27,28,29]. Several lines of evidence suggest these effects rely on reciprocal connections between the IL and amygdala [21, 30,31,32] and are regulated by projections from the ventral hippocampus to the IL [33, 34]. In all of these cases, activity in the IL or its projections was manipulated during extinction learning or retrieval. Here we show for the first time that IL activation up to 13 days before extinction learning has a similar effect to manipulations of IL activity during or after extinction learning. It remains unclear whether post-conditioning intra-IL PIC infusions suppress freezing and augment extinction learning not only influencing local plasticity within the IL, but also promoting plasticity in IL efferents associated with extinction learning such as amygdala intercalated cells [35, 36] or the nucleus reuniens [37, 38].
The finding that IL is engaged during the consolidation of fear conditioning to drive future extinction learning is paradoxical. However, recent studies have shown that disrupting IL activity up to 12 h after fear conditioning causes extinction deficits [15, 16]. Additionally, it was previously reported that intra-IL BDNF treatment performed 24 h after learning is sufficient to disrupt freezing, mimicking an extinction session [24]. We have now replicated this effect and shown that a similar outcome can be achieved with direct pharmacological stimulation. Taken together, our work and others show that the role of the IL in regulating conditioned freezing is not temporally restricted to the extinction learning experience.
This work points to the interesting possibility that the IL has a general role in inhibitory learning processes that occur in parallel with excitatory fear conditioning. This is consistent with reports that IL is involved in a variety of inhibitory learning processes, including latent inhibition [13, 14] and conditioned inhibition [39]. Accordingly, we propose that post-conditioning infusions of either BDNF or picrotoxin augment the consolidation of an inhibitory engram in the IL that is formed during (or shortly after) fear conditioning. We propose that this inhibitory engram promotes the suppression of freezing during subsequent extinction training and retrieval tests. This process appears to involve protein synthesis in the IL insofar as anisomycin injected in the IL immediately after fear conditioning prevents the picrotoxin effects. This is consistent with recent work showing that social buffering of fear induces plasticity in the IL, and optogenetic reactivation of IL neurons that were active during social interaction is sufficient to suppress later freezing [40]. Inhibitory engrams may be mediated by plasticity at inhibitory synapses and have been postulated to underlie the suppression of behavioral responses that occur during habituation, for example [41].
The establishment of excitatory and inhibitory engrams during fear conditioning has theoretical underpinnings in opponent-process theory [42]. Our results suggest that during fear conditioning, the IL encodes a process that opposes the aversive properties of the US. Although there are very few studies on the contribution of opponent processes to fear conditioning, Fanselow & Tighe (1988) suggested that an inhibitory opponent process defines the effective inter-trial interval in contextual conditioning [43]. Inhibitory engrams encoding opponent processes experienced during fear conditioning may have adaptive value by priming the network for future memory updating. However, it is important to note that the formation of inhibitory memories in the IL during fear memory consolidation remains speculative. Clearly, IL stimulation at multiple time-points after fear conditioning can reduce freezing during both extinction and extinction retrieval; this effect depends on protein synthesis soon after conditioning. However, future studies using either longitudinal physiological recordings or engram labelling techniques are necessary to establish whether these effects derive from inhibitory IL ensembles or whether they result from the disruption of an excitatory memory in the IL that promotes fear.
The observation that intra-IL picrotoxin or BDNF injections [44] facilitate extinction when injected two weeks after fear conditioning is intriguing and demands further investigation. Interestingly, Peters and colleagues (2010) found that when the BDNF injections were performed 24 h before conditioning, there was no effect of the drug [24]. This indicates that whatever the mechanism supporting fear suppression, it depends on fear conditioning. Our current understanding of cellular memory consolidation does not explain how memory becomes susceptible to pharmacological stimulation two weeks after it is acquired. One possibility is that the drug administration procedures (handling and transport) reactivate the fear memory and render it susceptible to disruption by impairing reconsolidation [45]. However, this is unlikely, because IL inactivation after fear memory reactivation has no effect on memory reconsolidation [46]. An alternative interpretation is that PIC infusions produce a general disinhibition in the IL, which disrupts the inhibitory memory formed during or soon after conditioning. Additional work is required to explore this possibility using other IL-related tasks that require behavioral flexibility, such as reversal learning.
In sum, our work suggests that the IL encodes an inhibitory memory during the consolidation of excitatory auditory fear conditioning. This inhibitory trace opposes fear and is promotes the subsequent extinction of fear. Future studies employing engram-tagging techniques are necessary to understand the nature and function of the inhibitory engram encoded in the IL during fear memory consolidation.
References
Milad MR, Pitman RK, Ellis CB, Gold AL, Shin LM, Lasko NB, et al. Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66:1075–82.
Maren S, Holmes A. Stress and fear extinction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41:58–79.
Morgan MA, Romanski LM, LeDoux JE. Extinction of emotional learning: contribution of medial prefrontal cortex. Neurosci Lett. 1993;163:109–13.
Milad MR, Quirk GJ. Neurons in medial prefrontal cortex signal memory for fear extinction. Nature. 2002;420:70–4.
Do-Monte FH, Manzano-Nieves G, Quiñones-Laracuente K, Ramos-Medina L, Quirk GJ. Revisiting the role of infralimbic cortex in fear extinction with optogenetics. J Neurosci. 2015;35:3607–15.
Giustino TF, Maren S. The role of the medial prefrontal cortex in the conditioning and extinction of fear. Front Behav Neurosci. 2015;9:298.
Quirk GJ, Russo GK, Barron JL, Lebron K. The role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in the recovery of extinguished fear. J Neurosci. 2000;20:6225–31.
Awad W, Ferreira G, Maroun M. Dissociation of the role of infralimbic cortex in learning and consolidation of extinction of recent and remote aversion memory. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:2566–75.
Chang CH, Maren S. Medial prefrontal cortex activation facilitates re-extinction of fear in rats. Learn Mem. 2011;18:221–5.
Santini E, Ge H, Ren K, Peña de Ortiz S, Quirk GJ. Consolidation of fear extinction requires protein synthesis in the medial prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2004;24:5704–10.
Burgos-Robles A, Vidal-Gonzalez I, Santini E, Quirk GJ. Consolidation of fear extinction requires NMDA receptor-dependent bursting in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2007;53:871–80.
Sotres-Bayon F, Diaz-Mataix L, Bush DE, LeDoux JE. Dissociable roles for the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and amygdala in fear extinction: NR2B contribution. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:474–82.
Lingawi NW, Westbrook RF, Laurent V. Extinction and latent inhibition involve a similar form of inhibitory learning that is stored in and retrieved from the infralimbic cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2017;27:5547–56.
Lingawi NW, Holmes NM, Westbrook RF, Laurent V. The infralimbic cortex encodes inhibition irrespective of motivational significance. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2018;150:64–74.
Bayer H, Bertoglio LJ. Infralimbic cortex controls fear memory generalization and susceptibility to extinction during consolidation. Sci Rep. 2020;10:15827.
Mukherjee A, Caroni P. Infralimbic cortex is required for learning alternatives to prelimbic promoted associations through reciprocal connectivity. Nat Commun. 2018;9:2727. [published correction appears in Nat Commun. 2019 Jul 12;10(1):3082].
Zhang Y, Fukushima H, Kida S. Induction and requirement of gene expression in the anterior cingulate cortex and medial prefrontal cortex for the consolidation of inhibitory avoidance memory. Mol Brain. 2011;4:4.
Moscarello JM, LeDoux JE. Active avoidance learning requires prefrontal suppression of amygdala-mediated defensive reactions. J Neurosci. 2013;33:3815–23.
Torres-García ME, Medina AC, Quirarte GL, Prado-Alcalá RA. Differential effects of inactivation of discrete regions of medial prefrontal cortex on memory consolidation of moderate and intense inhibitory avoidance training. Front Pharm. 2017;8:842.
Troyner F, Bertoglio LJ. Nucleus reuniens of the thalamus controls fear memory reconsolidation. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2021;177:107343.
Binette AN, Liu J, Bayer H, Crayton KL, Melissari L, Sweck SO, et al. Parvalbumin-positive interneurons in the medial prefrontal cortex regulate stress-induced fear extinction impairments in male and female rats. J Neurosci. 2023;43:4162–73.
Yoon KW, Covey DF, Rothman SM. Multiple mechanisms of picrotoxin block of GABA-induced currents in rat hippocampal neurons. J Physiol. 1993;464:423–39.
Bramham CR, Messaoudi E. BDNF function in adult synaptic plasticity: the synaptic consolidation hypothesis. Prog Neurobiol. 2005;76:99–125.
Peters J, Dieppa-Perea LM, Melendez LM, Quirk GJ. Induction of fear extinction with hippocampal-infralimbic BDNF. Science. 2010;328:1288–90.
Maren S, Ferrario CR, Corcoran KA, Desmond TJ, Frey KA. Protein synthesis in the amygdala, but not the auditory thalamus, is required for consolidation of Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;18:3080–8.
Wanisch K, Wotjak CT. Time course and efficiency of protein synthesis inhibition following intracerebral and systemic anisomycin treatment. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2008;90:485–94.
Vidal-Gonzalez I, Vidal-Gonzalez B, Rauch SL, Quirk GJ. Microstimulation reveals opposing influences of prelimbic and infralimbic cortex on the expression of conditioned fear. Learn Mem. 2006;13:728–33.
Laurent V, Westbrook RF. Inactivation of the infralimbic but not the prelimbic cortex impairs consolidation and retrieval of fear extinction. Learn Mem. 2009;16:520–9.
Sierra-Mercado D, Padilla-Coreano N, Quirk GJ. Dissociable roles of prelimbic and infralimbic cortices, ventral hippocampus, and basolateral amygdala in the expression and extinction of conditioned fear. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:529–38.
Bukalo O, Pinard CR, Silverstein S, Brehm C, Hartley ND, Whittle N, et al. Prefrontal inputs to the amygdala instruct fear extinction memory formation. Sci Adv. 2015;1:e1500251.
Bukalo O, Nonaka M, Weinholtz CA, Mendez A, Taylor WW, Holmes A. Effects of optogenetic photoexcitation of infralimbic cortex inputs to the basolateral amygdala on conditioned fear and extinction. Behav Brain Res. 2021;396:112913.
Hong J, Kim D. Freezing response-independent facilitation of fear extinction memory in the prefrontal cortex. Sci Rep. 2017;7:5363.
Marek R, Jin J, Goode TD, Giustino TF, Wang Q, Acca GM, et al. Hippocampus-driven feed-forward inhibition of the prefrontal cortex mediates relapse of extinguished fear [published correction appears in Nat Neurosci. 2018 Sep;21(9):1291]. Nat Neurosci 2018;21:384–92.
Soler-Cedeño O, Torres-Rodríguez O, Bernard F, Maldonado L, Hernández A, Porter JT. Plasticity of NMDA receptors at ventral hippocampal synapses in the infralimbic cortex regulates cued fear. eNeuro. 2019;6:ENEURO.0354-18.2019.
Amir A, Amano T, Pare D. Physiological identification and infralimbic responsiveness of rat intercalated amygdala neurons. J Neurophysiol. 2011;105:3054–66.
Hagihara KM, Bukalo O, Zeller M, Aksoy-Aksel A, Karalis N, Limoges A, et al. Intercalated amygdala clusters orchestrate a switch in fear state. Nature. 2021;594:403–7.
Ramanathan KR, Jin J, Giustino TF, Payne MR, Maren S. Prefrontal projections to the thalamic nucleus reuniens mediate fear extinction. Nat Commun. 2018;9:4527.
Totty MS, Tuna T, Ramanathan KR, Jin J, Peters SE, Maren S. Thalamic nucleus reuniens coordinates prefrontal-hippocampal synchrony to suppress extinguished fear. Nat Commun. 2023;14:6565.
Ng KH, Sangha S. Encoding of conditioned inhibitors of fear in the infralimbic cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2023;33:5658–70.
Gutzeit VA, Ahuna K, Santos TL, Cunningham AM, Sadsad Rooney M, Muñoz Zamora A, et al. Optogenetic reactivation of prefrontal social neural ensembles mimics social buffering of fear. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020;45:1068–77.
Barron HC, Vogels TP, Behrens TE, Ramaswami M. Inhibitory engrams in perception and memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2017;114:6666–74.
Solomon RL, Corbit JD. An opponent-process theory of motivation. I. Temporal dynamics of affect. Psychol Rev. 1974;81:119–45.
Fanselow MS, Tighe TJ. Contextual conditioning with massed versus distributed unconditional stimuli in the absence of explicit conditional stimuli. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1988;14:187–99.
Rosas-Vidal LE, Do-Monte FH, Sotres-Bayon F, Quirk GJ. Hippocampal–prefrontal BDNF and memory for fear extinction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39:2161–9.
Nader K. Reconsolidation and the dynamic nature of memory. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol. 2015;7:a021782.
Stern CA, Gazarini L, Vanvossen AC, Hames MS, Bertoglio LJ. Activity in prelimbic cortex subserves fear memory reconsolidation over time. Learn Mem. 2013;21:14–20.
Funding
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (R01MH117852 and R01MH065961) and by the WoodNext Foundation.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
HB and SM designed all the experiments and analyses. HB, JHJr, HLV, GMG, and VALJ performed stereotactic surgeries. HB, JHJr, CRO, HLV, GMG, and VALJ performed behavioral experiments. HB analyzed the data. HB and SM wrote the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Bayer, H., Hassell, J.E., Oleksiak, C.R. et al. Pharmacological stimulation of infralimbic cortex after fear conditioning facilitates subsequent fear extinction. Neuropsychopharmacol. (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-024-01961-9
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-024-01961-9
This article is cited by
-
A covert cortical ensemble for learned fear suppression
Neuropsychopharmacology (2024)