Abstract
Lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) is a potent classic serotonergic psychedelic, which facilitates a variety of altered states of consciousness. Here we present the first meta-analysis establishing dose-response relationship estimates of the altered states of consciousness induced by LSD. Data extracted from articles identified by a systematic literature review following PRISMA guidelines were obtained from the Altered States Database. The psychometric data comprised ratings of subjective effects from standardized and validated questionnaires: the Altered States of Consciousness Rating Scale (5D-ASC, 11-ASC) and the Mystical Experience Questionnaire (MEQ30). We performed meta-regression analyses using restricted cubic splines for data from studies with LSD doses of up to 200 μg base. Most scales revealed a sigmoid-like increase of effects, with a plateauing at around 100 μg. The most strongly modulated factors referred to changes in perception and illusory imagination, followed by positively experienced ego-dissolution, while only small effects were found for Anxiety and Dread of Ego Dissolution. The considerable variability observed in most factors and scales points to the role of non-pharmacological factors in shaping subjective experiences. The established dose-response relationships may be used as general references for future experimental and clinical research on LSD to compare observed with expected subjective effects and to elucidate phenomenological differences between psychedelics.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) is the prototype of classic serotonergic psychedelics, a group of substances which unfold their psychoactive properties predominantly via the serotonin 2 A (5-HT2A) receptor [1]. Psychedelics embrace structurally heterogenous subgroups like phenethylamines (e.g., mescaline) and tryptamines (e.g., psilocybin, N,N-DMT) [2], as well as substances from the ergoline subgroup (e.g., LSD) which have been characterized as “rigidified tryptamines” [3]. The term ‘psychedelics’ is also used in a broader sense, including non-serotonergic drugs like ketamine, PCP or MDMA. The use of the term ‘psychedelic experience’ is not limited to the effects induced by specific substances, instead refers to a group of psychological effects. However, there is no clear definition on the exact set of consciousness alterations that define a psychedelic experience. Here, we will refer to classic serotonergic psychedelics and the effects they induce when using the term ‘psychedelics’ or ‘psychedelic experience’. Several studies suggest that qualitatively, LSD might not be differentiated from other psychedelics with regard to the induced psychologic effects [4,5,6]. On the other hand, anecdotal reports mention differences in subjective experiences regarding different substances [7], and LSD somewhat differs from pharmacodynamical profiles of other 5-HT2A agonists, including a broader variety of receptor targets [3].
After its initial synthesis in 1938, LSD’s psychedelic properties have accidentally been discovered in 1943 by the Swiss pharmacologist Albert Hofmann [8]. Ever since, LSD has been the most extensively investigated psychedelic from the 1950s to the 1970s, with more than 1000 scientific papers published in the context of basic science, as well as in clinical research as a therapeutic tool [9,10,11]. Most intensively studied indications included, among others, alcoholism [12] and existential distress in life-threatening physical illness [13]. After a hiatus of more than 20 years, during which regulatory hurdles prevented research on psychedelics, research eventually resumed in the 1990s. Also, in recreational underground use LSD is by far the most frequently used psychedelic worldwide [14], and doses of psychedelics are often compared to LSD equivalents by users.
LSD has recently been re-evaluated for the treatment of different mental health conditions, like anxiety and depression in patients with [15, 16] and without life-threatening illness [15]. Psychological effects of psychedelics underlie specific temporal dynamics [9], including (acute) psychedelic experiences, subacute effects, and long-term (enduring) effects [17]. There is some evidence that the quality and intensity of acute psychedelic effects might predict therapeutic outcome [18]. Thus, the classification and description of acute psychedelic experiences appear to be of high importance when it comes to optimizing treatment interventions regarding efficacy and safety. In order to determine the optimal dosing ranges of LSD for future clinical studies, first and most importantly the influence of the LSD dose on the nature and intensity of acute subjective effects needs characterization. Over the last decades some gold standards for the assessment of altered states phenomena have been established in terms of several well-validated questionnaires for a retrospective assessment of altered experiences [9, 19,20,21]. Such standardized assessment allows meta-analytic comparisons, as recently presented to establish dose-response relationships for altered experiences induced by psilocybin [20].
The data to establish dose-response relationships for LSD is limited by the availability of studies that cohere to current research standards, as LSD has only recently returned to basic and clinical research. The range of LSD doses employed in current research is on average markedly below the doses administered in studies from the 1950 to 70 s. At that time, high doses mostly ranging between 400 and 600 μg were applied, especially in the treatment of alcohol use disorder, whereas in recent studies doses of 200 μg have not been exceeded, neither in healthy subjects [22], nor in clinical samples [15, 16, 23]. Regarding the lower end of the dose spectrum, applications of so-called microdosing are gaining increasing interest both in recreational and scientific settings. It is thought that such low doses, which do not induce subjectively identifiable drug effects, can positively affect cognitive processes and improve mental health indicators [24,25,26,27]. The exact dose range of LSD microdosing is still debated; a recent literature review suggests a “plausible microdose range” for LSD of 6–20 μg [24]. Only few studies directly tested the dose-dependency of LSD’s effects [22, 26, 28]. With the application of within-subject designs, these studies provide well-controlled experimental conditions, while they do not allow to assess study-specific impacts on participants experiences. Such non-pharmacological effects appear, however, particularly pronounced for psychedelic substances, and are generally referred to as set and setting effects [29, 30]. Cross-study comparisons are essential to obtain data-based estimates of the variability of subjective LSD experiences across research sites and experimental protocols, as no corresponding meta-analysis is available to date.
With the present meta-analysis, we aim to obtain estimates of the relationship between LSD doses and the quality of psychedelic experiences. The data stem from the Altered States Database (ASDB, http://alteredstatesdb.org, [31]), which is a regularly updated database with questionnaire data extracted from articles identified by a systematic literature review, adhering to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) 2020 Statement Guidelines [32]. Available data was found for the Altered States of Consciousness Rating Scale (5D-ASC, 11-ASC) and the Mystical Experience Questionnaire (MEQ30) for a dose range up to 200 μg.
Methods
Included data
Psychometric questionnaire data on the subjective experience of LSD were included in this meta-analysis. The data has been retrieved from the ASDB repository on Open Science Framework (OSF; https://osf.io/8mbru, version ASDB_v.2022-12-31), which contains data from MEDLINE-listed studies on altered states of consciousness published from 1975-01-01 to 2022-12-31. The ASDB stems from a systematic literature review according to PRISMA standards [32] as described in Prugger et al. (2022) [33] and Peters et al. (2023) [34]. Datasets investigating the subjective effects of LSD were retrieved from the ASDB. The PRISMA flowchart showing the process of item identification and screening can be found in the Supplementary Material.
Data were excluded if experimental conditions comprised applications of combinations of substances [28, 35, 36] such as pre-treatments with ketanserin, if the LSD dose was unclear [37], or if data were about recreational LSD usage [38]. From studies reporting multiple questionnaire applications at different time points during the same day of the experimental session [39, 40], only the final and complete questionnaire application was included describing the overall experience. Also, after consultation with the authors, the LSD doses in a series of reports [22, 35, 36, 39, 41] were adjusted as suggested in [28, 42], due to administration of capsules containing an unstable LSD formulation leading to dispersion of lower than presumed LSD doses. Studies reporting on administration of both LSD base and LSD tartrate have been included. LSD tartrate doses (from studies [43,44,45]) were converted to the LSD base equivalent by multiplying by 0.685 [46] (See Table 1).
Questionnaires
This meta-analysis included psychometric data from commonly applied questionnaires to assess the phenomenology of altered states of consciousness, namely from two versions of the Altered States of Consciousness Rating Scale (the 5D-ASC and the 11-ASC), and from the Mystical Experience Questionnaire (MEQ30). The Altered States of Consciousness Rating Scale [47,48,49] is a self-report questionnaire with 94 items rated on a visual analog scale. Two different analysis schemata are in use: In the 5D-ASC version (“5-Dimensional Altered States of Consciousness Rating Scale” [50, 51]), items are assigned to five core dimensions: (1) Auditory Alterations, (2) Dread of Ego Dissolution, (3) Oceanic Boundlessness, (4) Visionary Restructuralization, and (5) Vigilance Reduction. In the more recent 11-ASC version (“11-factor Altered States of Consciousness Rating Scale” [52]), only 42 of the 94 questionnaires items are used in the analysis, in which item scores are summarized along 11 factors: (1) Experience of Unity, (2) Spiritual Experience, (3) Blissful State, (4) Insightfulness, (5) Disembodiment, (6) Impaired Control and Cognition, (7) Anxiety, (8) Complex Imagery, (9) Elementary Imagery, (10) Audio-Visual Synesthesia, and (11) Changed Meaning of Percepts. Both analysis schemes have been validated and demonstrate good reliability (5D-ASC: Hoyt 0.88–0.95 [51, 53]); 11-ASC: mean Cronbach’s alpha of 0.83 [52]). The Mystical Experience Questionnaire, in its latest version the MEQ30 [54], consists of 30 items assigned to four scales: (1) Mystical, (2) Positive Mood, (3) Transcendence of Time and Space, and (4) Ineffability. This factor structure is currently recommended for analyses and has been assessed for reliability, yielding good scores for all four subscales (Cronbach’s alpha: 0.80–0.95) [55, 56]. A more detailed description of the questionnaires can be found in Schmidt and Majić (2016) [19], Majić et al. (2015) [9], and in a recent review by de Deus Pontual et al. (2022) [21].
Statistical analyses
We performed meta-regression analyses using restricted cubic splines. A restricted cubic spline model is non-linear and consists of a series of piecewise cubic polynomials that are connected at the position of the knots, with a linear curve before the first and after the last knot [57]. It was suggested that four or five knots are usually adequate to fully capture the underlying shape, provided the amount of included studies is sufficiently large [58, 59]. Given the limited number of available studies for the 5D-ASC and MEQ30, we chose three knots for those questionnaire-data and 4 knots for the 11-ASC. To account for repeated measures and potential heteroscedasticity of error-terms resulting from repeated measurements on the same or similar set of participants, we clustered standard errors at the study level and estimated the cluster-robust variance with the small sample adjustment proposed by Pustejovsky & Tipton (2022) [60]. We assumed a within-study correlation p = 0.8 for the variance-covariance matrix. The analyses were performed in R version 4.2.2 [61] with the metafor-package [62] using the restricted cubic spline model from the drc-package [63]. Cluster-robust variance was estimated with the clubSandwich-package [64] in the R robust function. The R-syntax of this analysis is available on GitHub: https://github.com/TimHirschfeld/doseresponse_LSD.
Results
Data description
Sixteen studies assessing the effects of LSD were included in this analysis, with overall 765 LSD applications. Table 1 contains a summary of studies included in this analysis. From the 11-ASC, 31 questionnaire datasets from 15 participant samples were included in the analysis; from the 5D-ASC, 18 datasets from nine samples were included; and from the MEQ30, 16 datasets from eight samples were included. Included studies assessed LSD doses ranging from 4 to 200 μg LSD base.
Dose-response analyses
Radar charts for each questionnaire and dose-response relationships for each factor and scale of the respective questionnaires are presented in Fig. 1 (5D-ASC and 11-ASC) and Fig. 2 (MEQ30). LSD dose-dependently increased effect estimates on all factors and scales of the applied questionnaires. For data exploration and exact dose-effect-determination, the results and summary statistics of this analysis can be accessed via this interactive web-application: https://dose-response-lsd.asdb.info/.
Discussion
A meta-analysis on psychometric data was performed to estimate dose-response relationships of subjective effects of LSD. The analysis included data from applications of doses from 4 to 200 μg LSD base, in healthy and clinical study participants. Given that the data is based on retrospective subjective reports relying on introspection, we did not employ assumptions on the specific shape of the fitted model (e.g., not applying a linear or a sigmoidal model); instead, we opted for fitting restricted cubic splines that allow for flexible model fits. Most of the fitted dose-response functions closely resemble sigmoid curves, which is expected and plausible for biophysiological processes. We established robust dose-response relationships, in which LSD dose increased effects on all scales and factors of the included questionnaires. On the 5D- and 11-ASC, the strongest dose-modulation was found for items referring to changes in perception and imagination (Visionary Restructuralization, Changed Meaning of Percepts) as well as positively experienced dissolution of boundaries between self and surroundings (Oceanic Boundlessness), whereas Anxiety, Auditory Alterations and Dread of Ego Dissolution were barely modulated. On the MEQ30, the strongest dose-modulation was found for Ineffability and the weakest for Mystical.
Most factors and scales exhibit a sigmoid-like shape of dose-effects that either flattens out or reaches a plateau at around 100 μg, while scales and factors with relatively small dose-modulation (Auditory Alterations, Dread of Ego-Dissolution, Anxiety) increase rather linearly. This is in line with a dose-response, within-subject study by Holze et al. (2021) that found similar rating scores for 100 µg and 200 µg for Blissful State, Insightfulness, and Changed Meaning of Percepts [28], but significantly higher rating scores for 200 µg compared to 100 µg on the scale Dread of Ego Dissolution (reflecting disembodiment and fear) [28], indicating a ceiling effect regarding positive subjective effects [65]. Despite considerable variability of effects, the present meta-analysis supports the suggestion that doses between 50 and 100 µg LSD base induce substantial psychological effects in most subjects—a dose range currently used in most clinical trials for therapeutic applications [65]. Accordingly, a dose of about 100 μg LSD base most likely induces a pattern of subjective experiences that resembles what is often called a “(full) psychedelic experience”, while the likelihood of adverse reactions is kept low. Please note that the term “psychedelic experiences/state” is not used consistently and no agreement on defining criteria of a psychedelic experience have been settled on.
LSD doses below 10 μg [65] or 20 μg [24] have previously been proposed as the “microdosing” range of LSD. The fitted curves in our analysis show an onset of effects below 20 μg LSD base. For instance, visual phenomena (11-ASC: Audio-Visual Synesthesia, Complex and Elementary Imagery) show an x-axis intercept at around 10 μg, and at 20 μg reach around 10% of the maximum score. These effects are unlikely due to placebo effects, as those of the given studies which included a placebo condition reported only negligible effects [22, 26, 28, 35, 39, 43,44,45, 66,67,68,69]. However, these estimates are strongly influenced by the fact that multiple studies investigated doses below 20 μg, while there are no studies available in the dose range from 20 to 50 μg, resulting in an overestimation of the curves slope at their onset. Most studies below 20 μg effectively show null-effects and make the microdosing range of 0–20 μg plausible, but more data is needed to determine the average onset of effects more precisely.
A recent systematic review indicates a mediating role of mystical-type experiences for treatment outcomes of psychedelic therapy [70], so special attention is drawn on the dose range to predict such experiences. Barrett et al. (2015) suggest that complete mystical-type experiences are reached with scores of ≥60% on each of the four factors of the MEQ30 [56]. Applying this criterion, the present analysis suggests that such experiences are rather unlikely to occur in the given dose range. Similarly, Liechti et al. (2017) report that full mystical-type experiences occur rather rarely at LSD doses of 200 μg. The authors suggest that mystical-type and spiritual experiences might highly depend on participant characteristics and experimental setting [39], which might explain that 25% and 35% of participants in a study by Family et al. (2022) met criteria for a full mystical experience after 50 µg and 100 µg, respectively [71]. However, considering the results of the MEQ30 factors determined in this meta-analysis, datapoints of mentioned study partially lie outside or in the margins of the calculated confidence intervals and thus may not reflect general outcomes. Overall, full mystical-type experiences seem unlikely to be induced with doses below 200 µg and their occurrence appears to be strongly influenced by non-pharmacological factors. Targeted therapeutic implementation of other factors than dose need to be further investigated.
Non-pharmacological factors that influence psychedelic experiences are often categorized in set (participant personality and preparation and expectation of substance use) and setting (environment of substance administration) [30, 72] and are thought to lead to considerable inter- and intra-individual variability of subjective effects [73, 74]. The studies included in our analysis vary greatly with regard to experimental setting. Bershad et al. (2019) reports of “living-room style” environments with possibilities to relax, read or watch movies between measurements [44], similar to Preller et al. (2017) (“in an aesthetic living-room-like room”) [35] and de Wit et al. (2022) [45]. In other studies, however, participants were also given tasks during LSD applications that involved greater effort, potentially inhibiting the manifestation of certain effects [75]. Previous work indicated that spatially constrained neuroimaging procedures may be demanding for some individuals and could increase the likelihood of challenging experiences [17]. Carhart-Harris et al. (2016) reported on fMRI and MEG measurements of over 60 min each, as well as previous MRI environment habituation and a subsequent battery of cognitive and behavioral tests [66]. From the studies included, also Holze et al. (2020) [67] and Liechti et al. (2017) [39] (fMRI data published in Müller et al., (2017) [41]) reported of fMRI measurements during the experimental session. Schmid et al. (2015) [22], Liechti et al. (2017) [39], Holze et al. (2021 [28], 2022 [5]) reported on “quiet standard hospital patient room” environments, and the study procedure by Family et al. (2022) [71] included a 60 min breathing exercise. Additional factors influencing the psychedelic experience and thereby increasing the variability within and between the given datasets may involve the subject’s age, previous experience with psychedelics or other mind-altering substances, as well as differences in individual pharmacokinetics [76, 77]. The study by Family et al. (2022) for instance reports of applications of 50 µg LSD to predominantly (9 of 12) psychedelic-naïve participants, as well as 75 and 100 µg LSD to psychedelic-non-naïve participants. They report of higher questionnaire ratings on all factors of the 5D-ASC and MEQ30 in the (naïve) 50 µg cohort compared to the (non-naïve) 75 µg cohort, as well as higher ratings on most factors in the (naïve) 50 µg cohort compared to the (non-naïve) 100 µg cohort [71]. The study by Wießner et al. (2021) also reports relatively high 11-ASC scores (especially for the factors Insightfulness, Spiritual Experience, Blissful State, and Complex Imaginary) [68] compared to other studies on the same LSD dose included in this analysis. This may result from relatively high lifetime use of other psychedelics or mind-altering substances among study participants (particularly ayahuasca, with a mean lifetime use of 69 ± 131 S.D.) and the fact that most participants (67%) identify themselves as spiritual. Other studies, however, have not observed any differences in subjective effects between experienced and non-experienced LSD users when compared within the same study [5, 22, 28]. Therefore, differences in extra-pharmacological factors likely led to the observed variance in the ratings of subjective experiences. The confidence intervals of the present study can yield as data-driven estimates for the expected variability of experiences, also in clinical samples (Note: The confidence intervals refer to the mean group effects, while the variance across participants can be substantially higher). Interestingly, factors assessing alterations of visual perception and cognitive abilities exhibit less variability (smaller CI range) than factors assessing experiences of unity, blissful state, and spirituality. This could be due to their highly personal character, which might be stronger influenced by non-pharmacological factors. Alternatively, variability might stem from less reliable assessment of these high-order concepts as compared to factors assessing perceptual alterations. Future studies with a more comparable assessment of non-pharmacological factors are needed to allow for systematic investigation of their exact contributions in shaping subjective experiences.
Further limitations should be considered when using the analysis at hand to determine LSD doses. First, administration methods varied across studies. Most studies administered LSD orally as capsules, while it was administered sublingually in two studies [44, 45] and intravenously in one study [66]. Moreover, LSD formulations differed across studies: most studies applied LSD base (or provided the dose calculated for LSD base), while some doses referred to LSD salt in the form of LSD tartrate—the typical formulation sold on the black market. No direct experimental comparison of the bioequivalence of the two formulations has been made to date. We transformed doses according to Holze et al. (2021) [46], who suggested that 1 µg of LSD base corresponds to ~1.46 µg of 1:1 LSD tartrate or 1.23–1.33 µg 2:1 LSD tartrate, depending on the salt form and amount of crystal water [65]. Moreover, the results reflect estimates on the variability of average effects in study samples based on doses of up to 200 µg, so the experiences of individual subjects may differ considerably. Finally, it should be noted that study samples are usually comprised of highly selected and well-prepared participants. For these reasons and because the quality and quantity of recreationally used products is often unclear, the present results do not necessarily apply to the general population or to recreational use outside controlled laboratory or therapeutic settings.
In conclusion, the present meta-analysis on subjective effects of LSD in study participants revealed dose-dependent alterations of consciousness with substantial heterogeneity of effects across laboratory and therapeutic settings on all scales and factors assessed by given questionnaires. The strongest dose-modulation was found for perceptual and imaginative changes followed by positively experienced dissolution of boundaries between self and surroundings with a limited spiritual or mystical-type character in the given dosage range. Dose-response relationships resemble sigmoid functions on most factors and scales of the questionnaires, with an observable ceiling effect at circa 100 μg LSD base. Results may be used as a general reference to relate observed with expected dose-specific effects.
References
Vollenweider FX, Vollenweider-Scherpenhuyzen MFI, Bäbler A, Vogel H, Hell D. Psilocybin induces schizophrenia-like psychosis in humans via a serotonin-2 agonist action. NeuroReport. 1998;9:3897–902. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199812010-00024.
Nichols DE. Chemistry and Structure–Activity Relationships of Psychedelics. In: Halberstadt AL, Vollenweider FX, Nichols DE, editors. Behav. Neurobiol. Psychedelic Drugs, vol. 36, Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg; 2017. p. 1–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2017_475.
Nichols DE. Psychedelics. Pharm Rev. 2016;68:264–355. https://doi.org/10.1124/pr.115.011478.
Hartman AM, Hollister LE. Effect of mescaline, lysergic acid diethylamide and psilocybin on color perception. Psychopharmacologia. 1963;4:441–51. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00403349.
Holze F, Ley L, Müller F, Becker AM, Straumann I, Vizeli P, et al. Direct comparison of the acute effects of lysergic acid diethylamide and psilocybin in a double-blind placebo-controlled study in healthy subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2022;47:1180–7. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-022-01297-2.
Wolbach AB, Miner EJ, Isbell H. Comparison of psilocin with psilocybin, mescaline and LSD-25. Psychopharmacologia. 1962;3:219–23. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00412109.
Shulgin AT, Shulgin A. Tihkal: the continuation. Berkeley:Transform; 1997.
Hofmann A. LSD, my problem child and insights/outlooks. Beckley Foundation. Oxford University Press; 2013.
Majić T, Schmidt TT, Gallinat J. Peak experiences and the afterglow phenomenon: When and how do therapeutic effects of hallucinogens depend on psychedelic experiences? J Psychopharmacol (Oxf). 2015;29:241–53. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881114568040.
Vollenweider FX, Kometer M. The neurobiology of psychedelic drugs: implications for the treatment of mood disorders. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:642–51. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2884.
Vollenweider FX, Preller KH. Psychedelic drugs: neurobiology and potential for treatment of psychiatric disorders. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2020;21:611–24. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-020-0367-2.
Krebs TS, Johansen P-Ø. Lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) for alcoholism: meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Psychopharmacol (Oxf). 2012;26:994–1002. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881112439253.
Reiche S, Hermle L, Gutwinski S, Jungaberle H, Gasser P, Majić T. Serotonergic hallucinogens in the treatment of anxiety and depression in patients suffering from a life-threatening disease: A systematic review. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2018;81:1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.09.012.
Evens R, Reiche S, Marek RM, Moon DU, Groß RE, Romanello A, et al. Psychedelic Experiences During the Early COVID-19 Pandemic: Findings From an International Online Survey. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12:732028 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.732028.
Holze F, Gasser P, Müller F, Dolder PC, Liechti ME. Lysergic acid diethylamide-assisted therapy in patients with anxiety with and without a life-threatening illness A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled Phase II study. Biol Psychiatry. 2022:S0006322322015530. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2022.08.025.
Gasser P, Holstein D, Michel Y, Doblin R, Yazar-Klosinski B, Passie T, et al. Safety and Efficacy of Lysergic Acid Diethylamide-Assisted Psychotherapy for Anxiety Associated With Life-threatening Diseases. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2014;202:513–20. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0000000000000113.
Studerus E, Kometer M, Hasler F, Vollenweider FX. Acute, subacute and long-term subjective effects of psilocybin in healthy humans: a pooled analysis of experimental studies. J Psychopharmacol Oxf Engl. 2011;25:1434–52. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881110382466.
Yaden DB, Griffiths RR. The Subjective Effects of Psychedelics Are Necessary for Their Enduring Therapeutic Effects. ACS Pharm Transl Sci. 2021;4:568–72. https://doi.org/10.1021/acsptsci.0c00194.
Schmidt TT, Majić T. Empirische Untersuchung veränderter Bewusstseinszustände. Handb. Psychoaktive Subst., Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg; 2016. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-55214-4_65-1.
Hirschfeld T, Schmidt TT. Dose–response relationships of psilocybin-induced subjective experiences in humans. J Psychopharmacol (Oxf). 2021;35:384–97. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881121992676.
de Deus Pontual AA, Senhorini HG, Corradi-Webster CM, Tófoli LF, Daldegan-Bueno D. Systematic Review of Psychometric Instruments Used in Research with Psychedelics. J Psychoactive Drugs. 2022 May 26:1–10. https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2022.2079108.
Schmid Y, Enzler F, Gasser P, Grouzmann E, Preller KH, Vollenweider FX, et al. Acute Effects of Lysergic Acid Diethylamide in Healthy Subjects. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;78:544–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.11.015.
Gasser P, Kirchner K, Passie T. LSD-assisted psychotherapy for anxiety associated with a life-threatening disease: a qualitative study of acute and sustained subjective effects. J Psychopharmacol Oxf Engl. 2015;29:57–68. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881114555249.
Polito V, Liknaitzky P. The emerging science of microdosing: A systematic review of research on low dose psychedelics (1955-2021) and recommendations for the field. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2022;139:104706 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104706.
Griffiths RR, Johnson MW, Carducci MA, Umbricht A, Richards WA, Richards BD, et al. Psilocybin produces substantial and sustained decreases in depression and anxiety in patients with life-threatening cancer: A randomized double-blind trial. J Psychopharmacol Oxf Engl. 2016;30:1181–97. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881116675513.
Hutten NRPW, Mason NL, Dolder PC, et al. Mood and cognition after administration of low LSD doses in healthy volunteers: A placebo controlled dose-effect finding study. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol J Eur Coll Neuropsychopharmacol. 2020;41:81–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2020.10.002.
Hutten NRPW, Mason NL, Dolder PC, et al. KPC. Motives and Side-Effects of Microdosing With Psychedelics Among Users. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2019;22:426–34. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijnp/pyz029.
Holze F, Vizeli P, Ley L, Müller F, Dolder P, Stocker M, et al. Acute dose-dependent effects of lysergic acid diethylamide in a double-blind placebo-controlled study in healthy subjects. Neuropsychopharmacol Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol. 2021;46:537–44. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-00883-6.
Modlin NL, Miller TM, Rucker JJ, Kirlic N, Lennard-Jones M, Schlosser D, et al. Optimizing outcomes in psilocybin therapy: Considerations in participant evaluation and preparation. J Affect Disord. 2023;326:18–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2023.01.077.
Zinberg NE. Drug, Set, and Setting: The Basis for Controlled Intoxicant Use. New Haven CT, Yale University Press; 1984.
Schmidt TT, Berkemeyer H. The Altered States Database: Psychometric Data of Altered States of Consciousness. Front Psychol. 2018;9:720 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01028.
Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.n71.
Prugger J, Derdiyok E, Dinkelacker J, Costines C, Schmidt TT. The Altered States Database: Psychometric data from a systematic literature review. Sci Data. 2022;9:720 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41597-022-01822-4.
Peters M, Prugger J, Dimkov P, Schmidt TT. Update of the Altered States Database (ASDB): 2022-12-31. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/wdfhg. Accessed 02 May 2023.
Preller KH, Herdener M, Pokorny T, Planzer A, Kraehenmann R, Stämpfli P, et al. The Fabric of Meaning and Subjective Effects in LSD-Induced States Depend on Serotonin 2A Receptor Activation. Curr Biol CB. 2017;27:451–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.030.
Kraehenmann R, Pokorny D, Aicher H, Preller KH, Pokorny T, Bosch OG, et al. LSD Increases Primary Process Thinking via Serotonin 2A Receptor Activation. Front Pharm. 2017;8:814 https://doi.org/10.3389/fphar.2017.00814.
Schmid Y, Gasser P, Oehen P, Liechti ME. Acute subjective effects in LSD- and MDMA-assisted psychotherapy. J Psychopharmacol Oxf Engl. 2021;35:362–74. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881120959604.
Griffiths RR, Hurwitz ES, Davis AK, Johnson MW, Jesse R. Survey of subjective ‘God encounter experiences’: Comparisons among naturally occurring experiences and those occasioned by the classic psychedelics psilocybin, LSD, ayahuasca, or DMT. PloS ONE. 2019;14:e0214377 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0214377.
Liechti ME, Dolder PC, Schmid Y. Alterations of consciousness and mystical-type experiences after acute LSD in humans. Psychopharmacol (Berl). 2017;234:1499–510. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-016-4453-0.
Preller KH, Burt JB, Ji JL, Schleifer CH, Adkinson BD, Stampfli P, et al. Changes in global and thalamic brain connectivity in LSD-induced altered states of consciousness are attributable to the 5-HT2A receptor. Elife. 2018;7:e35082 https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.35082.
Müller F, Lenz C, Dolder P, Lang U, Schmidt A, Liechti M, et al. Increased thalamic resting-state connectivity as a core driver of LSD-induced hallucinations. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2017;136:648–57. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.12818.
Holze F, Duthaler U, Vizeli P, Müller F, Borgwardt S, Liechti ME. Pharmacokinetics and subjective effects of a novel oral LSD formulation in healthy subjects. Br J Clin Pharm. 2019;85:1474–83. https://doi.org/10.1111/bcp.13918.
Murray CH, Tare I, Perry CM, Malina M, Lee R, de Wit H. Low doses of LSD reduce broadband oscillatory power and modulate event-related potentials in healthy adults. Psychopharmacol (Berl). 2021. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-021-05991-9.
Bershad AK, Schepers ST, Bremmer MP, Lee R, de Wit H. Acute Subjective and Behavioral Effects of Microdoses of Lysergic Acid Diethylamide in Healthy Human Volunteers. Biol Psychiatry. 2019;86:792–800. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.05.019.
de Wit H, Molla HM, Bershad A, Bremmer M, Lee R. Repeated low doses of LSD in healthy adults: A placebo-controlled, dose-response study. Addict Biol. 2022;27:e13143 https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.13143.
Holze F, Liechti ME, Hutten NRPW, Mason NL, Dolder PC, Theunissen EL, et al. Pharmacokinetics and Pharmacodynamics of Lysergic Acid Diethylamide Microdoses in Healthy Participants. Clin Pharm Ther. 2021;109:658–66. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpt.2057.
Dittrich A. Zusammenstellung eines Fragebogens (APZ) zur Erfassung abnormer psychischer Zustände [Construction of a questionnaire (APZ) for assessing abnormal mental states]. Z Klin Psychol Psychiatr Psychother. 1975;23:12–20.
Dittrich A. Ätiologie-unabhängige Strukturen veränderter Wachbewußtseinszustände: Ergebnisse empirischer Untersuchungen über Halluzinogene I. und II. Ordnung, sensorische Deprivation, hypnagoge Zustände, hypnotische Verfahren sowie Reizüberflutung. [Etiology-independent structures of altered states of consciousness. Results of empirical studies on hallucinogens of the first and second order, sensory deprivation, hypnagogic states, hypnotic procedures, and sensory overload]. Enke, Stuttgart, Germany; 1985.
Dittrich A. The standardized psychometric assessment of altered states of consciousness (ASCs) in humans. Pharmacopsychiatry. 1998;31:80–4. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2007-979351.
Bodmer I, Dittrich A, Lamparter D. Aussergewöhnliche Bewusstseinszustände-Ihre gemeinsame Struktur und Messung [Altered states of consciousness-Their common structure and assessment]. Welten Bewusstseins Bd. 1994;3:45–58.
Dittrich A, Lamparter D, Maurer M. 5D-ABZ: Fragebogen zur Erfassung Aussergewöhnlicher Bewusstseinszustände. Eine kurze Einführung [5D-ASC: Questionnaire for the Assessment of Altered States of Consciousness. A Short Introduction]. PSIN PLUS, Zurich, Switzerland; 2006.
Studerus E, Gamma A, Vollenweider FX. Psychometric Evaluation of the Altered States of Consciousness Rating Scale (OAV). PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e12412 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0012412.
Dittrich A, Lamparter D, Maurer M. 5D-ASC: Questionnaire for the Assessment of Altered States of Consciousness. A Short Introduction. 3rd ed. PSIN PLUS, Zurich, Switzerland; 2010.
MacLean KA, Leoutsakos J-MS, Johnson MW, Griffiths RR. Factor Analysis of the Mystical Experience Questionnaire: A Study of Experiences Occasioned by the Hallucinogen Psilocybin. J Sci Study Relig. 2012;51:721–37. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5906.2012.01685.x.
Barrett FS, Griffiths RR. The factor structure of the Mystical Experience Questionnaire (MEQ): Reply to Bouso et al., 2016. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2017;32:1 https://doi.org/10.1002/hup.2564.
Barrett FS, Johnson MW, Griffiths RR. Validation of the revised Mystical Experience Questionnaire in experimental sessions with psilocybin. J Psychopharmacol (Oxf). 2015;29:1182–90. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881115609019.
Stone CJ, Koo C-Y. Additive Splines in Statistics. American Statistical Association, Washington DC. 1985.
Harrell FE, Lee KL, Pollock BG. Regression models in clinical studies: determining relationships between predictors and response. J Natl Cancer Inst. 1988;80:1198–202. https://doi.org/10.1093/jnci/80.15.1198.
Harrell FE. Multivariable Modeling Strategies. In: Harrell Jr Frank E, editor. Regres. Model. Strateg. Appl. Linear Models Logist. Ordinal Regres. Surviv. Anal., Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2015. p. 63–102. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-19425-7_4.
Pustejovsky JE, Tipton E. Small-Sample Methods for Cluster-Robust Variance Estimation and Hypothesis Testing in Fixed Effects Models. J Bus Econ Stat. 2018;36:672–83. https://doi.org/10.1080/07350015.2016.1247004.
R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/. 2022.
Viechtbauer W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Softw. 2010;36(3):1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v036.i03.
Ritz C, Baty F, Streibig JC, Gerhard D. Dose-Response Analysis Using R. PLOS ONE. 2015;10:e0146021 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0146021.
Pustejovsky J. clubSandwich: Cluster-Robust (Sandwich) Variance Estimators with Small-Sample Corrections. R package version 0.5.8.9999. http://jepusto.github.io/clubSandwich/. 2023.
Liechti ME, Holze F. Dosing Psychedelics and MDMA. In: Barrett FS, Preller KH, editors. Disruptive Psychopharmacol. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2022. p. 3–21. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2021_270.
Carhart-Harris RL, Kaelen M, Bolstridge M, Williams TM, Williams LT, Underwood R, et al. The paradoxical psychological effects of lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD). Psychol Med. 2016;46:1379–90. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715002901.
Holze F, Vizeli P, Müller F, Ley L, Duerig R, Varghese N, et al. Distinct acute effects of LSD, MDMA, and D-amphetamine in healthy subjects. Neuropsychopharmacol Publ Am Coll Neuropsychopharmacol. 2020;45:462–71. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-019-0569-3.
Wießner I, Falchi M, Palhano-Fontes F, Feilding A, Ribeiro S, Tófoli LF. LSD, madness and healing: Mystical experiences as possible link between psychosis model and therapy model. Psychol Med. 2021;53:1–15. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721002531.
Holze F, Gasser P, Müller F, Dolder PC, Liechti ME. Lysergic Acid Diethylamide–Assisted Therapy in Patients With Anxiety With and Without a Life-Threatening Illness: A Randomized, Double-Blind, Placebo-Controlled Phase II Study. Biol Psychiatry. 2023;93:215–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2022.08.025.
Ko K, Knight G, Rucker JJ, Cleare AJ. Psychedelics, Mystical Experience, and Therapeutic Efficacy: A Systematic Review. Front Psychiatry. 2022;13:917199. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.917199.
Family N, Hendricks PS, Williams LT, Luke D, Krediet E, Maillet EL, et al. Safety, tolerability, pharmacokinetics, and subjective effects of 50, 75, and 100 µg LSD in healthy participants within a novel intervention paradigm: A proof-of-concept study. J Psychopharmacol (Oxf). 2022;36:321–36. https://doi.org/10.1177/02698811211069103.
Hartogsohn I. Constructing drug effects: A history of set and setting. Drug Sci Policy Law. 2017;3:2050324516683325. https://doi.org/10.1177/2050324516683325.
Russ SL, Carhart-Harris RL, Maruyama G, Elliott MS. States and traits related to the quality and consequences of psychedelic experiences. Psychol Conscious Theory Res Pr. 2019;6:1–21. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000169.
Haijen ECHM, Kaelen M, Roseman L, Timmermann C, Kettner H, Russ S, et al. Predicting Responses to Psychedelics: A Prospective Study. Front Pharm. 2018;9:897. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphar.2018.00897.
Savage C. The resolution and subsequent remobilization of resistance by LSD in psychotherapy. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1957;125:434–7. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005053-195707000-00015.
Lewis CR, Preller KH, Braden BB, Riecken C, Vollenweider FX. Rostral Anterior Cingulate Thickness Predicts the Emotional Psilocybin Experience. Biomedicines. 2020;8:34. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines8020034.
Vizeli P, Straumann I, Holze F, Schmid Y, Dolder PC, Liechti ME. Genetic influence of CYP2D6 on pharmacokinetics and acute subjective effects of LSD in a pooled analysis. Sci Rep. 2021;11:10851. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-90343-y.
Carhart-Harris RL, Muthukumaraswamy S, Roseman L, Kaelen M, Droog W, Murphy K, et al. Neural correlates of the LSD experience revealed by multimodal neuroimaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2016;113:4853–8. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1518377113.
Family N, Maillet EL, Williams LTJ, Krediet E, Carhart-Harris RL, Williams TM, et al. Safety, tolerability, pharmacokinetics, and pharmacodynamics of low dose lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) in healthy older volunteers. Psychopharmacol Berl. 2020;237:841–53. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-019-05417-7.
Becker AM, Klaiber A, Holze F, Istampoulouoglou I, Duthaler U, Varghese N, et al. Ketanserin Reverses the Acute Response to LSD in a Randomized, Double-Blind, Placebo-Controlled, Crossover Study in Healthy Participants. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2023;26:97–106. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijnp/pyac075.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
TTS initiated and conceptualized the work. JP conducted the literature search and data collection. TH performed the analyses with contributions of TTS. TH, JP, TM, and TTS jointly wrote, edited, and approved the final version of the paper.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Hirschfeld, T., Prugger, J., Majić, T. et al. Dose-response relationships of LSD-induced subjective experiences in humans. Neuropsychopharmacol. 48, 1602–1611 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-023-01588-2
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-023-01588-2
This article is cited by
-
Pharmacological and non-pharmacological predictors of the LSD experience in healthy participants
Translational Psychiatry (2024)