Abstract
While a myriad of factors likely contribute to the development of mental illness in young people, the social environment (including early adverse experiences) in concert with neurodevelopmental alterations is undeniably important. A number of influential theories make predictions about how and why neurodevelopmental alterations may mediate or moderate the effects of the social environment on the emergence of mental illness. Here, we discuss current evidence supporting each of these theories. Although this area of research is rapidly growing, the body of evidence is still relatively limited. However, there exist some consistent findings, including increased striatal reactivity during positive affective processing and larger hippocampal volumes being associated with increased vulnerability or susceptibility to the effects of social environments on internalizing symptoms. Limited longitudinal work has investigated neurodevelopmental mechanisms linking the social environment with mental health. Drawing from human research and insights from animal studies, we propose an integrated mediation-moderation model and outline future research directions to advance the field.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
The social environment, particularly experiences of adversity and stress, has profound effects on cognitive, social, and emotional functioning in young people. Childhood adversity is one of the strongest predictors of mental illness onset [1], particularly during the first two decades of life when many mental illnesses first emerge [2]. During this time, the brain is rapidly developing, and alterations in brain development are also thought to contribute to the onset of mental illness [3, 4]. With the ultimate goal of developing better prevention and intervention strategies to reduce the immense burden of child and adolescent mental illness, which has increased substantially in recent decades [5], research and theory have increasingly sought to better understand the role of both neurobiological and environmental factors in mental illness risk. To this end, the aim of the present review is to discuss theories that explore how the dynamics between the social environment and neurobiology intersect to shape the emergence of mental illness in young people. Drawing upon available evidence, with a specific emphasis on human neuroimaging studies, we aim to provide insights into the validity of these theories. We conclude with recommendations for a more integrated theoretical model and suggest avenues for future research in this area.
A key insight from developmental neuroscience, relevant to understanding the integrative role of the social environment and the brain in mental health and illness, is brain plasticity. Brain plasticity, which is substantially higher during childhood and adolescence relative to adulthood, refers to the ability of the brain to reorganize neural pathways based on input from the environment [6]. The overproduction and elimination of immature synapses during childhood and adolescence, driven by both intrinsic programming and environmental influences, are thought to be key mechanisms of plasticity [6, 7]. However, other mechanisms include the overproduction of neurons in early development, apoptosis or programmed cell death of excessive neurons, and at the molecular level, central nervous system receptor alterations [6, 7].
Of particular relevance to the development of mental illness in young people is the notion that social environmental exposures during periods of increased brain plasticity may alter neurodevelopment. Altered neurodevelopment may, in turn, increase or decrease susceptibility to the development of mental health problems. Such environmental input may have these effects via experience-expectant or experience-dependent processes [8, 9]. Experience-expectant processes refer to changes resulting from the presence or absence of environmental stimuli that the neural system has evolved to expect early in life (e.g., the presence of a sensitive and responsive caregiver vs. emotional neglect, provision of food and shelter vs. physical neglect, exposure to sensory and linguistic experiences vs. deprivation of such experiences) [10]. During such periods, expected inputs are thought to have an increased impact on brain organization, triggering periods of increased plasticity (i.e., sensitive periods). Conversely, experience-dependent plasticity refers to the brain’s ability to reorganize and adapt in response to individual experiences and environmental stimuli. Experience-dependent plasticity is thought to occur throughout the lifespan but diminishes as the brain matures. Adverse experiences that contribute to experience-dependent plasticity might include the presence of traumatic or stressful stimuli/events that threaten or are perceived to threaten (from an evolutionary standpoint) survival (e.g., physical or sexual abuse, exposure to crime, domestic violence, natural disaster, social exclusion). Positive social experiences that may also impact neurodevelopment via experience-dependent mechanisms include supportive family and peer relationships [11,12,13], exposure to stimulating educational settings [14, 15], etc.
Building on the notion of plasticity, a number of theories and models have emerged that consider the role of neurodevelopmental processes in the link between the social environment and the emergence of mental illness during childhood and adolescence. These theories serve to provide testable hypotheses, direct research efforts and move the field forward to advance knowledge, with the ultimate goal of clinical and policy translation. These theories differ in their developmental versus evolutionary focus, whether they take a deficit versus strengths-based approach, and whether they focus on adaptive versus maladaptive processes. They also differ in whether they primarily focus on neurobiology or neurodevelopment as a mechanism (i.e., mediator) versus a moderator of environmental-mental health associations (see Box 1). We next discuss these theories and their current empirical evidence, grouped by whether they take a mediation versus moderation approach.
Models of neurodevelopmental mechanisms linking the social environment with psychopathology (i.e., mediation models)
Theories aiming to explain neurodevelopmental mechanisms linking adverse social environmental experiences with mental health propose a causal chain whereby adverse experiences directly impact the developing brain, which in turn increases the risk of developing psychopathology. One such theory is the Stress Acceleration Hypothesis [SAH [16], which builds on evolutionary theories [including Life History Theory [17] that posit faster biological development to be an evolutionary adaptive strategy that allows for earlier reproduction in contexts of adversity. The SAH suggests that adverse experiences (particularly relating to the caregiving environment) accelerate neurodevelopmental processes to reach “adult-like” functioning earlier. Specifically, the hypothesis refers to the accelerated development of brain regions and connections considered important for emotion processing, emotion regulation, and memory (specifically, amygdala-prefrontal circuitry); because in situations defined by missing or inconsistent parental care, it may be adaptive to transition from a state of parent-regulation to self-regulation sooner. The SAH also argues that the precocious development of this circuitry, while initially adaptive, could contribute to impairment in emotion regulation abilities, thus having negative consequences for later mental health.
Although the SAH originally posited for this acceleration to occur in the context of caregiver-related adversity, this model has been extended and applied more broadly to early adverse experiences characterized by chronic stress [18]. Tooley et al. argue that chronic stress (which may occur, for example, in the context of socioeconomic disadvantage) may lead to accelerated brain development via the process described by the SAH, but other mechanisms may include repeated use of stress-regulation circuitry, and/or via increasing glucocorticoid levels and activation of inflammatory processes [18]. Specific neural circuits are not specified, however, and predictions are made about brain development in general. Rakesh et al. [19], similarly speculating about both adversity and brain development more generally, have suggested that adversity (specifically low socioeconomic status) is likely to be associated with a distinct developmental pattern involving slower brain development across childhood and adolescence. They make this prediction based on longitudinal evidence of socioeconomic disadvantage being associated with a slower rate-of-change and persistently lower cortical thickness, area, and volume throughout childhood and adolescence [19], and suggest that these brain changes mediate links with poor mental health (and other) outcomes. Specific mechanisms are suggested to include impaired synaptic pruning and slower neural circuit refinement. Another theory with similarities to SAH, the latent vulnerability theory [20], suggests that early maltreatment creates an underlying latent vulnerability, increasing future mental health risk. This latent vulnerability is suggested to involve alterations to one or more of several neural systems, including those that modulate threat processing, reward-processing, executive control, and emotion regulation. Notably, this theory does not specify that these alterations necessarily reflect accelerated (or delayed) neural development.
Another extension of the SAH was proposed by Colich et al. [21], who suggested that stress-associated acceleration may depend on the type of adversity experienced, in line with the Dimensional Model of Adversity and Psychopathology (DMAP) [22,23,24]. The DMAP proposes that adversity can be classified into two main types: threat and deprivation. Experiences of threat and harm (such as physical, sexual, and emotional abuse) can be classified as “threat”, whereas the lack of expected caring and nurturing input (e.g., lack of physical and emotional support, and cognitive/social stimulation) can be classified as experiences of deprivation. The theory posits that while they often co-occur, threat and deprivation have at least partially distinct neurodevelopmental correlates. Colich et al. [21], similarly to Tooley et al. and Rakesh et al., went beyond the idea of acceleration of the amygdala-prefrontal circuit, and discussed the idea of biological aging more globally. They proposed the idea that experiences characterized by threat, but not deprivation, would be associated with accelerated biological aging, including accelerated gray matter and amygdala-prefrontal connectivity development. A recent integrated model of dimensions of environmental experience [25] builds on these ideas further, suggesting that deprivation is likely to have complex effects, particularly relating to whether it is predictable vs. unpredictable, or within proximal vs. distal environments. For example, it is suggested that when experiences of deprivation are unpredictable, the neurobiology supporting some aspects of executive functioning (e.g., inhibitory control, working memory capacity) will show a delayed development, while alterations to other aspects may be mitigated in the case where these functions are important for survival (e.g., cognitive flexibility allows for flexibly switching between mental sets).
The recently developed change of pace theory [26] provides a similar account to the aforementioned models, insofar as it proposes different neurodevelopmental trajectories depending on specific forms of adversity experienced in childhood. The change of pace hypothesis contends that the type of adversity encountered determines whether biological maturation is expedited or retarded by focusing on the parent-child dyad, and suggests that changes in the rate of development occur primarily to eliminate gaps in parental caregiving. They purport that delaying maturation specifically lowers children’s total physiological requirements when there are unmet physiological needs, like in situations of deprivation or neglect (e.g., in case of inadequate nutrition or parental neglect). In the event of threat or abuse however, if there are unmet safety needs (e.g., shelter from threat or protection), accelerated development would boost children’s general ability for autonomous safety requirements provision, with this acceleration resulting in a later (post-childhood) stunting of developmental autonomy. While change of pace theory does not provide a comprehensive account of neural circuits implicated, the function and neural regulation of the HPA-axis is discussed as likely critical.
Empirical evidence for mediation models from longitudinal studies
While others have reviewed evidence for the aforementioned models, they have included both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies [18, 21] or have not focused on studies investigating mental health outcomes using statistical tests for mediation (e.g., [19]). Given that most of these models make predictions about developmental trajectories, cross-sectional data is arguably inappropriate for testing them [19]. This is not to say that cross-sectional examinations of brain structure, function or connectivity cannot provide useful information about the impact of environmental exposures on development. Rather, longitudinal research is needed to truly test deviations from typical developmental trajectories as proposed by the aforementioned models. Further, studies investigating associations between adverse social environments and brain development, without investigating links with mental health outcomes, are useful for testing these models (e.g., as reviewed by [18, 19, 21]). Here, however, we focus only on studies that investigated mental health outcomes using statistical tests of mediation, so that interpretation of the adaptive versus maladaptive nature of adverse environmental impacts on brain development was possible. As such, we exclusively review studies investigating longitudinal neurobiological mediators of the associations between adverse social environmental variables and mental health outcomes. Finally, we did not restrict our review of studies based on the specific type of adverse social environment assessed.
Some support for the SAH, Tooley et al., and the latent vulnerability theory has come from studies investigating longitudinal resting-state connectivity changes in adolescents. Links identified with mental health point to both adaptive and maladaptive neurobiological changes following adversity. Rakesh et al. [27], for example, found both childhood abuse and neglect to be associated with increases in within-salience network (SN) resting-state connectivity between ages 16 to 19, and this pattern of connectivity mediated associations with lower problematic substance use and higher depressive symptoms. This pattern of connectivity was suggested to reflect more advanced, or accelerated, development of circuitry responsible for the evaluation of internal and external states, thus consistent with the SAH/Tooley et al. The involvement of a network responsible for the detection of salient affective stimuli is consistent with latent vulnerability theory. The links identified with different mental health outcomes were intriguing. Given the role of the SN in reward processing [28], Rakesh et al. suggested that maltreatment-associated increases in SN connectivity may be associated with reduced reward sensitivity, which may decrease the likelihood of adolescents engaging in risky but rewarding behaviors such as substance use, and at the same time increase the risk for depression, which is associated with dampened reward function [29].
There have been similar reports of adversity-related alterations in resting-state connectivity development being associated with more positive mental health outcomes. For example, Brieant et al. [30] showed that lifetime negative life experiences were linked to reductions in cortico-limbic resting-state functional connectivity over time during early adolescence (from age 9 to 13), which, in turn, was associated with lower internalizing symptoms. Based on normative patterns of connectivity development, the authors suggest that this finding reflects accelerated maturation of neural circuitry responsible for emotion processing and regulation (consistent with SAH/Tooley et al./latent vulnerability theory), which is adaptive and beneficial for wellbeing. Further discussion on the possibility of these neurobiological alterations reflecting adaptive brain phenotypes is provided in subsequent sections.
In alignment with the integrated model of dimensions of environmental experience and change of pace theory, Rakesh et al. [31] found that increases in connectivity between cortical networks (particularly the default mode, frontoparietal, dorsal attention and salience networks) from age 16 to 19 mediated the relationship between childhood maltreatment and later depressive symptoms, with findings potentially driven by experiences of neglect. Given that between-network connectivity normatively decreases across adolescence, and increased connectivity between functionally distinct networks has been linked to poor cognitive performance [32], the authors suggested that findings may reflect a mechanism by which delayed functional connectivity development underlies cognitive impairments in adolescents with depression and a history of deprivation. Similarly, Hanson et al. [33] found that emotional neglect was associated with blunted development of reward-related ventral striatal activity between early and late adolescence (approximately 11–17 years of age). Additionally, blunted reward-related ventral striatal activity mediated the association between neglect and greater depressive symptoms approximately 1 year later. This finding may be interpreted to reflect delayed development of reward-related neural circuitry, which may be particularly relevant to symptoms of anhedonia associated with depression.
Regarding brain structural development, two studies have implicated hippocampal development in patterns consistent with different models. Miller et al. [34] found that greater perinatal adversity (e.g., low birth weight, low family income, maternal mental health problems) was associated with increased growth in the right hippocampal body across childhood (from age 4.5 to 7.5). Further, adversity was indirectly associated with late childhood (age 8.5) depressive symptoms via increased hippocampal body growth. Conversely, Barch et al. [35] found that early childhood poverty was related to reduced testosterone increases. This was, in turn, linked to smaller initial volume and reduced volumetric growth of the hippocampus during the transition from school age to adolescence (i.e., age 7 to 12). These neurobiological changes mediated associations with elevated emotion dysregulation and depressive symptoms during late adolescence (age ~19). As such, these two studies identify opposite findings of increased and reduced hippocampal development potentially reflecting maladaptive patterns of development contributing to increased vulnerability to the development of depression. In the former case, findings may reflect accelerated development following adversity, consistent with SAH/Tooley et al., whereas in the latter case, findings may reflect a delay in development, consistent with Rakesh et al.’s model, integrated model of dimensions of environmental experience, and change of pace theories (to the extent that poverty may reflect deprivation experiences).
While these studies provide some support for the aforementioned mediation models, there are a number of examples of relevant studies with null findings [36,37,38,39,40,41], clearly highlighting the need for further research in this area. Future work should also address limitations of the current evidence base. In particular, in many of the reviewed studies, conclusions about accelerated or delayed neurobiological development are limited, given (1) studies do not examine the full span of neurodevelopment (from birth to adulthood), (2) normative patterns of neurodevelopment (from which to compare) are not necessarily known (particularly for measures of brain function or functional connectivity), and (3) non-linear growth is rarely investigated [despite the normative non-linear brain developmental patterns for a number of neural metrics [42]. It is important to note, however, that a few studies exist that address some of these issues using methods other than mediation to test links with mental health [43].
Models of neurodevelopmental factors that modify associations between social environmental factors and mental health (i.e., moderation models)
Person-by-environment interaction frameworks are commonly used to understand individual risk or susceptibility to mental health problems. Such conceptual models have a common tenet that people are variably influenced by environmental factors, and one source of this variability is likely person-centered factors. These include biologically-based person-centered factors, including aspects of brain structure and function. Within the literature, there are two broad conceptualizations of person-by-environment or biology-by-environment interactions. Developmental theories purport that there is an optimal trajectory of human development that adversity can derail. Evolutionary-developmental theories argue against the existence of a single optimal trajectory of development. Instead, they posit that because humans have evolved to develop in various environments, both adverse and supportive, there may be multiple adaptive developmental trajectories. In this sense, optimal development is framed as context-dependent. Notably, these theories are not particularly specific regarding the types of relevant social environmental factors; rather, there is a general focus on early social environments that are either negative/adverse/marked by chronic stress or positive/supportive/low stress. While we do not cover all relevant models here, we focus on key models where measures of neurobiology have been utilized to examine interactions between biology and environment in relation to psychopathology in children and adolescents.
The broad class of developmental theories includes the dual-risk model [44], diathesis-stress theory [45], and cumulative stress theory [46]. These models generally argue that some individuals possess inherent vulnerabilities to poor developmental outcomes including psychopathology, which can be triggered by environmental stressors. Cumulative stress theory suggests that if there is an accumulation of stressors across the lifespan that exceeds a certain threshold, at-risk individuals are more likely to develop psychopathology [46]. Most formulations of these models suggest that the inherent vulnerability (or diathesis) factors are biological [47,48,49].
Evolutionary-developmental models, including the Differential Susceptibility model [50] and the biological sensitivity to context model [51], argue that individuals vary in their susceptibility to environmental influence, for better and worse. That is, highly susceptible individuals benefit more from supportive environments and suffer more in adverse circumstances compared to those with low susceptibility. Differential susceptibility developed from evolutionary theory [50], and biological sensitivity to context was formed in response to unexpected findings revealing that children with high immune reactivity exhibited the highest rates of illness in high-stress family environments and lowest illness occurrence in low-stress environments [51, 52]. Together, these frameworks, hereafter referred to as “differential susceptibility” due to their substantial overlap [53, 54], argue that susceptibility is an evolutionary-adaptive characteristic that is instantiated biologically early in life through genetics, intermediate phenotypes (e.g., the brain), and behavioral phenotypes. Differential susceptibility theory does not discount that dual-risk/diathesis-stress processes exist; rather, it argues that the same biological factor could lead to different outcomes in different contexts. If supportive environments and positive outcomes are not investigated, then differential susceptibility effects may be overlooked and mislabeled as vulnerability effects [55].
Extension to these theories include the match/mismatch hypothesis, which stems from evolution theory, and suggests that individuals with high “programming sensitivity” are more likely to be at mental health risk if a mismatch occurs between the early programming environment and the later adult environment. The benefits and costs of high programming sensitivity (which might take the form of higher physiological reactivity, for example) are context-dependent. Individuals with high programming sensitivity adapt to early environments in preparation for similar future experiences. If a context is encountered in the future that does not match the early environment, then there may be poor mental health outcomes for the individual. These individuals, however, are suggested to benefit from a match of early life and adult environment, even if both are aversive [56]. A similar idea was captured by the 3-hit hypothesis (where the first “hit” represents a specific genetic background) [57].
Empirical evidence for moderation models
The neural mechanisms underlying vulnerability or sensitivity to the social environment have been increasingly investigated by researchers interested in testing biology-by-environment interactions. Below, we review studies that have sought to test dual-risk/diathesis-stress and differential susceptibility theories, as currently there is limited human empirical work that has sought to test the nuances of some of the other theories. Notably, we only review studies that test the interaction between a social environmental variable and brain structure of function in association with mental health. Cross-sectional or longitudinal studies are reviewed given that these theories make no predictions about brain development per se. Again, we do not constrain our review of studies based on the negative or positive social environmental variables investigated.
A number of studies have investigated neural function during exposure to positive or rewarding stimuli in adolescents (e.g., [58,59,60]). Most results have supported dual-risk/diathesis-stress effects, with greater striatal activation to such stimuli being associated with higher internalizing [59, 61] or externalizing [60] symptoms in the context of adverse environments (e.g., high family conflict, [60, 61]; peer victimization [59]). These results may be interpreted as indicating that youth who are highly (neurally) sensitive to reward may be highly tuned to social interactions with friends and family. As a result, they may be more likely to be negatively affected by adverse family and peer environments, and more vulnerable to developing mental health problems as a result.
Others have found support for neural reactivity to positive stimuli/reward as reflecting differential susceptibility. Liu et al. [58], for example, formally (statistically) tested for differential susceptibility effects, and found that heightened amygdala reactivity to positive stimuli correlated with negative outcomes, such as increased internalizing symptoms and reduced prosocial behavior, in environments marked by negative family dynamics, such as low maternal warmth or imbalanced family functioning. Conversely, increased amygdala reactivity was associated with better outcomes, including lower symptom levels and greater prosocial behavior, in positive family settings characterized by high maternal warmth and balanced family functioning. Therefore, amygdala reactivity to positive stimuli may indicate differential susceptibility (rather than vulnerability alone) to the family environment.
Fewer studies have investigated neural activation associated with negative emotion processing (e.g., [62,63,64]) or other aspects of neural function (e.g., cognitive functioning [65]). These studies, conducted in late-childhood and adolescent samples, largely report findings consistent with dual-risk/diathesis-stress, with alterations in the function of regions and networks including the anterior cingulate cortex [63], inferior parietal sulcus [62], posterior cingulate, temporoparietal junction and amygdala [64], and executive network [65] being associated with increased externalizing problems when exposed to negative social environments (including low parental support [62], family disconnection [63], neighborhood crime [64] and neighborhood threat [65]).
A few studies have found patterns of resting-state connectivity to be associated with vulnerability to internalizing symptoms in adolescents, consistent with dual-risk/diathesis-stress. For example, lower coupling between the amygdala and vmPFC [66] and higher initial but steeper decreases in average whole-brain connectivity from age 9 to 19 years [67], have been associated with higher internalizing symptoms in the context of more averse environments (stressful life events [66], unpredictable negative life events [67]). In another study, lower connectivity in the central executive network and higher anti-correlation between the salience and default mode networks were associated with differential susceptibility in adolescents, whereby for adolescents with these patterns of connectivity, high maternal hostility was associated with higher anxiety symptoms, whereas low maternal hostility was associated with lower anxiety symptoms [68].
A number of studies have investigated cortical and subcortical structure as possible diathesis or susceptibility markers [69,70,71,72,73,74,75,76,77]. Most studies have focused on the adolescent period, while some have investigated infants [71, 73]. The hippocampus has been particularly implicated in these studies, with several studies of adolescents suggesting that larger hippocampal volume may confer vulnerability or perhaps susceptibility to environmental exposures, being associated with greater depressive symptoms in the context of more negative environments (high family conflict, family disconnection, high community crime, high levels of aggressive parenting behavior) and fewer depressive symptoms in the context of more positive environments (low family conflict, family connection, low community crime, low levels of aggressive parenting behavior) [70, 74, 75]. As suggested by Schriber et al. [74], given the role of the hippocampus in complex contextual memory processing [78], and findings of larger hippocampal volumes being associated with superior episodic learning and memory ability [79], findings may suggest that adolescents with larger hippocampal volumes process their social contexts more deeply, rendering them more susceptible to the influences of both positive and negative experiences. Notably, larger hippocampal volumes were found in one study of infants to reflect a susceptibility marker to maternal sensitivity in relation to attachment outcomes [73], indicating that larger hippocampal volumes may be a susceptibility marker across different developmental periods. Also notable is that there is some evidence for sex differences in this effect, with one study reporting a susceptibility effect for females but not males [75]. The interpretation of this sex difference is unclear but could reflect sex differences in neurodevelopmental trajectories leading to sex differences in specific periods of neuroplasticity.
Summary of support for models
Although not a systematic review of the literature, the studies discussed do offer some degree of support for many of the discussed models. Two relatively consistent findings emerged. Increased striatal reactivity to positive/rewarding stimuli appeared to reflect a vulnerability or diathesis, associated with high internalizing problems in the context of a negative social environment. Larger hippocampal volumes appeared to reflect a susceptibility marker, related to higher internalizing problems in the context of a negative social environment and fewer problems in the context of a positive social environment. There are currently too few longitudinal studies, however, to make strong conclusions about consistent effects of mediation models. Below we discuss how the reviewed theories and models may be integrated to guide future research and move the field forward.
Integration of models
The aforementioned theories and models are not mutually exclusive. It is possible that they all accurately describe aspects of the complex role of neurobiology in linking the social environment with mental health. It is also possible, if not very likely, that the environment shapes neurodevelopment, and patterns of neurodevelopment determine ongoing vulnerability or susceptibility to environmental exposures. It is important to point out that the descriptions of the theories provided above are relatively brief, and some of the theories provide far more complex accounts that provide a basis for integration across mediation and moderation models. For example, biological sensitivity to context theory suggests that increased sensitivity is driven by early environmental exposures, such that those growing up in more supportive or more adverse early environments are hypothesized to become more sensitive to future environments. In the former case, it would be adaptive to be maximally influenced by the developmental environment, and in the latter case, it would be adaptive to be vigilant to threats in order to increase chances of survival and eventual reproduction [53]. Latent vulnerability theory suggests that one of the ways that latent vulnerabilities, induced by early experiences of maltreatment, may increase risk is by altering responses to future stressors [20].
Few human neuroimaging studies have attempted to test such complex models involving neurobiology as both a mechanism/mediator and moderator of associations between the social environment and mental health outcomes, although we note one recent study that investigated prenatal adversity [80]. In this study, prenatal adversity (e.g., prenatal maternal mental health problems and poverty) was associated with a steeper decrease in structure-function connectivity coupling (SC-FC) from early- to mid-childhood, potentially suggestive of accelerated neurodevelopment. SC-FC during early childhood moderated the association between prenatal adversity and both internalizing and externalizing problems during mid-childhood. Specifically, high early childhood SC-FC appeared to act as a diathesis, such that only children with high SC-FC experienced poor mental health in the context of high prenatal adversity.
Here, we build on the reviewed theories and models, and together with insights from the reviewed studies, we propose an integrated mediation-moderation model of the role of neurodevelopment in linking the social environment with mental health (Fig. 1). We focus on adversity associated with the experiences of stress, but do not make predictions about specific dimensions of adversity. We make specific predictions in relation to hippocampal volume. However, we also discuss how the model may extend more broadly to neural circuitry supporting the detection and processing of salient affective stimuli.
We suggest that both positive and negative early environments may drive neurobiological changes that increase sensitivity to future environments, consistent with predictions from the biological sensitivity to context theory [53]. This may involve accelerated maturation of the hippocampus, to support increased contextual learning and memory processing [81]. Accelerated maturation of the hippocampus following early adversity is consistent with Miller et al.’s [34] finding (reviewed above) of perinatal adversity being associated with increased growth in hippocampal volume across early childhood. Accelerated maturation of neural systems supporting emotion and memory processing following adverse environments is also consistent with the SAH. We suggest that exposure to particularly positive environments may similarly lead to accelerated hippocampal maturation. Such a notion is consistent with animal research showing that both stressful and enriching early environments result in neurobiological markers of accelerated maturation in the hippocampus [82, 83]. This early accelerated maturation resulting in relatively larger hippocampal volumes, might persist throughout development, rendering individuals continuously sensitive to positive and negative environments—a concept consistent with differential susceptibility or biological sensitivity to context. This proposition is also consistent with the reviewed findings: larger hippocampal volumes in both infancy and adolescence were associated with increased susceptibility to both negative and positive environments; in the context of more negative environments, those with larger hippocampal volumes experienced internalizing symptoms, but in the context of positive environments, these individuals experienced fewer symptoms than their less susceptible peers [70, 73,74,75].
Following initial accelerated maturation in response to early adverse environments, we propose that hippocampal growth may slow after a period of time, due to the effects of the early adversity and associated stress on growth programs. In those exposed to early positive environments, growth subsequent to initial acceleration is not affected. The notion of early adversity being linked with an initial accelerated maturation of the hippocampus, combined with stunting of its growth, is consistent with the change of pace theory predictions about development following threat to safety needs. It is also consistent with animal work. Specifically, animal work has shown that early adversity or stress leads to markers of accelerated hippocampal maturation (precocious arrival of Parvalbumin-positive cells, earlier switch in NMDA receptor subunit expression, earlier rise in myelin basic protein expression), combined with markers of reduced hippocampal growth (earlier developmental decline in expression of markers of cell proliferation and differentiation) [82]. Accelerated maturation may be adaptive and have short-term advantages, but at the expense of truncated growth, leading to longer-term poor outcomes and risk for mental health problems. As reviewed above, Barch et al. [35] found early adversity (poverty) to be associated with reduced hippocampal growth from late childhood through adolescence, and this reduced growth was associated with higher depressive symptoms. Slowed hippocampal growth has been associated with the development of mental health problems, such as depression, in other human work (see also [84]).
In summary, we suggest that early exposure to adverse, stressful environments and positive environments, may result in an initial accelerated maturation of the hippocampus. Larger resulting volumes may promote increased susceptibility to future positive and negative/stressful environmental exposures. Early adverse/stressful exposures, however, result in subsequent reduced hippocampal growth, which leads to increased internalizing symptoms and potentially other poor outcomes. Figure 1 provides an overview of the proposed hippocampal alterations that may occur following adverse and positive environmental exposures across development.
We suggest that there may be similar effects for other brain structures and circuits involved in the detection and processing of salient positive and negative environmental stimuli, including the amygdala and connectivity of the SN. The amygdala is involved in the detection and processing of both positive and negative salient affective stimuli [85] and modulates physiological stress responses [86]. The SN, which involves connections between the amygdala and other midline regions, including the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, anterior insula, and putamen, is involved in vigilance toward social affective stimuli and, via connections with default mode and cognitive control networks, the generation of appropriate behavioral responses [87].
Existing work partly supports the role of these regions and networks as both mediators and moderators of links between the social environment and mental health in children and adolescents. For example, animal work has shown effects of early adversity on markers of accelerated maturation in the amygdala, similar to those observed in the hippocampus [88]. In humans, adversity has been found to predict larger amygdala volumes in childhood but smaller volumes in adolescence [89], potentially reflecting early acceleration followed by delayed growth. Moreover, increased amygdala reactivity to affective/salient stimuli has been proposed as a differential susceptibility marker [20], and this was supported in the study described above by Liu et al. [58]. Larger amygdala volume has also been found to confer differential susceptibility to depression outcomes in adolescent males [76]. Regarding the SN, Rakesh et al. [27], reviewed above, found that childhood maltreatment was associated with altered development of salience network connectivity. Others have theorized that SN connectivity represents a biomarker for differential susceptibility [90], and this was supported in the study described above by Ding et al. [68]. Further research is needed to test these predictions, including whether they apply to specific types of negative and positive environments, whether chronicity and severity are important factors, when precisely a switch from accelerated maturation to delayed growth occurs, and which specific measures of hippocampal, amygdala, and SN structure, function and connectivity are relevant.
Below, we discuss some of the key limitations in the literature, including areas of theories and models that require further development, in addition to limitations of the current empirical research. Although not comprehensive, we believe they represent important issues that if addressed, will move the field forward considerably.
Limitations of the current literature, and future directions
Deficit-focused models and the idea of resilience
With the exception of differential susceptibility theory and our proposed integrated mediation-moderation model, the prominent focus of the aforementioned models, and much of the empirical literature, has been around risk factors and deficits associated with brain development and mental health. Although such deficit-focused research has provided valuable insights into neurodevelopment and the neurobiological basis of mental illness, it may have oversimplified the complexity of brain and psychological development, with other factors, such as protective factors, often overlooked. Additionally, findings from deficit-focused neurodevelopmental research may contribute to the stigmatization of mental illness, given the emphasis of “problems” associated with mental illness as opposed to acknowledging strengths and potentials for recovery and growth. Shifting focus toward resilience rather than risk could potentially address these limitations.
Recent reviews on the topic of resilience neurobiology have demonstrated that the structure and function of threat, reward, and cognitive control neural networks may be associated with resilience in adults [91,92,93], that is, associated with positive (or absence of negative) mental health outcomes following adversity. However, our recent systematic review on the subject found that fewer studies have investigated the role of brain development in resilience to poor mental health outcomes in children and adolescents [94].
Further exploration of resilience, particularly in the context of neurobiology, holds promise for informing preventive and therapeutic interventions aimed at promoting mental well-being in youth. It is important to note, however, that while early conceptualizations of resilience suggested it to be an individual trait, this approach has been criticized, including by those suggesting that it may result in power imbalances and discrimination within society [95]. More contemporary conceptualizations emphasize resilience to be a dynamic multisystem process of adaptation post experiences of significant negative experiences; a process that involves brain (and other aspects of biological) development, and other contextual factors at the family, community, and broader levels [96]. Future work, however, is needed to address heterogeneity in the definition of resilience and how best to operationalize and test it [see detailed discussion in [94].
Adaptation vs. maladaptation
Although the different discussed theories describe both adaptive and maladaptive brain changes, as described above, the current relevant mental health research primarily takes a deficit approach. Any adaptive brain changes identified (in terms of mental health outcomes) tend to be identified “accidentally” (see [94]). Further work is needed, for example, to identify whether brain changes following adversity are adaptive in the short term but maladaptive longer-term, whether they are adaptive versus maladaptive depending on the context (as proposed by differential susceptibility and our integrated mediation-moderation model), or are adaptive in terms of one outcome but maladaptive in terms of another outcome. Regarding the latter notion, one example of this, which was discussed above, was a finding by Rakesh et al. [27] of increases in SN resting-state functional connectivity to relate to higher depressive symptoms but lower problematic substance use.
Further, regarding adaptive brain changes, there are open questions about whether these brain changes are inherent or driven by supportive environments. Alternatively, they may reflect susceptibility markers, such that later negative and positive experiences propel individuals to negative and positive outcomes, respectively. To comprehensively test differential susceptibility effects, studies are needed that measure positive and negative environments, in addition to positive and negative outcomes. It cannot necessarily be assumed that low levels of a negative environment equate to high levels of a positive environment, for example.
Type of environment and timing of environmental and brain measures
The vast majority of available studies have investigated the early and mid-adolescent periods, with few studies investigating early and mid-childhood. The early-mid-adolescent period has been suggested as particularly important for the expression of differential susceptibility [97], given that it is associated with increased sensitivity to social stimuli [98] and increased prevalence of psychopathology [5]. Schriber and Guyer [97] suggest that measures of neurobiology may be more likely to reflect susceptibility (sensitivity to both positive and negative environments) than vulnerability (sensitivity only to negative environments) during adolescence relative to other periods (given that adolescence is a period of heightened sensitivity to both negative and positive social contexts); future research is needed to test this possibility.
More studies of early-mid childhood, however, are needed to test mediation-based theories, particularly those that focus on the early rearing environment or “early” adversity more generally (including our proposed model). Investigating neurodevelopment immediately or soon after the onset of early adverse experiences will shed light on the initial neurobiological response to adversity. Further, the change of pace [26], Rakesh et al.’s model [19], and our proposed integrated mediation-moderation model make predictions about patterns of development across childhood and adolescence. Longitudinal studies that capture many years of development are needed to comprehensively test these models.
Relatedly, the majority of studies have not measured the timing of exposure to the social environmental factor of interest. The timing of onset, in addition to the chronicity and severity of exposure, are likely to be important in shaping neurodevelopment or interacting with neurodevelopment to influence mental health [82, 99]. Clarifying timing effects will help to refine models, and more generally determine whether there are specific sensitive periods for different social environmental exposures, and whether these differ for different neural regions and circuits.
Regarding the type of social environment investigated, existing studies have been quite mixed and have investigated varying aspects of the family environment (e.g., childhood maltreatment, family connectedness, parenting behavior), the peer environment (e.g., peer victimization), and broader family, neighborhood, and community-level factors (e.g., family poverty, neighborhood disadvantage, community crime), in addition to general negative life events. Of note, few studies have investigated the peer environment, which may be particularly relevant for differential susceptibility effects during adolescence (as per above discussion), or other experiences of social stress, such as minority-related discrimination. There is currently minimal research using mediation frameworks to test differential threat versus deprivation experiences (and how they may interact with unpredictability) as per change of pace theory and the integrated model of dimensions of environmental experience. While differential susceptibility theory suggests that positive environmental exposures are not merely the inverse of negative environmental exposures [54], not all moderation studies tested positive environmental exposures and hence may have been unable to detect differential susceptibility effects. As such, more research is needed to test whether there is specificity regarding the types of environmental factors and contexts that may influence and interact with neurobiology to predict mental health.
Integration of human and animal research
Some of the discussed theories and models are, in part, based on evidence from animal research. Animal research, while limited in providing accounts of specific psychiatric disorders, is particularly important for elucidating how different types of environmental exposures causally influence the brain, and how this plays out over time [100]. For example, as outlined above, markers of both accelerated maturation (e.g., precocious arrival of Parvalbumin-positive cells), and growth (e.g., developmental changes in expression of markers of cell proliferation and differentiation) can be investigated over time [82]. Specific drivers of increased neural susceptibility can also be investigated. For example, mouse knockout models have begun to test the function of the SERT polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) s-allele (which has been linked to differential susceptibility) in inhibitory control over excitatory neurons in the cortex and expression of various GABAergic markers [101].
Animal research will thus be beneficial for testing aspects of the discussed models. Comprehensive animal studies investigating whether and how adverse and positive early environments influence neurodevelopmental markers of increased differential susceptibility would be particularly informative. Some relevant work has been done; a recent study, for example, manipulated prenatal stress and rearing-environment quality in prairie voles, and found that prenatal stress was associated with heightened behavioral and physiological reactivity and also with increased susceptibility to both positive and negative rearing experiences [102].
Conclusion
To conclude, while the role of the social environment in impacting mental health and the development of mental illness in young people has been acknowledged for several decades, knowledge of how neurobiological and neurodevelopmental factors play a role in this lags behind. There have been many theoretical advancements more recently, however, that provide testable hypotheses to move the field forward. A growing research body has started to shed light on the validity of these models. We propose that further research is particularly needed that tests integrated models to provide a comprehensive account of the role of the social environment and neurodevelopment in developmental psychiatry.
References
Rothenberg WA, Bizzego A, Esposito G, Lansford JE, Al-Hassan SM, Bacchini D, et al. Predicting adolescent mental health outcomes across cultures: a machine learning approach. J Youth Adolesc. 2023;52:1–25.
Solmi M, Radua J, Olivola M, Croce E, Soardo L, Salazar de Pablo G, et al. Age at onset of mental disorders worldwide: large-scale meta-analysis of 192 epidemiological studies. Mol Psychiatry. 2022;27:281–95.
Rakesh D, Allen NB, Whittle S. Balancing act: neural correlates of affect dysregulation in youth depression and substance use—a systematic review of functional neuroimaging studies. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2020;42:100775.
Steinberg L. A dual systems model of adolescent risk‐taking. Dev Psychobiol. 2010;52:216–24.
Twenge JM. Increases in depression, self‐harm, and suicide among U.S. adolescents after 2012 and links to technology use: possible mechanisms. Psychiatr Res Clin Pr. 2020;2:19–25.
Johnston MV, Ishida A, Ishida WN, Matsushita HB, Nishimura A, Tsuji M. Plasticity and injury in the developing brain. Brain Dev. 2009;31:1–10.
Ismail FY, Fatemi A, Johnston MV. Cerebral plasticity: windows of opportunity in the developing brain. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2017;21:23–48.
Fandakova Y, Hartley CA. Mechanisms of learning and plasticity in childhood and adolescence. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2020;42:100764.
McLaughlin KA, Gabard-Durnam L. Experience-driven plasticity and the emergence of psychopathology: a mechanistic framework integrating development and the environment into the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) model. J Psychopathol Clin Sci. 2022;131:575.
McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Nelson CA. Neglect as a violation of species-expectant experience: neurodevelopmental consequences. Biol Psychiatry. 2017;82:462–71.
Whittle S, Simmons JG, Dennison M, Vijayakumar N, Schwartz O, Yap MB, et al. Positive parenting predicts the development of adolescent brain structure: a longitudinal study. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2014;8:7–17.
Stepanous J, Munford L, Qualter P, Nees F, Elliott R. Longitudinal associations between peer and family relationships, emotional symptoms, and regional brain volume across adolescence. J Youth Adolesc. 2023;52:734–53.
Becht AI, Wierenga LM, Mills KL, Meuwese R, van Duijvenvoorde A, Blakemore S-J, et al. Beyond the average brain: individual differences in social brain development are associated with friendship quality. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2021;16:292–301.
Piccolo LR, Merz EC, Noble KG. Pediatric Imaging N and Genetics Study. School climate is associated with cortical thickness and executive function in children and adolescents. Dev Sci. 2019;22:e12719.
Rakesh D, Zalesky A, Whittle S. The role of school environment in brain structure, connectivity, and mental health in children: a multimodal investigation. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2023;8:32–41.
Callaghan BL, Tottenham N. The stress acceleration hypothesis: effects of early-life adversity on emotion circuits and behavior. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2016;7:76–81.
Belsky J, Steinberg L, Draper P. Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: an evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Dev. 1991;62:647–70.
Tooley UA, Bassett DS, Mackey AP. Environmental influences on the pace of brain development. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2021;22:372–84.
Rakesh D, Whittle S, Sheridan MA, McLaughlin KA. Childhood socioeconomic status and the pace of structural neurodevelopment: accelerated, delayed, or simply different? Trends Cogn Sci. 2023;27:833–51.
McCrory EJ, Viding E. The theory of latent vulnerability: reconceptualizing the link between childhood maltreatment and psychiatric disorder. Dev Psychopathol. 2015;27:493–505.
Colich NL, Rosen ML, Williams ES, McLaughlin KA. Biological aging in childhood and adolescence following experiences of threat and deprivation: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2020;146:721.
McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Lambert HK. Childhood adversity and neural development: deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;47:578–91.
McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA. Beyond cumulative risk: a dimensional approach to childhood adversity. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2016;25:239–45.
Sheridan MA, McLaughlin KA. Dimensions of early experience and neural development: deprivation and threat. Trends Cogn Sci. 2014;18:580–5.
Ellis BJ, Sheridan MA, Belsky J, McLaughlin KA. Why and how does early adversity influence development? Toward an integrated model of dimensions of environmental experience. Dev Psychopathol. 2022;34:447–71.
Roubinov D, Meaney MJ, Boyce WT. Change of pace: how developmental tempo varies to accommodate failed provision of early needs. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;131:120–34.
Rakesh D, Allen NB, Whittle S. Longitudinal changes in within-salience network functional connectivity mediate the relationship between childhood abuse and neglect, and mental health during adolescence. Psychol Med. 2023;53:1552–64.
Marusak HA, Etkin A, Thomason ME. Disrupted insula-based neural circuit organization and conflict interference in trauma-exposed youth. NeuroImage Clin. 2015;8:516–25.
Forbes EE, Dahl RE. Research review: Altered reward function in adolescent depression: what, when and how? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53:3–15.
Brieant AE, Sisk LM, Gee DG. Associations among negative life events, changes in cortico-limbic connectivity, and psychopathology in the ABCD Study. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2021;52:101022.
Rakesh D, Kelly C, Vijayakumar N, Zalesky A, Allen NB, Whittle S. Unraveling the consequences of childhood maltreatment: deviations from typical functional neurodevelopment mediate the relationship between maltreatment history and depressive symptoms. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2021;6:329–42.
Quinn ME, Stange JP, Jenkins LM, Corwin S, DelDonno SR, Bessette KL, et al. Cognitive control and network disruption in remitted depression: a correlate of childhood adversity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2018;13:1081–90.
Hanson JL, Hariri AR, Williamson DE. Blunted ventral striatum development in adolescence reflects emotional neglect and predicts depressive symptoms. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;78:598–605.
Miller JG, Gluckman PD, Fortier MV, Chong YS, Meaney MJ, Tan AP, et al. Faster pace of hippocampal growth mediates the association between perinatal adversity and childhood depression. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2024;67:101392.
Barch DM, Shirtcliff EA, Elsayed NM, Whalen D, Gilbert K, Vogel AC, et al. Testosterone and hippocampal trajectories mediate relationship of poverty to emotion dysregulation and depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2020;117:22015–23.
Backhausen LL, Granzow J, Fröhner JH, Artiges E, Paillère‐Martinot M, Lemaître H, et al. Interplay of early negative life events, development of orbitofrontal cortical thickness and depression in young adulthood. JCPP Adv. 2023;4:e12210.
Barch DM, Donohue MR, Elsayed NM, Gilbert K, Harms MP, Hennefield L, et al. Early childhood socioeconomic status and cognitive and adaptive outcomes at the transition to adulthood: the mediating role of gray matter development across five scan waves. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2022;7:34–44.
Pozzi E, Rakesh D, Gracia-Tabuenca Z, Bray KO, Richmond S, Seal ML, et al. Investigating associations between maternal behavior and the development of functional connectivity during the transition from late childhood to early adolescence. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2023;9:398–406.
Rakesh D, Cropley V, Zalesky A, Vijayakumar N, Allen NB, Whittle S. Neighborhood disadvantage and longitudinal brain-predicted-age trajectory during adolescence. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2021;51:101002.
Rakesh D, Elzeiny R, Vijayakumar N, Whittle S. A longitudinal study of childhood maltreatment, subcortical development, and subcortico-cortical structural maturational coupling from early to late adolescence. Psychol Med. 2023;53:7525–36.
Whittle S, Pozzi E, Rakesh D, Kim JM, Yap MBH, Schwartz OS, et al. Harsh and inconsistent parental discipline is associated with altered cortical development in children. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2022;7:989–97.
Mills KL, Goddings A-L, Herting MM, Meuwese R, Blakemore S-J, Crone EA, et al. Structural brain development between childhood and adulthood: convergence across four longitudinal samples. Neuroimage. 2016;141:273–81.
Keding TJ, Heyn SA, Russell JD, Zhu X, Cisler J, McLaughlin KA, et al. Differential patterns of delayed emotion circuit maturation in abused girls with and without internalizing psychopathology. Am J Psychiatry. 2021;178:1026–36.
Sameroff AJ. Development systems: contexts and evolution. In Mussen P, editor. Handbook of child psychology, vol. 1. New York: Wiley; 1983, pp. 237–94.
Monroe SM, Simons AD. Diathesis-stress theories in the context of life stress research: implications for the depressive disorders. Psychol Bull. 1991;110:406.
McEwen BS. Stress, adaptation, and disease: allostasis and allostatic load. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1998;840:33–44.
Burmeister M, McInnis MG, Zöllner S. Psychiatric genetics: progress amid controversy. Nat Rev Genet. 2008;9:527–40.
Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, et al. Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science. 2003;301:386–9.
Caspi A, Moffitt TE. Gene–environment interactions in psychiatry: joining forces with neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:583–90.
Belsky J. Differential susceptibility to rearing influences: an evolutionary hypothesis and some evidence. In: Ellis B, Bjorklund D, editors. Origins of the social mind: evolutionary psychology and child development. New York: Guilford Press; 2005. pp. 139–163.
Boyce WT, Ellis BJ. Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary–developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Dev Psychopathol. 2005;17:271–301.
Boyce WT, Chesney M, Alkon A, Tschann JM, Adams S, Chesterman B, et al. Psychobiologic reactivity to stress and childhood respiratory illnesses: results of two prospective studies. Psychosom Med. 1995;57:411–22.
Belsky J, Pluess M. Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychol Bull. 2009;135:885.
Ellis BJ, Boyce WT, Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van Ijzendoorn MH. Differential susceptibility to the environment: an evolutionary–neurodevelopmental theory. Dev Psychopathol. 2011;23:7–28.
Belsky J. The differential susceptibility hypothesis: sensitivity to the environment for better and for worse. JAMA Pediatr. 2016;170:321–2.
Nederhof E, Schmidt MV. Mismatch or cumulative stress: toward an integrated hypothesis of programming effects. Physiol Behav. 2012;106:691–700.
Daskalakis NP, Bagot RC, Parker KJ, Vinkers CH, de Kloet ER. The three-hit concept of vulnerability and resilience: toward understanding adaptation to early-life adversity outcome. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38:1858–73.
Liu S, Oshri A, Kogan SM, Wickrama K, Sweet L. Amygdalar activation as a neurobiological marker of differential sensitivity in the effects of family rearing experiences on socioemotional adjustment in youths. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2021;6:1052–62.
Sequeira SL, Butterfield RD, Silk JS, Forbes EE, Ladouceur CD. Neural activation to parental praise interacts with social context to predict adolescent depressive symptoms. Front Behav Neurosci. 2019;13:222.
Turpyn CC, Jorgensen NA, Prinstein MJ, Lindquist KA, Telzer EH. Social neural sensitivity as a susceptibility marker to family context in predicting adolescent externalizing behavior. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2021;51:100993.
Yang B, Anderson Z, Zhou Z, Liu S, Haase CM, Qu Y. The longitudinal role of family conflict and neural reward sensitivity in youth’s internalizing symptoms. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2023;18:nsad037.
Huffman LG, Oshri A. Neural signatures of the development of antisocial behaviours and callous‐unemotional traits among youth: the moderating role of parental support. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2022;82:205–21.
Schriber RA, Rogers CR, Ferrer E, Conger RD, Robins RW, Hastings PD, et al. Do hostile school environments promote social deviance by shaping neural responses to social exclusion? J Res Adolesc. 2018;28:103–20.
Weissman DG, Gelardi KL, Conger RD, Robins RW, Hastings PD, Guyer AE. Adolescent externalizing problems: contributions of community crime exposure and neural function during emotion introspection in Mexican‐origin youth. J Res Adolesc. 2018;28:551–63.
Conley MI, Rapuano KM, Benson-Williams C, Rosenberg MD, Watts R, Bell C, et al. Executive network activation moderates the association between neighborhood threats and externalizing behavior in youth. Res Child Adolesc Psychopathol. 2023;51:789–803.
Hanson JL, Albert WD, Skinner AT, Shen SH, Dodge KA, Lansford JE. Resting state coupling between the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex is related to household income in childhood and indexes future psychological vulnerability to stress. Dev Psychopathol. 2019;31:1053–66.
Chahal R, Miller JG, Yuan JP, Buthmann JL, Gotlib IH. An exploration of dimensions of early adversity and the development of functional brain network connectivity during adolescence: implications for trajectories of internalizing symptoms. Dev Psychopathol. 2022;34:557–71.
Ding Q, Xu J, Peng S, Chen J, Luo Y, Li X, et al. Brain network integration underpins differential susceptibility of adolescent anxiety. Psychol Med. 2024;54:193–202.
Deane C, Vijayakumar N, Allen NB, Schwartz O, Simmons JG, Bousman CA, et al. Parenting × brain development interactions as predictors of adolescent depressive symptoms and well-being: differential susceptibility or diathesis-stress? Dev Psychopathol. 2020;32:139–50.
Herzberg MP, DeJoseph ML, Luby J, Barch DM. Threat experiences moderate the link between hippocampus volume and depression symptoms prospectively in adolescence. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2024;66:101359.
Nolvi S, Rasmussen JM, Graham AM, Gilmore JH, Styner M, Fair DA, et al. Neonatal brain volume as a marker of differential susceptibility to parenting quality and its association with neurodevelopment across early childhood. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2020;45:100826.
Norbom LB, Rokicki J, Eilertsen EM, Wiker T, Hanson J, Dahl A, et al. Parental education and income are linked to offspring cortical brain structure and psychopathology at 9–11 years. JCPP Adv. 2024;4:e12220.
Rifkin-Graboi A, Kong L, Sim L, Sanmugam S, Broekman B, Chen H, et al. Maternal sensitivity, infant limbic structure volume and functional connectivity: a preliminary study. Transl Psychiatry. 2015;5:e668.
Schriber RA, Anbari Z, Robins RW, Conger RD, Hastings PD, Guyer AE. Hippocampal volume as an amplifier of the effect of social context on adolescent depression. Clin Psychol Sci. 2017;5:632–49.
Whittle S, Yap MB, Sheeber L, Dudgeon P, Yücel M, Pantelis C, et al. Hippocampal volume and sensitivity to maternal aggressive behavior: a prospective study of adolescent depressive symptoms. Dev Psychopathol. 2011;23:115–29.
Yap MB, Whittle S, Yücel M, Sheeber L, Pantelis C, Simmons JG, et al. Interaction of parenting experiences and brain structure in the prediction of depressive symptoms in adolescents. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65:1377–85.
Hanson JL, Knodt AR, Brigidi BD, Hariri AR. Lower structural integrity of the uncinate fasciculus is associated with a history of child maltreatment and future psychological vulnerability to stress. Dev Psychopathol. 2015;27:1611–9.
Ergorul C, Eichenbaum H. The hippocampus and memory for “what,” “where,” and “when”. Learn Mem. 2004;11:397–405.
Ashtari M, Avants B, Cyckowski L, Cervellione KL, Roofeh D, Cook P, et al. Medial temporal structures and memory functions in adolescents with heavy cannabis use. J Psychiatr Res. 2011;45:1055–66.
Chan SY, Ngoh ZM, Ong ZY, Teh AL, Kee MZ, Zhou JH, et al. The influence of early-life adversity on the coupling of structural and functional brain connectivity across childhood. Nat Ment Health. 2024;2:52–62.
Rosero MA, Winkelmann T, Pohlack S, Cavalli J, Nees F, Flor H. Memory-guided attention: bilateral hippocampal volume positively predicts implicit contextual learning. Brain Struct Funct. 2019;224:1999–2008.
Bath K, Manzano-Nieves G, Goodwill H. Early life stress accelerates behavioral and neural maturation of the hippocampus in male mice. Horm Behav. 2016;82:64–71.
Serra FT, Araujo BHS, Placencia EVD, Henrique JS, Dona F, Torres LB, et al. Enriched environment and exercise effects on parvalbumin expression and distribution in the hippocampal formation of developing rats. Brain Res Bull. 2020;160:85–90.
Whittle S, Lichter R, Dennison M, Vijayakumar N, Schwartz O, Byrne ML, et al. Structural brain development and depression onset during adolescence: a prospective longitudinal study. Am J Psychiatry. 2014;171:564–71.
Garavan H, Pendergrass JC, Ross TJ, Stein EA, Risinger RC. Amygdala response to both positively and negatively valenced stimuli. Neuroreport. 2001;12:2779–83.
Herman JP, Ostrander MM, Mueller NK, Figueiredo H. Limbic system mechanisms of stress regulation: hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2005;29:1201–13.
Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct. 2010;214:655–67.
Manzano Nieves G, Bravo M, Baskoylu S, Bath KG. Early life adversity decreases pre-adolescent fear expression by accelerating amygdala PV cell development. Elife. 2020;9:e55263.
VanTieghem M, Korom M, Flannery J, Choy T, Caldera C, Humphreys KL, et al. Longitudinal changes in amygdala, hippocampus and cortisol development following early caregiving adversity. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2021;48:100916.
Homberg JR, Jagiellowicz J. A neural model of vulnerability and resilience to stress-related disorders linked to differential susceptibility. Mol Psychiatry. 2022;27:514–24.
Bearden CE, Karlsgodt KH. Risk and resilience in extraordinary times. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2021;6:136.
Leal ASM, Silvers JA. Neurobiological markers of resilience to early-life adversity during adolescence. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2021;6:238–47.
Roeckner AR, Oliver KI, Lebois LA, van Rooij SJ, Stevens JS. Neural contributors to trauma resilience: a review of longitudinal neuroimaging studies. Transl Psychiatry. 2021;11:508.
Zhang L, Rakesh D, Cropley V, Whittle S. Neurobiological correlates of resilience during childhood and adolescence—a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2023;105:102333.
Schwarz S. Resilience in psychology: a critical analysis of the concept. Theory Psychol. 2018;28:528–41.
Masten AS, Lucke CM, Nelson KM, Stallworthy IC. Resilience in development and psychopathology: multisystem perspectives. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2021;17:521–49.
Schriber RA, Guyer AE. Adolescent neurobiological susceptibility to social context. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2016;19:1–18.
Foulkes L, Blakemore S-J. Is there heightened sensitivity to social reward in adolescence? Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2016;40:81–85.
Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:434–45.
Waters RC, Gould E. Early life adversity and neuropsychiatric disease: differential outcomes and translational relevance of rodent models. Front Syst Neurosci. 2022;16:860847.
Greven CU, Lionetti F, Booth C, Aron EN, Fox E, Schendan HE, et al. Sensory processing sensitivity in the context of environmental sensitivity: a critical review and development of research agenda. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;98:287–305.
Hartman S, Freeman SM, Bales KL, Belsky J. Prenatal stress as a risk—and an opportunity—factor. Psychol Sci. 2018;29:572–80.
Hayes AF. Beyond Baron and Kenny: statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Commun Monogr. 2009;76:408–20.
MacKinnon DP, Krull JL, Lockwood CM. Equivalence of the mediation, confounding and suppression effect. Prev Sci. 2000;1:173–81.
Funding
The authors were funded by The University of Melbourne (SW and LZ) and King’s College London (DR). These organizations had no role in the design of the study, collection, and analysis of data and decision to publish. Open Access funding enabled and organized by CAUL and its Member Institutions.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
All authors made a substantial contribution to the conception of the work, and the interpretation of data for the work. All authors contributed to drafting the work and revising it critically for important intellectual content. All authors provided final approval of the version to be published, and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Whittle, S., Zhang, L. & Rakesh, D. Environmental and neurodevelopmental contributors to youth mental illness. Neuropsychopharmacol. (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-024-01926-y
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-024-01926-y
This article is cited by
-
Concurrent and longitudinal neurostructural correlates of irritability in children
Neuropsychopharmacology (2024)