Introduction

Normal aging is associated with declines in specific cognitive domains. For example, executive function, memory, and processing speed decline with age, whereas verbal skills, semantic knowledge, affective function, perceptual priming, and implicit learning remain relatively intact [1, 2]. Poor executive function may contribute to other age-related cognitive weaknesses, including reductions in memory performance, and visuospatial skill [3]. These age-related declines in executive function are associated with poorer quality of life [4] and an increased risk of psychiatric disorders, including in major depressive disorder (MDD [5]).

Numerous magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies have sought to identify functional connectivity correlates of normal brain aging and age-related executive function decline. These functional MRI (fMRI) studies of normal aging have consistently reported lower within-network resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) in older age within executive/attentional control, default mode (DMN), and sensorimotor networks [6,7,8]—some of which are correlated with declines in executive function [9]. Additionally, impulsivity in healthy older adults arises from altered representations of future rewards in the prefrontal cortex [10], and deficient sensitivity to immediate rewards in the dorsal striatum [11]. However, few studies have identified predictors of increased risk for accelerated brain aging [12].

MDD may increase the risk for accelerated aging. Age-sensitive aspects of executive function, including cognitive flexibility, and attentional and motor impulsivity, are reduced in MDD [13, 14], and late-life MDD is associated with aberrant rsFC and deficits in executive function [15, 16]. Among cognitive symptoms, impulsivity may be particularly relevant to MDD outcomes. Greater impulsivity is positively correlated with depressive severity [17]. Impulsivity has shown to mediate the relationship between hopelessness and suicidality in young adult and late-life MDD [18, 19], and may contribute to the observation that aging-related executive function decline is a predictor of suicidal ideation in older adults [20]. Consequently, characterizing abnormalities of functional brain aging in MDD may provide a more complete understanding of the interplay of age-related declines in impulsivity and depressive symptomatology.

MDD is also associated with accelerated cellular and molecular aging [21, 22], and a recent structural MRI study reported accelerated age-related cortical thinning in depression [23]. Although multiple high-impact studies have characterized functional network alterations in late-life depression [15, 24], relatively few have examined connectivity changes across the lifespan. Thus, there is limited evidence that speaks directly to the question of whether or not age-related connectivity changes are accelerated in depression and, if so, how they relate to deficits in executive function and mood—a potentially critical question for understanding the biological basis of heterogeneity in depression across the lifespan.

Neuroimaging data are characterized by high interindividual variability in age-related changes [25]. This age-related variability correlates with disease risk, such as the progression of cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease [26], and some studies suggest that “brain age” may be a stronger risk predictor than chronological age [9, 27,28,29,30,31]. This hypothesis has motivated increased interest in predicting “brain age” based on neuroimaging variables and investigating whether deviations between brain age and chronological age—known as the brain-predicted age difference (brain-PAD)—are clinically relevant. To date, most brain-PAD studies have predicted age using structural MRI measures, including gray/white matter volume and cortical thickness. An older brain-PAD predicts who is more likely to progress from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s Disease [32] and is associated with increased mortality in healthy older adults [33]. Only two studies used rsFC to predict age, and both focused on healthy human subjects [34, 35]. One reported an association between brain-PAD and cognitive function in adults: brain-PADs were older in individuals with objective cognitive impairment [35]. Some [36, 37], but not all [38] brain-PAD studies using structural neuroimaging reported significantly older brain-PAD in MDD. It is unknown whether age-related changes in functional network organization are altered in depression.

Molecular markers of aging have been consistently associated with pharmacotherapy nonresponse [39,40,41,42]. Whether these age-related rsFC markers also predict poorer response to antidepressant treatments such as repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) [43] has not been determined. Older chronological age was modestly correlated with rTMS nonresponse in early rTMS trials [44,45,46,47,48], though a recent trial using higher stimulation intensities reported that older age was associated with better response [49]. Interindividual differences in both neurophysiologic and antidepressant response to rTMS may be driven by age-related changes in rsFC. Understanding the relationship between brain aging in MDD and rTMS response may be particularly valuable as it could facilitate tailoring rTMS stimulation parameters for MDD patients.

Here, we set out to investigate whether age-related rsFC changes are accelerated in depression and to evaluate how they influence cognition, mood, and rTMS response. First, to identify patterns of brain aging based on rsFC, we trained a support vector regression model to predict chronological age using rsFC in healthy human subjects. We hypothesized that the accuracy of this model for predicting age in healthy controls would be significantly better than chance. Our second aim was to test for evidence of accelerated brain aging in depression by evaluating the accuracy of the age-prediction model in an independent sample of MDD individuals. We hypothesized that the brain-PAD would be significantly higher in MDD patients than in healthy controls. Our final aim was to test whether accelerated brain aging in MDD is associated with (1) deficits in executive function, as measured by the severity of impulsivity in the MDD group and, (2) rTMS treatment response. We hypothesized that the brain-PAD in depressed patients would correlate with greater impulsivity, indicating greater deficits in executive function. Given previous work demonstrating the relationship between cellular aging and antidepressant nonresponse, we also hypothesized that accelerated brain aging (brain-PAD) would be a negative predictor of treatment response to active rTMS targeting the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC), but not to placebo, in a previously published three-arm placebo-controlled clinical trial [50].

Materials and methods

Subjects

We used three neuroimaging datasets from two scanners. The first dataset came from the open-access Enhanced Nathan Kline Institute Rockland County Sample [51]. Participants between 18 and 85 years old were selected for preprocessing from Releases 1–8 of the Cross-Sectional Lifespan Connectomics Study. The second dataset included healthy older adults who were also scanned at the Nathan Kline Institute, and recruited at the Weill Cornell Medicine Institute of Geriatric Psychiatry via community advertisements for non-psychiatric comparison subjects (ClinicalTrials.gov: NCT01728194). The final dataset included actively depressed subjects and HCs, originally recruited as part of a randomized controlled trial of rTMS targeting the DMPFC ([50] ClinicalTrials.gov:NCT02702154). MDD participants were referred to the MRI-Guided rTMS Clinic at Toronto Western Hospital and HC were recruited from the community. MDD participants were randomized to receive one of two active treatments (1 Hz DMPFC-rTMS or 20 Hz DMPFC-rTMS) or placebo rTMS, twice-daily, for three weeks. In total, the three studies yielded data from 848 subjects between 18 and 89 years old (736 HC and 112 MDD; Table 1 and Fig. S2). For additional details on inclusion and exclusion criteria, comorbidities, and medication status, see the Supplementary Methods and Results. All participants provided written informed consent, and studies were approved by their respective Research Ethics Board or Institutional Review Board.

Table 1 Gender (A) and Age (B) for all subjects.

rsfMRI data acquisition and preprocessing

Scans from 710/736 (96.47%) HC and 109/116 (93.97%) MDD participants were deemed usable by criteria defined a priori, including framewise displacement (FD), and whole-brain temporal signal-to-noise (tSNR) (Supplementary Results, Table S1 and Fig. S3). Participants’ FD was also used in all post hoc analyses to ensure that there was no association between imaging quality and predicted age associations by diagnosis or with clinical scales. We controlled for scanner-related differences in rsFC measures using ComBat Harmonization [52, 53], but to avoid biasing our held-out test sample, these controls were implemented iteratively within the model training loop and are described in the “Model training” subsection. For additional details on neuroimaging acquisition parameters and preprocessing procedures, see the Supplementary Methods.

Clinical assessments

At baseline, MDD participants completed the 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS [54]), and the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11 [55]). The BIS-11 is a 30-item self-reported questionnaire assessing attentional, motor, and non-planning impulsivity. Percent HDRS improvement from baseline to 1-month post-treatment was also collected.

Model training

All models were implemented using LIBSVM V3.23 [56] in Matlab V2019a (The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, Massachusetts, USA), using an ε-support vector regression with a radial basis function. To establish model performance and optimize three parameters (number of rsFC features, cost, and gamma parameters), 1000 iterations of the following were performed. First, whole-brain correlation matrices representing 33,411 rsFC features (using regions of interest visualized in Fig. S1) were partitioned into training (90%) and test sets (10%). Next, we conducted parametric ComBat Harmonization [52, 53] on the training set, with age as a covariate, to account for scanning acquisition differences. The resultant parametric adjustments derived from the training set were applied to the test set to account for scanner differences.

Next, we ranked rsFC features that were significantly and stably correlated with age. To generate these rankings, we performed bootstrapping with replacement (5000 iterations) on the training set, parametrically correlating age with all 33,411 rsFC features. Features were considered stable if they were correlated with age at a threshold of p < 0.0001 across >80% of the bootstrapping iterations. Stable features were ranked by the mean p value for their correlation with age across the 5000 bootstrapping iterations.

We then performed a support vector regression with a grid search to optimize the number of features (50–600 features), the cost parameter (10e−5 to 10e5), and gamma parameter (10e−5 to 10e−2). This resulted in 528 models per iteration; each model was applied to the held-out test set and the squared correlation coefficient was extracted for each combination of model parameters. The mean squared correlation coefficient across all iterations was used to determine the optimal model parameters.

In order to test whether the optimal combination of model parameters produced a significantly predictive model, we then repeated the aforementioned 1000 training and testing iterations, shuffling the ages of individuals at each iteration. If there were fewer than 600 stably predictive rsFC features during feature selection, rsFC features were ranked on the mean p values generated using all 5000 bootstraps irrespective of subsample stability. The null distribution generated by this analysis was used to evaluate the statistical significance of our model’s performance.

Performance in MDD

Once the optimal hyperparameters were identified, feature z-normalization, feature selection, and modeling were repeated using leave-one-out cross-validation to generate HC brain-PAD scores to compare against MDD brain-PAD. Brain-PAD was calculated as the predicted age minus chronological age. A final model was generated using the complete HC set; this model was applied to the MDD dataset, and we calculated the squared correlation coefficient and brain-PAD. To describe the relationship between age and rsFC features by diagnosis, we also repeated bootstrapping with replacement with the MDD group. Differences in rsFC-age relationships between MDD and controls were assessed by correlating brain-age associations by ROI between the MDD and controls; significantly negative correlations indicate a difference in the whole-brain rsFC-age associations for a given ROI between the MDD and HC groups. To test whether predicted brain age in MDD patients was larger than in HC, we used a univariate generalized linear model (GLM) with age, gender, and diagnosis as main effects to compare MDD brain-PAD against the brain-PAD from HC from the same site and scanner. We also tested whether brain-PAD of both HC and MDD differed by gender or correlated with neuroimaging quality measures (FD and whole-brain signal-to-noise).

Finally, we tested whether MDD brain-PAD was associated with baseline HDRS severity, executive function, and symptom improvement. Due to recent debate on the factor structure of the BIS-11 [57], we first performed an exploratory factor analysis on the BIS-11 items, using maximum likelihood extraction with a varimax rotation. Eigenvalues generated from a parallel analysis was used to identify the optimal number of factors. Next, we performed a GLM predicting brain-PAD, with gender as a factor, and age, BIS-11 factors, and HDRS baseline score as covariates. Main effects for all variables and two-way interactions only for age, gender, and HDRS score were included in the GLM. Second, to test whether brain-PAD was associated with individual differences in rTMS response, we conducted a GLM with percent HDRS improvement as the dependent variable and assessed whether there was a main effect of brain-PAD, age, gender, or treatment arm on percent HDRS improvement. We also modeled brain-PAD*Group (1 Hz versus 20 Hz; active versus placebo), Age* brain-PAD*Group and Gender* brain-PAD*Group interactions to determine whether there were any interactions between brain-PAD and antidepressant response by treatment arm. We accounted for multiple comparisons across the three GLMs using a Bonferroni correction.

Results

Shared age-related rsFC changes in HC and MDD

In order to characterize age-related rsFC changes and test whether similar changes occur in HC and MDD, we constructed whole-brain correlation matrices depicting the association between age and rsFC and identified both shared and divergent patterns of rsFC in the two samples. In HC, 962 out of 33,411 rsFC features were deemed stably predictive with age. rsFC decreased with chronological age in 954 of these rsFC features (mean correlation = −0.22 ± 0.04, range = −0.48 to −0.18), especially those involving within-network connectivity in sensorimotor, auditory, and cingulo-opercular task control networks and within a limbic network, which included the bilateral hippocampi, amygdalae, and subgenual cingulate (Fig. 1A). rsFC increased with chronological age in just 8 of 962 rsFC features (mean correlation = 0.20 ± 0.02, range = 0.18–0.22) involving connectivity predominately between just two networks: posterior DMN nodes and dorsal anterior cingulate node of the salience network (Figs. 1B and S4). MDD participants had similar trend of decreased age-related network associations in rsFC (Fig. 1C, D). However, 13 of 259 ROIs exhibited age-related rsFC changes that significantly differed between HC and MDD. These ROIs were predominantly within the DMN and limbic networks, and included the bilateral subgenual cingulate, and left hippocampus, lateral orbitofrontal, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Fig. 1E, Supplementary Results).

Fig. 1: Chronological age is associated with decreased within-network rsFC and increased between-network rsFC between the SN and DMN in both HC and MDD.
figure 1

The counts of positive (A) and negative (B) correlation coefficients that met stably predicted the relationship between age and rsFC in HC (p < 0.0001, 80% of bootstrapping iterations, 5000 iterations). Each cell represents the number of within- or between-network features that stably correlated with chronological age in HC. The counts of positive (C) and negative (D) correlation coefficients that met stably predicted the relationship between age and rsFC in MDD. As a priori-defined stability criteria failed to yield any rsFC features, we opted for a more liberal height and stability threshold for this sample (p < 0.0005, 66.7% of bootstrapping iterations, 5000 iterations). E Regions of interest where its whole-brain rsFC correlations with age significantly differed between HC and MDD (rho range = −0.29 to −0.15, FDR-p < 0.04). Briefly, most of the differences between HC and MDD in rsFC correlations with age involved default mode network and subgenual cingulate rsFC. For an additional description of these differences, see the Supplementary Results. AN: affective network, COTC: cingulo-opercular task control network, DAN: dorsal attention network, DMN: default mode network, FPN: frontoparietal network, SM: sensorimotor, SN: salience network, VAN: ventral attention network.

Robust rsFC-based prediction of chronological age in HC

Having identified correlations between rsFC and chronological age in healthy controls, we trained a support vector regression model to predict chronological age based on the most stable age-predictive rsFC features. To identify the optimal hyperparameters, we implemented a grid search to tune the number of rsFC features, and the cost and gamma parameters, optimizing for the explained variance between the predicted and chronological age in a held-out HC sample. The best performing parameters were 600 rsFC inputs, a cost parameter of 0.1, and a gamma parameter of 0.0001 (Fig. 2A). Performance of the model trained on the real data was superior to that of models trained on shuffled data (Fig. 2B; p < 0.001). The mean R2 for the same parameters in the shuffled data was 0.014 ± 0.019. Across all training iterations, 948 ± 62 rsFC features were stably correlated with chronological age (Fig. 2C). In contrast, there were 29 ± 24 stable features over all iterations using shuffled data.

Fig. 2: Tuning and training a support vector regression model predicting brain age based on rsFC in HC.
figure 2

A Grid search for optimizing SVR model parameters. Heatmaps of R2 representing the relationship between the chronological and predicted age in held-out data as a function of gamma and cost parameters, and number of rsFC features. The best combination of parameters is enlarged and presented in the red box (mean R2 = 0.552 ± 0.071). B Histogram visualizing the R2 across all 1000 training iterations for the best combination of parameters in real (red) and shuffled (gray) data. Real data R2 95% confidence interval [CI]: 0.403 0.682, range: 0.312–0.764; shuffled data R2 CI: 0.000–0.071, range: 1.129e − 9 to 0.136. C Histogram representing the number of rsFC features stably predictive of age across all 1000 training iterations. The vertical red line indicates the mean. D Scatterplot representing the correlation between chronological and predicted age in HC. The red lines indicate the regression line and the 95% confidence interval. E The top 10 most predictive rsFC features ranked by support vector regression weights. These features consisted of rsFC between: dorsal anterior cingulate cortex/DMPFC and motor regions; bilateral dorsal and ventral striatum; striatum and right inferior frontal gyrus; right IFG and orbitofrontal cortex; right parietal and visual regions; and between the bilateral medial temporal gyrus. dACC: dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, DMPFC: dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, IFG: inferior frontal gyrus, IPL: inferior parietal lobule, MTG: medial temporal gyrus, OFC: orbitofrontal cortex, TPJ: temporoparietal junction.

In preparation for comparing the brain-PAD of HC and MDD subjects, we performed two analyses to estimate brain-PAD (prediction error) in HC in held-out data using either leave-one-out cross-validation or ten-fold cross-validation. In leave-one-out cross-validation, the mean brain-PAD was 0.03 ± standard deviation 12.90 years (absolute brain-PAD = 10.50 ± 7.48 years; Fig. S5), and the correlation coefficient between the predicted and chronological age was 0.75 (Fig. 2D). A single ten-fold cross-validation using the entire sample or only the Rockland sample alone yielded similar results. For the entire dataset, the mean brain-PAD was 0.02 ± 13.25 years (absolute brain-PAD = 10.80 ± 7.66 years), and the correlation coefficient between the predicted and chronological age was 0.73. For the SVR using only the Rockland dataset, the brain-PAD was −0.15 ± 12.92 years (absolute brain-PAD = 10.58 ± 7.41, correlation coefficient = 0.73). Brain-PAD was not associated with demographic or fMRI quality control variables (Supplementary Results). To quantify the impact of scanner and acquisition parameter heterogeneity on SVR performance, we performed two supplemental analyses, training our model on two sites and testing on the third, with and without ComBat Harmonization for scanner-related differences. As expected, SVR performance was lower in both models (r = 0.55 and r = 0.28, respectively) but remained significantly predictive (p = 0.001–0.014) (Supplementary Results). The ten most predictive rsFC features were identified using the top 10 weights in the support vector regression model (Fig. 2E).

Functional brain aging is accelerated in MDD

In order to determine whether brain aging was accelerated in MDD patients relative to HC, a final model was trained using the entire HC dataset and optimal hyperparameters. The 600 rsFC features used in this model (Fig. 3A–C) consisted predominantly of decreased intra-network rsFC correlating with chronological age, especially within the sensorimotor, visual, auditory, salience, and cingulo-opercular task control networks.

Fig. 3: Input rsFC features for the final support vector regression model and brain-PAD associations in MDD.
figure 3

A Top 40 regions of interest predicting age, colored by brain network. The top ROIs were identified by ranking ROIs by the number of rsFC input features included in the model. B The number of rsFC features included in the model by within- or between-network rsFC of the top 40 ROIs. C The mean rsFC correlation with chronological age for within- and between-network rsFC of the top 40 ROIs. D Kernel density estimations of the brain-PAD in the MDD and HC sample from the same site and scanner. Brain-PAD in MDD was significantly higher than that of HC from the same site and scanner (Estimate = −3.66, SE = 1.49, p = 0.015, HC = 3.38 ± 14.06 years; MDD = 5.49 ± 12.65 years). E Greater financial impulsivity on the BIS-11 factor was associated with an older brain-PAD (Estimate = 3.08, SE = 1.00, p = 0.003); factor scores are normalized and account for other regressors included in the GLM. F Interaction of HDRS score and gender; males with high depressive symptomatology had older brain-PADs (Estimate = −2.27, SE = 0.69, p = 0.002); HDRS scores are normalized and account for other regressors included in the GLM. G HDRS percent improvement in the placebo-controlled TMS trial (PAD*Group (Placebo > Active) Estimate = 1.43, SE = 0.49, p = 0.004). Improvements only in the placebo arm were significantly associated with brain-PAD (Placebo estimate = 0.91, SE = 0.35, p = 0.015; 20 Hz Estimate = −0.40, SE = 0.71, p = 0.58; 1 Hz Estimate = −0.26, SE = 0.47, p = 0.59). Amyg: amygdala, AN: affective network, COTC: cingulo-opercular task control network, dACC: dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, DAN: dorsal attention network, DMN: default mode network, FPN: frontoparietal network, HC: healthy controls, HDRS: Hamilton Depression Rating Scale, Hipp hippocampus, IFG: inferior frontal gyrus, Ins insula, MDD: major depressive disorder, OFC: orbitofrontal cortex, rACC: rostral anterior cingulate cortex, PAD: brain-predicted age difference, SM: sensorimotor network, sgACC: subgenual anterior cingulate cortex, SMA supplementary motor area, SN: salience network, Thal thalamus, VAN: ventral attention network.

This model was then applied to a separate set of rsFC data from MDD patients, and we compared brain-PAD in the MDD sample with the leave-one-out cross validated HC brain-PAD. To avoid scanner-related confounds, we restricted this comparison to HC subjects scanned on an identical pulse sequence on the same scanner as our MDD sample. The MDD brain-PAD was 2.11 years higher than that of HC collected from the same site and scanner (HC = 3.38 ± 14.06 years; MDD = 5.49 ± 12.65 years). To compare the predicted age difference in MDD subjects relative to HC, we implemented a GLM testing for effects of diagnosis, age, and gender (Table 2A; full model R2 = 0.42). There were significant main effects for age and diagnosis (such that brain-PAD was significantly higher in MDD patients; Fig. 3D), but not for gender. Together, these results indicate functional brain aging is accelerated in depression compared to HCs, as indexed by increased brain-PAD in the MDD sample.

Table 2 Parameter estimates for all main effects and significant interactions.

Accelerated brain aging predicts financial impulsivity and depression severity in men

Previous studies indicate that accelerated aging is associated with increased clinical symptomatology and deficits in executive function. To this end, we tested whether MDD brain-PAD was associated with HDRS severity, and cognitive function as indexed by the BIS-11. To reduce the dimensionality of the BIS-11 data [57], we first performed an exploratory factor analysis on the BIS-11 items. The exploratory factor analysis revealed five BIS-11 factors (Table S2). These five factors were: (1) low motor and non-planning impulsivity, (2) high motor impulsivity, (3) attentional impulsivity and restlessness, (4) deficits in problem solving and puzzles, and (5) financial impulsivity. We found that brain-PAD was strongly associated with individual differences in the financial impulsivity factor, but not the other four factors (Fig. 3E and Table 2B, model R2 = 0.60). Higher financial impulsivity was associated with higher brain-PAD; this factor was associated with the BIS-11 items related to saving, spending, and having extraneous thoughts (Table S2). There was also a significant interaction between gender and HDRS score, such that depressed men with greater depression severity had an increased brain-PAD (Fig. 3F).

Previous studies indicate biological markers of aging such as telomere length are associated with pharmacotherapy nonresponse [39,40,41,42] and chronological age is modestly correlated with rTMS nonresponse [49, 58], leading us to hypothesize accelerated brain aging as indexed by the brain-PAD would be associated with rTMS nonresponse. Thus, we tested whether pretreatment brain-PAD predicted improvements to active or placebo DMPFC-rTMS in a recently published, sham-controlled trial (Table 2C; model R2 = 0.20). Unexpectedly, we found brain-PAD significantly predicted treatment response in the placebo group, but not in either active rTMS arm (Fig. 3F). The two three-way interactions were not significant, meaning neither residual age nor gender effects influenced the relationship between treatment arm, brain-PAD, and HDRS improvement. FD did not impact the results of any of the three GLMs (Supplementary Results).

Discussion

The principal finding is that brain aging was accelerated in MDD patients. We identify robust age-related correlations in specific brain networks in a large-scale multisite sample. Of note, there is considerable overlap between networks exhibiting age-related changes and those that are altered in depression, indicating that they could contribute significantly to heterogeneity in depression pathophysiology across the lifespan. On average, brain-PAD was modestly but significantly elevated in MDD patients compared to HC. Interestingly, in depressed men but not in depressed women, more severe depressive symptoms were associated with older brain-PADs. Furthermore, brain-PAD was positively associated with increased financial impulsivity. Unexpectedly, brain-PAD was not associated with rTMS response but was positively correlated with improvement in the placebo arm.

Brain-PAD was 2.11 years higher in MDD subjects compared to HC. Some [36, 37], but not all [38] brain-PAD studies using structural neuroimaging reported significantly older brain-PAD in MDD samples. Consistent with our findings, a large, recent study focused on structural measures [59] showed this effect is small, which might explain this inconsistency in previous studies. It is also worth noting that our model included rsFC from nodes commonly associated with depression pathophysiology—including the subgenual cingulate, hippocampus, insula, and amygdala [60]—and depression-related abnormalities in rsFC in these regions may contribute to a significantly older brain-PAD in MDD. Combining the two neuroimaging modalities is a way to bolster prediction accuracy [35]. A multimodal model is therefore a logical next step to gain a more comprehensive understanding of brain aging in MDD.

We found impulsivity specifically in economic decision-making and extraneous thoughts significantly correlated with brain-PAD in the MDD sample. It should be noted that the specificity of this finding was not predicted a priori, and our data do not rule out associations with other aspects of impulsivity. Although normal aging correlates with deficits in complex financial decision-making, simple financial decision-making, such as paying bills, remains intact [61]. Furthermore, other aspects of motor and non-planning impulsivity, such as risk-taking and sensation-seeking, are negatively correlated with age [62]. Previous studies have shown that decision-making changes during aging are associated with activity in the ventral striatum during monetary loss anticipation [63] and altered PCC rsFC [64]. Interestingly, impulsivity during economic decision-making is also associated with rsFC within the frontoparietal and cingulo-opercular networks that were heavily represented in our model [65], and in other RSFC models predicting interindividual differences in delay discounting [66].

It is also worth noting that employment stability or socioeconomic status may moderate the relationship between increased financial impulsivity and brain age in MDD. The MDD group in this study were treatment-resistant; this population is more likely to be unemployed and have a significantly lower socioeconomic status relative to nontreatment-resistant MDD or healthy individuals [67]. Socioeconomic status is significantly correlated with maladaptive economic decision-making [68] and worsening brain aging markers like network modularity [69] and hippocampal volume [70]. A logical next step would be to address the independent contributions of such social inequities in the complex interplay between the neurophysiological correlates of aging and psychiatric disorders.

We also found that accelerated brain aging correlated with more severe depressive symptoms in depressed men, but not in depressed women. Previously published studies report larger brain-PADs among nondepressed males [33] and robust sex differences in molecular aging factors, including genomic stability and epigenetic methylation [71], and rsFC [72]. However, gender did not otherwise account for differences in brain-PAD in MDD or HC in our analysis. Interestingly, this finding was not replicated in a recent study investigating structural brain aging in MDD; there was no association between brain age and clinical factors like severity or age of onset [59]. While we did not formally collect age of depression episode onset, future studies should aim to address the interplay between clinical or demographic factors in MDD and brain aging. Further studies will be needed to determine whether there is a causative link between depression severity and brain aging and whether these mechanisms are modulated by sex.

Contrary to our prediction, brain-PAD was not significantly correlated with the antidepressant response to active DMPFC-rTMS. rTMS was delivered at 120% of the resting motor threshold, possibly minimizing the negative impact of any age-related atrophy, increased scalp-to-cortex distance [73], or associated disruptions in rsFC. Furthermore, previously reported studies reported that cortico-striato-thalamocortical and frontolimbic circuitry are implicated in DMPFC-rTMS response [74,75,76]. These rsFC features were less predominant in our model predicting age, possibly rendering it a poor predictor of active DMPFC-rTMS response. Surprisingly, we found a strong positive correlation between brain-PAD and placebo rTMS response. Previous studies indicate either no correlation or a modest anticorrelation between placebo responsivity and chronological age [77]. However, increased RSFC between the DMN and SN has been observed as a predictor of antidepressant placebo response [78]. Interestingly, all positively correlated RSFC features with age involved the SN and/or DMN, five of which were inter-network RSFC between these two networks.

Several limitations should be noted. First, like several studies on this topic [36,37,38, 59], we tested for evidence of accelerated brain aging in MDD by training a model to predict healthy aging and applying it to MDD patients. This approach is advantageous because it provides a means of identifying rsFC patterns that are characteristic of healthy aging and then testing whether they are altered in MDD. As in prior studies [36,37,38, 59], we interpret this finding of increased brain-PAD in MDD as evidence of accelerated or potentially pathological brain aging, but other factors may also contribute to an increased prediction error in MDD. For example, to the extent that age-related changes in rsFC in HC are largely non-overlapping with age-related changes occurring in MDD subjects, this could also contribute to an increased prediction error. Arguing against this interpretation, the data in Fig. 1 indicate that age-related rsFC changes are similar in both HC and MDD subjects. It is also noteworthy that the absolute brain-PAD in HC was slightly higher than in previously reported studies predicting age using structural MRI [36, 59, 79,80,81,82,83]. This study was also not sufficiently powered to train separately for males and females; brain-PAD may differ between males and females in separately trained models [33]. However, there were no main effects of gender on brain-PAD, and gender was accounted for in each GLM. There were also significant site differences, both in terms of subjects’ age and rsFC quality. Ideally, all subjects should be acquired with the same scanner parameters at the same site to minimize fMRI-related confounds. To account for this potential issue, we corrected for site differences in the training and test sets of every model. Additionally, brain-PAD was not associated with rsFC data quality in either HC or MDD. We also compared brain-PAD in MDD and HC participants with neuroimaging data acquired from the same scanner to mitigate any effects related to age, inclusion criteria, scanner acquisition parameters, or data quality differences across scanners. Lastly, it is unclear whether increased brain-PAD is driving increased impulsivity and depressive symptom severity or vice versa. Further studies will be needed to elucidate the uni- and bi-directional relationships between age and behavior.

In conclusion, this study predicted brain age in HC and MDD patients using rsFC. Functional brain aging was accelerated in MDD patients compared to HC, and accelerated brain aging in MDD was associated with increased impulsivity. The study provides evidence for the notion that MDD is associated with accelerated brain aging, and that accelerated aging is associated with worsened impulsivity and depression severity. Establishing these biologically based relationships will be critical to more comprehensively understand the etiology and heterogeneity of MDD, with the hopes of identifying novel treatments to address the significant personal and economic burden of this disorder.

Funding and disclosure

KD is supported by a Canadian Institutes for Health Research Banting Postdoctoral Fellowship and has received a Canadian Institutes for Health Research Vanier Scholarship. LWV has received grants from the NIMH. JD has received research support from the Arrell Family Foundation, the Buchan Family Foundation, Brain Canada, the Canadian Biomarker Integration Network in Depression, the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR), the Klarman Family Foundation, NIH, the Ontario Brain Institute, the Toronto General and Western Hospital Foundation, and the Weston Family Foundation; he has received travel stipends from Lundbeck and ANT Neuro; he has served as an advisor for BrainCheck, Restorative Brain Clinics, NeuroStim Clinics, and TMS Neuro Solutions. FMG and CL are supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health, the National Institute on Drug Abuse, the Rita Allen Foundation, the Klingenstein-Simons Foundation, the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation, the Hope for Depression Research Foundation, and the Pritzker Neuropsychiatric Disorders Research Consortium. CL is listed as an inventor on Cornell University patent applications on neuroimaging biomarkers for depression that are pending or in preparation. The authors report no other financial relationships or conflicts.