Abstract
Cessation of therapy with a selective serotonin (5-HT) reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) is often associated with an early onset and disabling discontinuation syndrome, the mechanism of which is surprisingly little investigated. Here we determined the effect on 5-HT neurochemistry of discontinuation from the SSRI paroxetine. Paroxetine was administered repeatedly to mice (once daily, 12 days versus saline controls) and then either continued or discontinued for up to 5 days. Whereas brain tissue levels of 5-HT and/or its metabolite 5-HIAA tended to decrease during continuous paroxetine, levels increased above controls after discontinuation, notably in hippocampus. In microdialysis experiments continuous paroxetine elevated hippocampal extracellular 5-HT and this effect fell to saline control levels on discontinuation. However, depolarisation (high potassium)-evoked 5-HT release was reduced by continuous paroxetine but increased above controls post-discontinuation. Extracellular hippocampal 5-HIAA also decreased during continuous paroxetine and increased above controls post-discontinuation. Next, immunohistochemistry experiments found that paroxetine discontinuation increased c-Fos expression in midbrain 5-HT (TPH2 positive) neurons, adding further evidence for a hyperexcitable 5-HT system. The latter effect was recapitulated by 5-HT1A receptor antagonist administration although gene expression analysis could not confirm altered expression of 5-HT1A autoreceptors following paroxetine discontinuation. Finally, in behavioural experiments paroxetine discontinuation increased anxiety-like behaviour, which partially correlated in time with the measures of increased 5-HT function. In summary, this study reports evidence that, across a range of experiments, SSRI discontinuation triggers a rebound activation of 5-HT neurons. This effect is reminiscent of neural changes associated with various psychotropic drug withdrawal states, suggesting a common unifying mechanism.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Selective serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT) reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) are widely used in the pharmacological treatment of major depressive disorder (MDD) and anxiety-related disorders, as well as a range of other common and disabling neuropsychiatric conditions. Despite their high therapeutic value, abrupt cessation of treatment with an SSRI can produce a debilitating set of psychological and somatic symptoms including heightened anxiety, sleep disruption and sensory disturbances [1, 2]. These symptoms are commonly referred to as a ‘discontinuation’ rather than ‘withdrawal’ syndrome (in part due to SSRIs not being associated with compulsive use, tolerance and craving) and appear within days of treatment cessation. In a recent observational study half of MDD patients experienced the discontinuation syndrome and many reported that their symptoms were severe [3], suggesting a greater clinical problem than previously recognised. This adverse effect of treatment cessation is common to different SSRIs as well as other antidepressants, but the risk of discontinuation syndrome differs between drugs [4,5,6]. In particular, paroxetine was estimated to be 10 to 100 times more likely to induce a discontinuation syndrome than other SSRIs [5]. Notably, the mechanisms underpinning SSRI discontinuation are currently unknown.
Emerging evidence suggests that SSRI discontinuation occurs across species, which offers opportunities for mechanistic studies. Specifically, rats administered chronic citalopram demonstrated increased startle responsivity within 2 days of discontinuation [7]. In addition, we recently found that mice discontinued from repeated treatment with paroxetine exhibited altered behaviour on the elevated plus maze (EPM) test of anxiety 2 days post-discontinuation [8]. There is strong evidence linking certain discontinuation symptoms such as anxiety with changes in brain 5-HT [9]. Moreover, since the primary target of SSRIs is the 5-HT transporter, and since 5-HT neurons are well known to adapt to continuous SSRI exposure [10], further changes in 5-HT function during SSRI discontinuation seem likely, as speculated in earlier reviews [11,12,13]. Surprisingly, however, the effect of SSRI discontinuation on the 5-HT system has been little investigated to date.
In one of the few such studies (that was not in itself directed at discontinuation mechanisms) carried out 30 years ago, continuous administration of fluoxetine to rats was reported to decrease the 5-HT metabolite 5-HIAA in hippocampus and other brain regions [14]. Interestingly, a striking increase in 5-HIAA was evident over 2 weeks following treatment cessation. Similar findings were later observed in mice administered citalopram [15]. It is now well known that continuous SSRI treatment causes an adaptive fall in 5-HT metabolism and synthesis, an effect likely mediated through indirect activation of 5-HT autoreceptors [16,17,18]. Although changes in 5-HT metabolism/synthesis do not always model 5-HT release [19, 20], these early findings with fluoxetine and citalopram are suggestive of a rebound increase in 5-HT function over days following discontinuation. Notably, evidence of rebound increases in neural function have been observed after withdrawal from various psychotropic drugs including morphine, benzodiazepines and alcohol [21,22,23,24], suggesting potential parallels between the mechanisms of cessation of treatment with these different psychotropic drugs.
Against this background the current study investigated the effect of paroxetine discontinuation on 5-HT function in mice using a combination of neurochemical, immunohistochemical and behavioural approaches.
Methods and Materials
Animals
C57BL/6 J male mice (7 weeks; Charles River) were habituated to the holding facility for one week prior to use. Mice were group housed (3–6 per cage, 21 oC, 12 h light-dark cycle) in cages lined with sawdust bedding and cage enrichment, with ad libitum access to food and water. Experiments followed the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act of 1986 and ARRIVE guidelines. We did not use a mixed sex design because in our recent study female mice did not demonstrate behavioural evidence of SSRI discontinuation [8]. This possibly reflects recent clinical findings that compared to women, men were more likely to experience discontinuation symptoms, and symptoms are more likely to be severe [5].
Drug treatment
Mice were allocated to treatment groups by stratified randomisation. Mice received once-daily injections of either 10 mg/kg s.c. paroxetine (1 mg/ml; Abcam) or saline vehicle for 12 days, then treatment was either continued or swapped to saline (discontinuation groups) for a further 2 or 5 days. This dose regimen was based on our recent study showing that mice discontinued from repeated treatment with paroxetine exhibited anxiety-like behaviour; twice daily paroxetine (10 mg/kg s.c.) had similar effects to once daily, and effects were detected after discontinuation from treatment for 12 and 28 days but not 7 days [8]. In some experiments (ex vivo neurochemistry, immunohistochemistry), 90 min prior to brain removal mice were tested on the elevated plus maze (EPM) to provide behavioural measures to correlate with the neurochemical effects of SSRI discontinuation.
Ex vivo 5-HT neurochemistry
After cervical dislocation brains were rapidly removed and frozen in isopentane on dry ice, prior to storage at −80 oC. Subsequently, hippocampus, striatum, frontal cortex and midbrain were dissected on ice, weighed, and then placed in 0.09 M perchloric acid prior to sonication and centrifugation (13,000 rpm for 15 min, 4 oC). Supernatant 5-HT and 5-HIAA, as well as dopamine and its metabolite DOPAC, were measured using high performance liquid chromatography (HPLC) with electrochemical detection [25].
In vivo microdialysis
Mice were anaesthetised throughout the experiment with urethane (initial dose 1 g/kg i.p., supplemented as required) and maintained at 36 ± 1 °C using a homeothermic blanket. A microdialysis probe (2 mm, Microbiotech MAB4) was stereotaxically implanted into the ventral hippocampus (AP −3.0 mm, ML ± 2.9 mm, DV −4.0 mm relative to bregma and the dura surface; [26]) and perfused (2 µl/min) with artificial cerebrospinal fluid (in mM: 140 NaCl, 4 KCl, 1.2 Na2HPO4, 0.27 NaH2PO4, 1 MgCl2, 2.4 CaCl2 and 7.2 glucose). After a 60 min post-implantation period, samples were collected every 20 min for 120 min. The perfusion medium was then switched (20 min) to one containing 56 mM KCl, followed 60 min later by 100 mM KCl. Microdialysate samples were analysed for 5-HT and 5-HIAA using HPLC with electrochemical detection (see above). To obtain an overall measure of sensitivity to high KCl, the response to 56 mM KCl was divided by that to 100 mM KCl to provide the 56:100 mM KCl response ratio for each mouse, and then a group mean ± SEM value was obtained.
c-Fos/TPH2 immunohistochemistry
Ninety min after EPM exposure (see below) mice were injected with pentobarbital (200 mg/kg i.p.) and transcardially perfused with phosphate buffered saline (PBS) followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS. Brains were kept at 4 oC in 4% PFA for 48 h, then stored in cryoprotective 30% sucrose in PBS at 4 oC. Cryostat cut sections (30 μm, coronal) containing the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN) were sequentially washed in PBS, ammonium chloride and PBS with 0.3% TWEEN® 80 (PBS-T). Sections were then incubated in PBS-T with 10% donkey serum for 1 h at room temperature, and then incubated overnight at 4 oC with the primary antibodies for c-Fos (Abcam, ab214672), TPH2 (Abcam, Ab121013) and NeuN (Abcam, Ab104224) in PBS-T with 2% donkey serum. Sections were then washed in PBS-T and incubated for 2 h (room temperature) with donkey anti-rabbit IgG (Alexa Flor™ 488; Invitrogen, A21206), donkey anti-goat IgG (Alexa Flor™ 568; Invitrogen, A11057) and donkey anti-mouse IgG (Alexa Flor™ 647; Invitrogen, A21202) in PBS-T with 2% donkey serum. After final washes in PBS-T then PBS, sections were mounted on glass slides and imaged (Olympus Epifluorescence Microscope BX40 with ImageJ Micromanager v1.4). c-Fos/TPH2 double-labelled DRN neurons were counted in three sections per mouse, averaged, and expressed as the number of neurons per mm2. All counting was conducted blind to treatment.
qPCR analysis
For PCR analysis, the midbrain raphe region and frontal cortex were rapidly dissected from frozen tissue sections (1 mm). RNA extraction, cDNA conversion and qPCR were conducted as described [27]. In brief, RNA was extracted (Qiagen RNeasy Mini Kit) and eluted into RNase-free water (20 μl for midbrain, 25 μl for cortex) prior to conversion to cDNA using a high-capacity cDNA Reverse Transcription Kit (Life Technologies) and T100 Thermocycler (Bio-Rad). QPCR was performed (200 ng RNA) using a LightCycler® 480 instrument (Roche Diagnostics) with the following primers (5’ to 3’ at 300 nM): 5-HT1A forward GACAGGCGGCAACGATACT, reverse CCAAGGAGCCGATGAGATAGTT [28]; 5-HT1B forward CCCATCAGCACCATGTACAC, reverse GACTTGGTTCACGTACACAG [29]; 5-HT2A forward CAGGCAAGTCACAGGA TAGC, reverse TTAAGCAGAAAGAAAATCCCACAG [30]; 5-HT4 forward CCTCACAGCAACTTCTCCTT, reverse TCCCCTGACTTCCTCAAATA [31]. β-actin was the housekeeping gene; forward CATTGCTGACAGGATGCAGAAGG, reverse TGCTGGAAGGTGGACAGTGAGG [32]. Reactions (384 well-plates, 10 μl reaction volume, 5 μl Precision®PLUS qPCR Master Mix with SYBRgreen, 25 ng cDNA) used the following cycle: enzyme activation for 2 min at 95 oC, 40 cycles of 10 s at 95 oC, 1 min at 60 oC, then held at 4 oC. Data were analysed using ΔCt values with outliers identified by ROUT analysis of 2-ΔΔCt values.
Elevated plus maze
EPM experiments were performed as previously described [8]. In brief, experiments were conducted in the light phase (10:00–15:00 h) by an observer blind to treatment. Mice were run on the EPM on one occasion only. The EPM (50 cm above the floor) comprised 2 open arms (35 × 6 cm) perpendicular to 2 closed arms (35 × 6 cm, 20 cm walls) with a central region (6 × 6 cm), placed in a dimly lit room. Mice were placed facing the walls of the closed arm (counterbalanced between the 2 closed arms) and movement was automatically tracked for 300 s (ANY-maze software, Stoelting Co.). Head dips were counted manually.
Statistical analysis
Data were initially assessed for normality with D’Agostino Pearson’s test. Multi-time point microdialysis data were analysed using repeated measures ANOVA with Bonferroni’s multiple comparisons test. Other parametric data were analysed by one-way ANOVA with post-hoc Fisher’s Least Significant Difference (LSD) since we had predictions of outcome and analysis was pre-planned. Although the latter test does not correct for multiple comparisons, the main outcome comparisons remain statistically significant when analysed using the post-hoc Tukey test. The Kruskal Wallis test with Uncorrected Dunn’s test was used for non-parametric data. ROUT analysis was used to identify outliers in all datasets. Data were analysed with GraphPad Prism (v9) by an experimenter blind to treatment group. P < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Results
Paroxetine discontinuation increased 5-HT metabolism in hippocampus ex vivo
Initial experiments investigated whether discontinuation from paroxetine caused a rebound increase in 5-HT metabolism in hippocampus and other brain regions as previously reported for fluoxetine [14]. For hippocampus, continued paroxetine tended to reduce levels of 5-HIAA and 5-HT compared to saline controls, but this effect recovered on discontinuation day 2 and increased above control levels on discontinuation day 5 (5-HIAA, F(3,47) = 4.626, p = 0.0065; 5-HT, F(3,47) = 5.632, p = 0.0022; for post hoc analysis see Fig. 1A). Other regions showed similar trends to hippocampus although statistically the most consistent finding was 5-HT and 5-HIAA levels were higher on discontinuation day 5 compared to continued paroxetine; striatum (5-HIAA, F(3,47) = 3.529, p = 0.0218; 5-HT, F(3,47) = 2.820, p = 0.0490; Fig. 1B), frontal cortex (5-HIAA, F(3,47) = 4.435, p = 0.0082; 5-HT, F(3,47) = 2.722, p = 0.0551; Fig. 1C) and midbrain (5-HIAA, F(3,47) = 5.305, p = 0.0031; 5-HT, F(3,47) = 4.609, p = 0.0066; Fig. 1D).
Compared to saline controls, neither continuous treatment with paroxetine nor 2 or 5 days of discontinuation altered tissue levels of DOPAC or dopamine in any brain region (Tables S1 and S2).
Paroxetine discontinuation caused fall in basal extracellular 5-HT but increased 5-HT metabolism in hippocampus in vivo
Next, we investigated the effect of paroxetine discontinuation on basal extracellular 5-HT and 5-HIAA in hippocampus using in vivo microdialysis (Fig. 2A). Mice receiving continuous paroxetine had 2-3 times higher levels of extracellular 5-HT than saline controls (Fig. 2B). This effect rapidly reversed on discontinuation from paroxetine, and basal extracellular 5-HT fell to saline controls at discontinuation days 2 and 5 (F(3,28) = 9.229, p = 0.0002; for post hoc analysis see Fig. 2B).
In comparison, basal extracellular levels of 5-HIAA were 40–50% lower in mice receiving continuous paroxetine compared to saline controls. This effect reversed on day 2 after discontinuation and on day 5 levels of 5-HIAA were ~30% above saline controls (F(3,28) = 10.165, p < 0.0001; for post hoc analysis see Fig. 2C).
Thus, continuous paroxetine increased basal extracellular 5-HT in hippocampus and this effect fell rapidly to saline control levels following discontinuation. However, on discontinuation hippocampal extracellular 5-HIAA showed a rebound increase as detected with ex vivo measurements.
Paroxetine discontinuation increased potassium-evoked 5-HT release in hippocampus in vivo
Basal extracellular levels of 5-HT reflect a combination of synaptic processes such as 5-HT reuptake, diffusion, synthesis, metabolism, and release. Therefore, we measured depolarisation-evoked 5-HT release by local perfusion with high KCl, which is commonly a used tool in microdialysis studies to mimic physiological exocytosis. In saline controls 56 mM KCl caused a short-lasting increase in 5-HT of ~30%. This response was attenuated in mice receiving continuous paroxetine, which reversed on discontinuation day 2, and was greater than saline controls on discontinuation day 5 (F(3,27) = 6.198, p = 0.0024; for post hoc analysis see Fig. 2D).
In comparison, 100 mM KCl evoked an increase in 5-HT of ~250% in saline controls, and this effect was not different across the treatment groups (F(3,25) = 0.4583, p = 0.7139; Fig. 2D). Using the 56:100 mM KCl response ratio as an overall measure of sensitivity to high KCl, a rebound increase in depolarisation-evoked 5-HT was evident on both discontinuation days 2 and 5 (F(3,24) = 6.642, p = 0.002; for post hoc analysis see Fig. 2D). Collectively, these findings suggest that 5-HT terminals in the hippocampus were more sensitive to depolarisation on days 2 and 5 following paroxetine discontinuation.
Paroxetine discontinuation increased c-Fos expression in DRN 5-HT neurons
The above findings suggest that 5-HT neurons are more excitable during paroxetine discontinuation. To investigate this further, co-localisation of c-Fos and TPH2 was used as a marker of 5-HT neuron activity. Multiple c-Fos/TPH2 co-labelled neurons were observed in the DRN (Fig. 3A). Interestingly, on both discontinuation day 2 and 5 the number of c-Fos/TPH2 co-labelled neurons was increased compared to continuous paroxetine and saline controls (one-way ANOVA: day 2, F(2,16) = 5.637, p = 0.0140; day 5, F(2,15) = 5.523, p = 0.0159; for post hoc analysis see Fig. 3B). Compared to saline controls the number of c-Fos/TPH2 co-labelled DRN neurons had a tendency to be reduced by continuous paroxetine although this effect was not statistically significant (Fig. 3B).
This finding of increased c-Fos/TPH2 co-labelled neurons in the DRN of mice discontinued from paroxetine further supports the idea that 5-HT neurons were more excitable.
Possible role of 5-HT feedback in effects of paroxetine discontinuation
A possible mechanism underlying hyperexcitable 5-HT neurons is the state of 5-HT1A autoreceptor desensitization which may have been exposed on cessation of paroxetine treatment. PCR analysis investigated whether paroxetine discontinuation was associated with adaptive changes in 5-HT1A autoreceptor expression, as well as the expression of other 5-HT receptor subtypes in the midbrain raphe region and prefrontal cortex linked to the feedback control of DRN 5-HT neurons (e.g. [33]).
On discontinuation day 2 mice had reduced 5-HT1A mRNA expression in both the midbrain raphe region and frontal cortex compared to continued paroxetine treatment (see Table S3 for statistical details) but expression of the 5-HT1B receptor in the midbrain raphe region or 5-HT2A and 5-HT4 receptors in frontal cortex were unchanged (Table S3). On discontinuation day 5, however, there was no effect of treatment on the expression of any 5-HT receptor subtype in either the midbrain raphe region or frontal cortex (Table S3).
To further test whether changes in 5-HT feedback may be involved in discontinuation responses, we tested the effect on c-Fos expression of the selective 5-HT1A receptor antagonist WAY-100635 at a dose (1 mg/kg s.c.) previously shown to block 5-HT1A autoreceptors in mice [34]. WAY-100635 increased the number of c-Fos/TPH2 co-labelled neurons compared to saline-controls (167.3 ± 6.0 versus 95.7 ± 13.6 neurons/mm2; t(10) = 3.118, p = 0.0109). This result adds to the plausibility that loss of 5-HT1A receptor feedback control contributed to increased excitability of 5-HT neurons on paroxetine discontinuation on day 2 and potentially day 5.
Paroxetine discontinuation increased anxiety-like behaviour on the EPM
Finally, given the well-established link between increased 5-HT and anxiety-like behaviour [9, 35], experiments tested the temporal relationship between the discontinuation-evoked increase in 5-HT function and changes in anxiety-like behaviour as assessed by performance on the EPM.
On discontinuation day 2 mice spent less time in the open arms (F(2,33) = 9.902, p = 0.0004), made fewer open arm entries (F(2,33) = 7.708, p = 0.0018) and showed fewer head dips (F(2,33) = 11.29, p = 0.0002) compared to mice receiving either continued paroxetine or saline (for post hoc analysis see Fig. 4B). These mice also had reduced distance travelled on the EPM (F(2,33) = 7.415, p = 0.0022; Fig. 4B), supporting the notion of increased behavioural inhibition in the anxiogenic environment, and consistent with the mice having a high anxiety phenotype. We have observed this reduction in locomotion previously and controlled for a non-specific motor deficit using a separate locomotor test [8]. In contrast, on discontinuation day 5 the performance of mice on the EPM was not different from continued paroxetine or saline controls (open arm time: F(2,33) = 1.451, p = 0.2490; open arm entries: F(2,33) = 1.100, p = 0.3449; head dips: F(2,33) = 2.422, p = 0.1043; distance: F(2,33) = 2.588, p = 0.0908; Fig. 4C). The behavioural data were inspected for correlations with effects of SSRI discontinuation on ex vivo 5-HT metabolism in hippocampus and c-Fos expression in the DRN but no significant correlations were observed.
Overall, these data indicate evidence in some measures of a rebound increase in 5-HT function observed on discontinuation day 2 was associated with increased anxiety-like behaviour. However, this temporal correlation was not apparent on discontinuation day 5 in that evidence of a rebound increase in 5-HT was not accompanied by changes in anxiety-like behaviour at this time point.
Discussion
Abrupt cessation of a course of treatment with an SSRI is often associated with a disabling discontinuation syndrome (see Introduction), which is currently of unknown cause and little studied. Here we found that across experiments, abrupt cessation of treatment with paroxetine was associated with evidence of increased 5-HT neuronal function. Specifically, paroxetine discontinuation; i) increased tissue levels of 5-HIAA and 5-HT, especially in hippocampus ex vivo (without altering dopamine or its metabolite DOPAC), ii) increased extracellular 5-HIAA in hippocampus in vivo, iii) increased depolarisation-evoked release of hippocampal 5-HT in vivo, and iv) increased c-Fos immunoreactivity in 5-HT neurons (TPH2-immunolabelled) in the DRN ex vivo. These effects occurred during a 5-day discontinuation period, with some (evoked 5-HT release, increased c-Fos expression) being already detected after 2 days. Finally, several experiments observed a predicted decrease in 5-HT neuronal function in mice receiving continued paroxetine that preceded the rebound increase when the drug treatment was subsequently withdrawn. Although basal extracellular levels of 5-HT did not show evidence of a rebound increase, as noted above, this measurement likely reflects the net effect of changes in 5-HT reuptake, diffusion, synthesis, metabolism, and release. The main findings are summarised in Table 1.
The emergence of these discontinuation effects is consistent with the short half-life of paroxetine in rodents (t1/2 = 6.3 h, [36]) as well as paroxetine having non-linear metabolism kinetics and inhibiting its own metabolising enzyme (CYP2D6) such that when plasma paroxetine levels fall its metabolism increases and its washout is accelerated [37]. Moreover, we have previously shown in rats that plasma levels paroxetine were readily detectable in animals 12 h after 5 mg/kg s.c. paroxetine administered twice daily for 15 days but were negligible 24 h later [38]. The timing also fits with behavioural effects appearing within 2 days of paroxetine discontinuation as observed in mice, both here and in our recent study [8], as well as in previous studies in depressed patients [39, 40]. In addition, earlier studies reported increased 5-HT metabolism/synthesis in rodent hippocampus and other brain regions within days of discontinuation from fluoxetine or escitalopram but with a somewhat later onset with fluoxetine likely due to the drug’s longer half-life [14, 15]. Here, the rebound increase in 5-HT neuron function was not investigated beyond 5 days after paroxetine discontinuation but the latter studies on 5-HT metabolism/synthesis suggest that the rebound effect may last several weeks.
A plausible explanation for the rebound increase in 5-HT transmission following paroxetine cessation is removal of the constant suppression of 5-HT neurons produced during repeated exposure to the drug, mediated by negative 5-HT feedback mechanisms. That these feedback mechanisms were operational during continuous paroxetine was evident both as reduced 5-HT metabolism and reduced depolarisation-evoked release of 5-HT in hippocampus. Indeed, previous studies consistently report reduced brain 5-HT synthesis and metabolism in rodents chronically exposed to paroxetine and other SSRIs [17, 18]. The current finding of reduced depolarisation-evoked 5-HT release in animals treated continuously with paroxetine appears to be in contradiction with earlier studies reporting that depolarisation-evoked release of 3H-5-HT in guinea pig hippocampus and other brain regions was enhanced by prolonged administration of paroxetine [41, 42]. Interestingly, however, to allow reuptake of the 3H-5-HT the latter experiments measured the release of 3H-5-HT after a 48–96 h washout of treatment, when potentially the discontinuation effects observed here had set in.
The negative feedback effects of continuous SSRI administration likely result from the inhibition of 5-HT uptake causing a sustained elevation in extracellular 5-HT. Here, continuous paroxetine increased extracellular 5-HT 2-3 fold in hippocampus in accord with previous findings in this and other brain regions including the DRN, as evident in a recent meta-analysis of rat data [43]. Increased extracellular 5-HT will activate various 5-HT feedback mechanisms including somatodendritic 5-HT1A autoreceptors and terminal 5-HT1B autoreceptors, and potentially subpopulations of 5-HT receptors located on postsynaptic neurons that are involved in 5-HT neuron control [33]. Indeed, SSRI-induced decreases in 5-HT neuronal activity and synthesis/metabolism can be attenuated by 5-HT1A/1B receptor antagonists [18, 44]. Hence, a likely scenario is that on abrupt cessation of SSRI administration and reversal of 5-HT transporter inhibition, extracellular 5-HT rapidly falls (Fig. 2B) relieving inhibitory 5-HT feedback systems, which triggers a rebound increase in 5-HT synthesis, release, metabolism, and neuronal excitability.
The present finding of reduced 5-HT1A receptor mRNA in the midbrain raphe region and frontal cortex at 2 days after paroxetine discontinuation comprises evidence that loss of 5-HT feedback control contributes to the rebound increase in 5-HT transmission. This change was not evident at 5 days but 5-HT1A autoreceptor desensitization during continuous SSRI administration has been difficult to demonstrate at the gene/protein expression, as opposed to functional, level [45,46,47]. Previously, mice with a genetic depletion of somatodendritic 5-HT1A autoreceptors demonstrated increased firing of 5-HT neurons and increased physiological reactivity to stress [48]. Moreover, here administration of the selective 5-HT1A receptor antagonist WAY-100635 mimicked the effect of paroxetine discontinuation on c-Fos/TPH2 double-labelled DRN neurons, in accord with previous studies showing that WAY-100635 (0.1 mg/kg i.v.) increased the firing of 5-HT neurons [49, 50]. These effects of WAY-100635 are most likely mediated by the 5-HT1A receptor for which its affinity is almost 2 orders higher than the next binding site tested, α1-adrenoceptors [51], which are well known to inhibit 5-HT activity when antagonised. This is not to say that 5-HT1A receptor blockade fully models the effects of SSRI discontinuation, not least because such agents block not only pre- but also postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors. Nevertheless, rapid relief from negative 5-HT feedback control seems a likely contributor to an increase in excitability of 5-HT neurons after SSRI discontinuation although the present study does not rule out the involvement of other adaptive changes.
A rebound increase in 5-HT seems difficult to reconcile with reinstatement of SSRI treatment being commonly used to manage the discontinuation. However, SSRI reinstatement would likely restore 5-HT feedback (i.e. indirectly activate 5-HT1A autoreceptors) and thereby dampen down 5-HT neuron hyperexcitability that is a potential driver of discontinuation symptoms. A rebound increase in 5-HT also contrasts with ideas that SSRI discontinuation is mediated by reduced synaptic 5-HT following relief of 5-HT transporter blockade [11]. As evidence, tryptophan depletion in SSRI-treated patients was suggested to elicit SSRI discontinuation effects rather than simple depression relapse, although it is now clear that these effects are distinguishable [52]. However, a decrease in 5-HT availability by tryptophan depletion might relieve 5-HT feedback to generate 5-HT neuron instability and trigger some of the effects of SSRI discontinuation.
The current study found that on day 2 following paroxetine discontinuation, mice showed increased anxiety-like behaviour on the EPM. This replicates the finding in our recent study [8] and is consistent with an earlier report of increased acoustic startle responsivity in rats within a few days of discontinuation from citalopram [7]. There is much evidence that increased 5-HT transmission generates an anxiogenic effect on the EPM and in other anxiety paradigms [9, 35], suggesting that the discontinuation-induced increase in 5-HT function and anxiety-like behaviour may be causally linked. However, the anxiogenic effect of paroxetine discontinuation had dissipated by discontinuation day 5 when increases in 5-HT neuronal function were still evident. Explanations for this mismatch in timing include the possibility that there is adaptation to increased 5-HT function on discontinuation day 2, resulting in normalisation of behaviour on the EPM on discontinuation day 5. It is also possible that other mechanisms contribute to increased anxiety-like behaviour. For example, it is speculated that a rebound increase in cholinergic transmission contributes to discontinuation effects of various antidepressants including paroxetine, which has moderate affinity for muscarinic receptors [11, 12, 53]. Also, an increase in anxiety-like behaviour could be the result of the rapid lifting of the inhibitory 5-HT tone on noradrenergic neurons exerted by paroxetine [54].
Aside from anxiety, a rebound increase in 5-HT neurotransmission after paroxetine discontinuation might generate other behaviours or physiological changes that were not monitored here. For instance, we recently reported that mice demonstrated evidence of sleep disruption that commenced 2 days after discontinuation from paroxetine and continued for up to 9 days [55]. Sleep is well known to be regulated by 5-HT, and sleep disturbances are a recognised feature of SSRI discontinuation syndrome.
Interestingly, increased 5-HT neuron excitability in response to SSRI discontinuation has parallels with a previously proposed account of withdrawal from psychotropic drug administration. According to this account, adaptive influences develop when neural systems are subjected to prolonged suppression and rebound when, on drug removal, these oppositional influences no longer meet resistance [23]. Thus, rebound increases in neurotransmission are reported in response to discontinued administration of other psychotropic drugs; for example, a rebound increase in excitatory transmission is associated with alcohol and benzodiazepine withdrawal [21, 24] whereas opiate withdrawal is associated with elevated noradrenergic activity [22]. Variation in the symptoms of withdrawal from these different classes of psychotropic drugs likely reflects the different neurotransmitter systems involved [23].
In conclusion, we report evidence that SSRI discontinuation is associated with a rebound increase in 5-HT function that lasts many days. We speculate that this effect is linked to a rapid fall in extracellular 5-HT following abrupt cessation of SSRI administration, leading to disinhibition of 5-HT neurons via removal of inhibitory 5-HT feedback. This response to SSRI discontinuation resembles the changes of other neural systems following cessation of treatment with other psychotropic drugs, suggesting a common neurobiological mechanism.
Data availability
Correspondence and requests for materials and raw data should be addressed to Trevor Sharp.
References
Haddad P. Newer antidepressants and the discontinuation syndrome. J Clin Psychiatry. 1997;58:17–21.
Horowitz M, Taylor D. How do we determine whether antidepressants are useful or not? Lancet Psychiatry. 2019;6:888.
Davies J, Read J. A systematic review into the incidence, severity and duration of antidepressant withdrawal effects: Are guidelines evidence-based? Addict Behav. 2019;97:111–21.
Price JS, Waller PC, Wood SM, MacKay AV. A comparison of the post-marketing safety of four selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors including the investigation of symptoms occurring on withdrawal. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 1996;42:757–63.
Gastaldon C, Schoretsanitis G, Arzenton E, Raschi E, Papola D, Ostuzzi G, et al. Withdrawal Syndrome Following Discontinuation of 28 Antidepressants: Pharmacovigilance Analysis of 31,688 Reports from the WHO Spontaneous Reporting Database. Drug Safety. 2022;45:1539–49.
Fava GA, Gatti A, Belaise C, Guidi J, Offidani E. Withdrawal Symptoms after Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitor Discontinuation: A Systematic Review. Psychother Psychosom. 2015;84:72–81.
Bosker FJ, Tanke MA, Jongsma ME, Cremers TI, Jagtman E, Pietersen CY, et al. Biochemical and behavioral effects of long-term citalopram administration and discontinuation in rats: role of serotonin synthesis. Neurochem Int. 2010;57:948–57.
Collins HM, Pinacho R, Ozdemir D, Bannerman DM, Sharp T. Effect of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor discontinuation on anxiety-like behaviours in mice. J Psychopharmacol. 2022;36:794–805.
Handley SL, McBlane JW. Serotonin mechanisms in animal models of anxiety. Braz J Med Biol Res. 1993;26:1–13.
Sharp T. Molecular and cellular mechanisms of antidepressant action. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2013;14:309–25.
Blier P, Tremblay P. Physiologic mechanisms underlying the antidepressant discontinuation syndrome. J Clin Psychiatry. 2006;67:8–13.
Harvey BH, Slabbert FN. New insights on the antidepressant discontinuation syndrome. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2014;29:503–16.
Renoir T. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor antidepressant treatment discontinuation syndrome: a review of the clinical evidence and the possible mechanisms involved. Front Pharmacol. 2013;4:45.
Trouvin JH, Gardier AM, Chanut E, Pages N, Jacquot C. Time course of brain serotonin metabolism after cessation of long-term fluoxetine treatment in the rat. Life Sci. 1993;52:PL187–92.
Stenfors C, Ross SB. Evidence for involvement of 5-hydroxytryptamine(1B) autoreceptors in the enhancement of serotonin turnover in the mouse brain following repeated treatment with fluoxetine. Life Sci. 2002;71:2867–80.
Fuller RW, Perry KW, Molloy BB. Effect of an uptake inhibitor on serotonin metabolism in rat brain: studies with 3-(p-trifluoromethylphenoxy)-N-methyl-3-phenylpropylamine (Lilly 110140). Life Sci. 1974;15:1161–71.
Ogren SO, Ross SB, Hall H, Holm AC, Renyi AL. The pharmacology of zimelidine: a 5-HT selective reuptake inhibitor. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1981;290:127–51.
Barton CL, Hutson PH. Inhibition of hippocampal 5-HT synthesis by fluoxetine and paroxetine: evidence for the involvement of both 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B/D autoreceptors. Synapse. 1999;31:13–19.
Kuhn DM, Wolf WA, Youdim MB. Serotonin neurochemistry revisited: A new look at some old axioms. Neurochem Int. 1986;8:141–54.
Sharp T, Bramwell SR, Clark D, Grahame-Smith DG. In vivo measurement of extracellular 5-hydroxytryptamine in hippocampus of the anaesthetized rat using microdialysis: changes in relation to 5-hydroxytryptaminergic neuronal activity. J Neurochem. 1989;53:234–40.
Rogawski MA. Update on the neurobiology of alcohol withdrawal seizures. Epilepsy Curr. 2005;5:225–30.
Srivastava AB, Mariani JJ, Levin FR. New directions in the treatment of opioid withdrawal. Lancet. 2020;395:1938–48.
Lerner A, Klein M. Dependence, withdrawal and rebound of CNS drugs: an update and regulatory considerations for new drugs development. Brain Commun. 2019;1:fcz025.
Warlick H, Leon L, Patel R, Filoramo S, Knipe R, Joubran E, et al. Application of gabapentinoids and novel compounds for the treatment of benzodiazepine dependence: the glutamatergic model. Mol Biol Rep. 2023;50:1765–84.
Antoniadou I, Kouskou M, Arsiwala T, Singh N, Vasudevan SR, Fowler T, et al. Ebselen has lithium-like effects on central 5-HT(2A) receptor function. Br J Pharmacol. 2018;175:2599–610.
Franklin, KBJ, Paxinos, G. The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. Academic Press, (1997).
Radford-Smith DE, Probert F, Burnet P, Anthony DC. Modifying the maternal microbiota alters the gut-brain metabolome and prevents emotional dysfunction in the adult offspring of obese dams. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2022;119:e2108581119.
Gorlova A, Ortega G, Waider J, Bazhenova N, Veniaminova E, Proshin A, et al. Stress-induced aggression in heterozygous TPH2 mutant mice is associated with alterations in serotonin turnover and expression of 5-HT6 and AMPA subunit 2A receptors. J Affect Disord. 2020;272:440–51.
Guilloux JP, David DJ, Xia L, Nguyen HT, Rainer Q, Guiard BP, et al. Characterization of 5-HT(1A/1B)-/- mice: an animal model sensitive to anxiolytic treatments. Neuropharmacology. 2011;61:478–88.
Couch Y, Xie Q, Lundberg L, Sharp T, Anthony DC. A Model of Post-Infection Fatigue Is Associated with Increased TNF and 5-HT2A Receptor Expression in Mice. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0130643.
Vogelgesang S, Niebert S, Renner U, Möbius W, Hülsmann S, Manzke T, et al. Analysis of the Serotonergic System in a Mouse Model of Rett Syndrome Reveals Unusual Upregulation of Serotonin Receptor 5b. Front Mol Neurosci. 2017;10:61.
Yu Z, Lin D, Zhong Y, Luo B, Liu S, Fei E, et al. Transmembrane protein 108 involves in adult neurogenesis in the hippocampal dentate gyrus. Cell Biosci. 2019;9:9.
Sharp T, Boothman L, Raley J, Quérée P. Important messages in the ‘post’: recent discoveries in 5-HT neurone feedback control. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2007;28:629–36.
Trillat AC, Malagié I, Mathé-Allainmat M, Anmela MC, Jacquot C, Langlois M, et al. Effects of WAY 100635 and (-)-5-Me-8-OH-DPAT, a novel 5-HT1A receptor antagonist, on 8-OH-DPAT responses. Eur J Pharmacol. 1998;347:41–9.
Ohmura Y, Tsutsui-Kimura I, Sasamori H, Nebuka M, Nishitani N, Tanaka KF, et al. Different roles of distinct serotonergic pathways in anxiety-like behavior, antidepressant-like, and anti-impulsive effects. Neuropharmacology. 2020;167:107703.
Kreilgaard M, Smith DG, Brennum LT, Sánchez C. Prediction of clinical response based on pharmacokinetic/pharmacodynamic models of 5-hydroxytryptamine reuptake inhibitors in mice. Br J Pharmacol. 2008;155:276–84.
Bourin M, Chue P, Guillon Y. Paroxetine: a review. CNS Drug Rev. 2001;7:25–47.
Hajos-Korcsok E, McTavish SF, Sharp T. Effect of a selective 5-hydroxytryptamine reuptake inhibitor on brain extracellular noradrenaline: microdialysis studies using paroxetine. Eur J Pharmacol. 2000;407:101–7.
Rosenbaum JF, Fava M, Hoog SL, Ascroft RC, Krebs WB. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor discontinuation syndrome: a randomized clinical trial. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;44:77–87.
Michelson D, Fava M, Amsterdam J, Apter J, Londborg P, Tamura R, et al. Interruption of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment. Double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Br J Psychiatry. 2000;176:363–8.
Blier P, Bouchard C. Modulation of 5-HT release in the guinea-pig brain following long-term administration of antidepressant drugs. Br J Pharmacol. 1994;113:485–95.
el Mansari M, Bouchard C, Blier P. Alteration of serotonin release in the guinea pig orbito-frontal cortex by selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Relevance to treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1995;13:117–27.
Fritze S, Spanagel R, Noori HR. Adaptive dynamics of the 5-HT systems following chronic administration of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors: a meta-analysis. J Neurochem. J Neurochem. 2017;142:747–55.
Gartside SE, Umbers V, Hajós M, Sharp T. Interaction between a selective 5-HT1A receptor antagonist and an SSRI in vivo: effects on 5-HT cell firing and extracellular 5-HT. Br J Pharmacol. 1995;115:1064–70.
Jolas T, Haj-Dahmane S, Kidd EJ, Langlois X, Lanfumey L, Fattaccini CM, et al. Central pre- and postsynaptic 5-HT1A receptors in rats treated chronically with a novel antidepressant, cericlamine. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1994;268:1432–43.
Le Poul E, Laaris N, Doucet E, Laporte AM, Hamon M, Lanfumey L. Early desensitization of somato-dendritic 5-HT1A autoreceptors in rats treated with fluoxetine or paroxetine. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol. 1995;352:141–8.
Albert PR, Francois BL. Modifying 5-HT1A Receptor Gene Expression as a New Target for Antidepressant Therapy. Front Neurosci. 2010;4:35.
Richardson-Jones JW, Craige CP, Guiard BP, Stephen A, Metzger KL, Kung HF, et al. 5-HT1A autoreceptor levels determine vulnerability to stress and response to antidepressants. Neuron. 2010;65:40–52.
Hajós M, Hoffmann WE, Tetko IV, Hyland B, Sharp T, Villa AE. Different tonic regulation of neuronal activity in the rat dorsal raphe and medial prefrontal cortex via 5-HT(1A) receptors. Neurosci Lett. 2001;304:129–32.
Fornal CA, Metzler CW, Gallegos RA, Veasey SC, McCreary AC, Jacobs BL. WAY-100635, a potent and selective 5-hydroxytryptamine1A antagonist, increases serotonergic neuronal activity in behaving cats: comparison with (S)-WAY-100135. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1996;278:752–62.
Forster EA, Cliffe IA, Bill DJ, Dover GM, Jones D, Reilly Y, et al. A pharmacological profile of the selective silent 5-HT1A receptor antagonist, WAY-100635. Eur J Pharmacol. 1995;281:81–8.
Delgado PL. Monoamine depletion studies: implications for antidepressant discontinuation syndrome. J Clin Psychiatry. 2006;67:22–6.
Owens MJ, Morgan WN, Plott SJ, Nemeroff CB. Neurotransmitter receptor and transporter binding profile of antidepressants and their metabolites. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1997;283:1305–22.
Szabo ST, Blier P. Functional and pharmacological characterization of the modulatory role of serotonin on the firing activity of locus coeruleus norepinephrine neurons. Brain Res. 2001;922:9–20.
Collins HM, Pinacho R, Tam S, Sharp T, Bannerman DM, Peirson SN. Continuous home cage monitoring of activity and sleep in mice during repeated paroxetine treatment and discontinuation. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2023;240:2403–18.
Funding
This work was supported by a Wellcome Trust PhD Studentship in Basic Science (HC; grant no. 219982/Z/19/Z), a studentship from the Oxford-MRC Doctoral Training Partnership (SG), and research support funds from the University of Oxford.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
HMC performed experiments, analysed the data, and contributed to writing the manuscript. FPC and SG performed the qPCR experiments. YS, ED, and DO contributed to the ex vivo tissue analysis. CL contributed to the EPM experiments. RP aided design of the work and manuscript preparation. TS and DMB made contributions to the conception and design of the work, drafting, and revising the manuscript, and analysis and interpretation of data.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Collins, H.M., Gullino, L.S., Ozdemir, D. et al. Rebound activation of 5-HT neurons following SSRI discontinuation. Neuropsychopharmacol. 49, 1580–1589 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-024-01857-8
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-024-01857-8