Introduction

Anxiety disorders (ADs) are the most frequent group of mental disorders [1] and contribute significantly to the large burden of mental illness worldwide [2]. Cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) is the gold standard for treating ADs and other fear-related disorders like obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) [3]. While CBT is an effective treatment with few side-effects, not all patients profit from it [4]. A recent meta-analysis of placebo-controlled CBT trials [5] has revealed large effect sizes only for generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), OCD, and acute stress disorder (ASD). Effect sizes for PTSD, social anxiety disorder (SAD), and panic disorder (PD) were small to moderate.

Exposure therapy is widely regarded as the vital therapeutic component of CBT for ADs [6]. This assumption is underlined by the above-mentioned meta-analysis [5] showing that treatments which chiefly used exposure techniques have larger effect sizes than those utilizing both cognitive and behavioral techniques, and cognitive techniques alone. Augmentation of exposure therapy is thus an ideal starting point in the quest of improving treatments for anxiety and related disorders. It involves exposing patients under controlled conditions to situations that elicit pathological fear, thereby inducing fear extinction, a process well characterized and understood based on human and animal fear-conditioning research [7]. Impaired extinction learning has been observed in individuals with anxiety and related disorders [4, 8, 9] and the success of exposure therapy is predicted by extinction learning [10]. Decades of intense animal and human research have uncovered both associative and neurobiological mechanisms underlying extinction. This has also opened the door to translational research, which allowed the identification of agents like d-cycloserine [11] or cortisol [12,13,14] that may be utilized to enhance therapeutic success.

We propose that the peptide hormone insulin should be examined for its potential to augment exposure success. Insulin is widely known for its regulatory role in metabolism, but also has enhancing effects on memory and learning [15]. Insulin is produced by the pancreatic ß-cells and its main function is to control glucose metabolism in the periphery of the body. However, insulin receptors (IRs) are also widely distributed in the brain [16], with particularly high densities in the olfactory bulb, cerebral cortex, hypothalamus, and hippocampus [17]. While the peripheral IRs primarily act on glucose regulation, central IRs exert functions related to brain development, plasticity, and cognitive processes, in particular modulation of memory and attention processes [15, 18,19,20]. The experimental manipulation of central effects of insulin by intravenous application is limited by severe peripheral side-effects, i.e. hypoglycemia [21]. However, intranasal application can prevent such peripheral side-effects while providing a direct route to the central nervous system (CNS) [22]. In patients with Alzheimer’s disease, intranasal insulin can improve memory performance and prevent deterioration [23]. In healthy subjects, it increases performance in hippocampus-dependent tasks [20, 24,25,26]. Long-term administration of intranasal insulin also improved executive function in bipolar disorder patients [27]. While some studies using long-term insulin administration showed similar cognitive enhancement in both sexes [20], sex-dependent effects of insulin have been reported, with men being more sensitive to its anorexigenic and women to its acute cognitive enhancing properties [24, 28]. When used as an unconditioned stimulus (US), intranasal insulin was shown to produce a conditioned serum insulin response [29], indicating promising applications in learning processes.

Fear extinction is not a passive process, but the result of a newly formed inhibitory memory [30]. Attending the large evidence on the effects of intranasal insulin as a cognitive enhancer, we expect this hormone to also enhance fear extinction memory. To date, this has not been investigated. The aim of the present study was to investigate the effects of exogenous intranasal insulin on fear extinction processes in healthy subjects. We carried out a double-blind, placebo-controlled study to test the effect of an acute dose of intranasal insulin (160 IU) administered before fear extinction learning using a differential fear-conditioning paradigm. Furthermore, reinstatement of fear and re-extinction were tested 24 h later to examine the stability of possible insulin effects after return of fear (ROF). We hypothesized that intranasal insulin prior to extinction would facilitate extinction and diminish reinstatement while enhancing re-extinction. As a subsidiary aim, we investigated interactions of sex with insulin effects on fear extinction processes given the reported sex-specific effects of insulin on memory.

Methods and materials

Participants

Data were acquired from 131 healthy students at Saarland University. Exclusion criteria were: tinnitus; body-mass index (BMI) outside the normal range (men: 20–25 kg/m2; women: 19–24 kg/m2; German Nutrition Society); drug or medication intake within the last 6 months, except occasional use of painkillers and moderate caffeine/nicotine consumption; acute medical or psychiatric symptoms/complaints; excessive physical exercise. To control for hormonal effects of the menstrual cycle, only women taking oral contraceptives were included, with exception of contraceptives containing drosperinone due to its mineralocorticoid receptor antagonist effects [31]. Study procedures followed the Declaration of Helsinki, were approved by the local medical ethical committee (Ärztekammer des Saarlandes), and were registered in the German Clinical Trial Register (DRKS00010551). At application to partake in the experiment, a participant information sheet was given with details on the procedures applied (e.g. electroshock administration, insulin or placebo administration), as well as the general research question (effects of intranasal insulin in memory processes). Participants gave written informed consent and received moderate monetary incentive on completion of the study.

Two participants did not come to the first day of experiment, while two others discontinued participation. Due to malfunctions on day 1, four participants were excluded from all analyses (Supplementary Fig. S1). The final sample consisted of 123 participants (63 females) with a median age of 23 years (range 18–35).

Group assignment and pharmacological manipulation

In a double-blind design, participants were randomly assigned within sex to intranasal insulin or intranasal placebo, resulting in the following division: insulin group (n = 62, 31 females), placebo group (n = 61, 32 females). Participants received 160 units of intranasal insulin (Insulin Human Actrapid Penfill® 100I.E./ml; Novo Nordisk, Mainz, Germany), a quantity which has shown effects on cognitive function [32], or placebo (dilution buffer for insulin). Eight 0.1-ml puffs of substance were applied into each nostril via high-precision medical nose pump (Aero Pump, Hochheim, Germany). Substance was administered on day 2, 45 min before fear extinction to ensure central effects during the critical time of extinction learning [22, 33]. Upon arrival, before starting the extinction, and before departure, blood sugar levels were controlled via a blood glucose meter (Accu-Chek Aviva, Roche Diagnostics Deutschland, Mannheim, Germany). In the period between substance administration and beginning of the extinction phase, participants were allowed to read pre-selected magazines after being asked to leave their belongings, including mobile devices, in a separate room.

Stimuli and apparatus

Conditioned stimuli (CS) consisted of four validated face pictures (two female) from the Radboud Faces Database [34], showing neutral expressions (female: nr. 1 and 19, male: nr. 7 and 25). Pairs were chosen based on matching valence and arousal ratings obtained in a pre-study (M Arousal: nr. 1 = 29.96, nr. 19 = 30.41; nr. 7 = 31.93, nr. 25 = 28.43; M Valence: nr. 1 = 47.96, nr. 19 = 48.93; nr. 7 = 45.17, nr. 25 = 48.30). Given that social anxiety is the most common AD [35] and threatening social experiences can provoke lasting fear [36], we chose socially relevant CS. These stimuli have a higher comparability with common aversive experiences than abstract stimuli, and consequently lend a higher ecological validity to the present paradigm. In a pseudo-randomized fashion (balanced by sex and group), each participant watched either female or male faces. Each picture was shown for 8 s followed by a black screen with randomized intertrial-interval (ITI) of 10–15 s duration. An acoustic startle stimulus was presented on all CS-trials 7 s after picture onset, and during the ITI (noise alone, NA), 5 s after picture offset. NA trials were presented as often as the single CS. The acoustic startle stimulus was a white noise (105 dB, 50 ms, instantaneous rise-time) presented binaurally via 24-Bit sound card (Creative Sound Blaster Z, Creative Technology Ltd., Singapore) and audiometric headphones (Holmco PD-81, Holmberg GmbH & Co. KG, Germany). As US, a moderate 200 ms electroshock was applied to the lower left arm of the subject immediately at the offset of the CS+. Intensity was adjusted individually by each subject on day 1 (possible range: 1 mA to 10 mA) and kept constant in the following days. The presentation order was pseudo-randomized with the restriction that (a) no more than two consecutive presentations of the same stimulus-type would occur; (b) each half of the experiment would have a balanced number of each trial-type.

Procedure

The differential fear-conditioning procedure took place on three consecutive days: acquisition of fear was established on day 1; pharmacological manipulation and extinction of fear on day 2; reinstatement and re-extinction on day 3 (Fig. 1). Participants were asked to abstain from alcoholic/caffeinated beverages consumption and sport activities prior to the experiment. To ensure a similar glycemic state every day, they were instructed to have their last meal prior to 22.00 of the previous day. For a better applicability of the fasting period and to control for the natural circadian rhythms of metabolic hormones [37], testing took place between 8.00 and 12.00. A cover story was used to increase compliance: participants were informed that a saliva sample would be collected to control if the fasting period had been respected. A routine recall from waking until arrival was completed as an additional compliance control [38, 39]. Participants were prepared for recording of electromyographic (EMG) eye blink of the left orbicularis oculi muscle, skin conductance response (SCR), electrocardiogram (ECG), and for electroshock following published guidelines [40, 41]. Every experimental session started with a resting phase of 3 min (black screen with fixation cross) and a startle habituation (10 startle probes).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Diagram of the experimental design.

Day 1: acquisition

After the resting phase, participants were instructed to adjust the intensity of the US by gradually increasing the intensity of the electroshock up to being “highly unpleasant and demanding some effort to tolerate, while not being painful”. Instructions for acquisition indicated that one of two pictures would be sometimes followed by an electroshock. Acquisition consisted of 12 NA, 12 CS, and 12 CS+, with 75% reinforcement. Partial reinforcement allows for a slower extinction learning [42], where effects of a cognitive enhancer can be better studied.

Day 2: extinction

Forty-five minutes after pharmacological administration, participants started the experiment. Participants were instructed that an electroshock could or could not appear sometimes and that the same pictures from the previous day would be presented. Extinction consisted of 12 unpaired NA/CS/CS+ trials.

Day 3: reinstatement and re-extinction

Participants were instructed that an electroshock could or could not be administered during the experiment and that the faces from the previous days would be presented. During reinstatement, three unpredicted US were administered with randomized ISI of 15–20 s. Re-extinction followed consisting of six unpaired NA/CS/CS+ trials.

Physiological measures

Physiological data were recorded with ActiveTwo-Software (BioSemi, Amsterdam, Netherlands) at a sampling rate of 2048 Hz. Data were further analyzed with Autonomic Nervous System Laboratory (ANSLAB) version 2.6 [43] and manually inspected.

Eyeblink startle responses were measured from EMG activity of the orbicularis oculi muscle using Ag–AgCl active electrodes. Startle response amplitude was computed as the difference between peak—highest value of the startle response within 20–150 ms after acoustic stimulus onset—and baseline—mean EMG in the 50-ms window before acoustic stimulus onset. Artifacts were set to missing data, while trials with no visible startle response were scored as zero. Startle magnitudes were calculated including zero responses.

SCR was measured through two Nihon-Kohden electrodes filled with isotonic electrode gel attached to the proximal part of the palm of the subject's non-dominant hand. SCR to the CS was calculated by subtracting the average baseline (2 s before stimulus onset) from the maximum score after CS onset (0–7 s) [44,45,46,47].

For each participant, outliers (|Z| > 3) and missing data from startle (Placebo group: 1.4%, Insulin group: 1.1% of all data) and SCR (placebo group: 1.6%, insulin group: 1.5% of all data) were replaced by linear trend at point separately for experimental phase (acquisition, extinction, reinstatement, re-extinction) and CS-type [14, 48]. To minimize between-subject and day variability, both startle and SCR were T-scored.

Self-report and subjective measures

Before the first day of experiment, participants filled out the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II) [49] and the trait-form of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-T, [50]). The state-form of the STAI (STAI-S, [50]) was acquired on each day at the beginning and end of the session. US-expectancy ratings were collected before the beginning (pre), in the middle (mid), and at the end (post) of each conditioning phase with a continuous visual analog scale, ranging from very low (0) to very high expectancy (100), prompting participants to retrospectively rate how much they expected the CS to be followed by an electroshock. At the end of day 2, participants indicated which substance they believed was administered to them (“insulin”, “placebo”, “I do not know”).

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted with IBM SPSS (version 22), the level of significance set to (α) = 0.05. Due to experimental malfunctions on day 3, four participants had to be excluded from analyses regarding this day. Six participants were excluded from startle analysis due to complete absence of startle eye-blinks (non-responders) and three others due to missing values in >5% of their total startle data. Two participants were excluded from SCR analysis on day 2 and another one on day 3, due to technical problems.

To assess conditioning to the CS+ in the physiological data, a mixed-design-ANOVA with Group (insulin vs. placebo) as between-groups factor, and CS-type (CS+ vs. CS) and Time (Blocks 1–6, each with two trials of each CS-type) as within-participants factors was conducted.

Since we hypothesized that insulin effects might be modulated by sex, all analysis from day 2 on (after pharmacological manipulation) included Sex as a between factor. To assess discrimination between the CS+ and CS, difference-scores (CS+–CS; Diff X, with X specifying the dependent variable) were used for analysis of extinction, reinstatement, and re-extinction [51, 52]. Following data correction recommendations [53] and studies with similar designs [14, 54,55,56], blocks were averaged into early and late phases, each containing one half of the respective phase, to better represent learning effects. Extinction and re-extinction were tested with a mixed-design-ANOVA with Sex and Group as between factors and Time (early vs. late) as within-variable. Reinstatement was tested in a similar fashion with Time consisting of late extinction vs. early re-extinction.

For the US-expectancy ratings, similar analyses were conducted, with the within-factor Time (pre vs. mid vs. post) for acquisition, extinction, and re-extinction. For reinstatement analysis, post-extinction vs. pre- and post-reinstatement were usedFootnote 1.

Additionally, to check for contextual anxiety throughout the experiment [57,58,59], raw NA trials were analyzed in a mixed-design-ANOVA with Sex and Group as between factors, and Phase (acquisition, extinction, and re-extinction) and Time (early vs. late) as within-variables.

The Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied whenever sphericity adjustment was required (adjusted p-values are reported with uncorrected degrees-of-freedom and epsilon-values). Where not specified, means and standard error are reported. Follow-up analysis of three-way interactions were done with Bonferroni-adjusted pairwise comparisons for each Time point within each Sex group, comparing placebo and insulin.

Raw SCR (CS+, CS) and fear-potentiated startle (FPS) (CS+, CS, NA) through all trials and sessions are depicted separately by group (acquisition) or group and sex (extinction, reinstatement, re-extinction) in Supplementary Figs. S2S11.

Results

Demographic variables

There were no significant differences between groups regarding age, BMI, BDI, and STAI-T (Table 1).

Table 1 Demographic characterization of the insulin and placebo group.

Glucose check

No differences between groups was found regarding glucose levels throughout extinction (all Ps > 0.05) (Supplementary Fig. S12). Glucose slightly decreased from the beginning to the end of the experiment (Time: F2,232 = 7.73, P = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.64), with all subjects remaining in the euglycemic state at all time points (>70 mg/dL).

Subjective ratings and US intensity

Groups did not differ in their estimation of substance administered (X2(2, N = 123) = 0.41, P = 0.82; Supplementary Table S2), nor in the selected US level (Placebo: 4.03 ± 0.26, Insulin: 4.04 ± 0.27; t121 = 0.22, P = 0.83, d = 0.04). STAI-S on the 3 days did not show differences between groups (Ps > 0.05), but all participants reported higher state anxiety at the end of the experiment (Time effect on each day: Ps < 0.001).

Contextual anxiety throughout the experiment: NA startle

Insulin had no influence per se on background anxiety throughout the different phases of the experiment. In absence of any group-related effects, it was found that women showed in general higher context anxiety (Sex: F1,108 = 6.22, P = 0.014, ηp2 = 0.05) and that for all participants contextual anxiety was lower at the end of the experiment (Time: F1,108=56.26, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.34).

Acquisition

Skin conductance response

As expected, the CS+ (52.18 ± 0.23) elicited a higher SCR than the CS (47.82 ± 0.23) during acquisition (Time: F5,605 = 39.44, P < 0.001, ε = 0.70, ηp2 = 0.25; CS-type: F1,121 = 92.88, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.43; CS-type × Time: F5,605 = 3.58, P = 0.006, ε = 0.85, ηp2 = 0.03; no significant interactions with Group).

Fear-potentiated startle

As expected, the CS+ (52.93 ± 0.29) elicited a higher FPS than the CS (47.89 ± 0.26) during acquisition (Time: F5,560 = 56.38, P < 0.001, ε = 0.91, ηp2 = 0.34; CS-type: F1,112 = 154.34, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.58; CS-type × Time interaction: F5,560 = 3.24, P = 0.009, ε = 0.91, ηp2 = 0.03; no significant interactions with Group).

US-expectancy

Participants correctly identified the CS+ as predicting the shock (CS+: 69.77 ± 20.79; CS: 25.57 ± 23.81; Time: F2,242 = 8.49, P = 0.002, ε = 0.68, ηp2 = 0.07; CS-type: F1,121 = 534.47, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.82; CS-type × Time interaction: F2,242 = 241.22, P < 0.001, ε = 0.68, ηp2 = 0.95). CS+ was rated with a higher expectancy than the CS in the mid and post (Ps < 0.001), but not in the pre-ratings. No significant interactions with Group were found.

Extinction

Skin conductance response

A main effect of Time (F1,117 = 16.96, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.13), and an interaction of Time × Group × Sex was found for the DiffSCR (F1,117 = 7.93, P = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.07). Post-hoc tests showed that in the early extinction phase, DiffSCR was lower for women in the insulin group (−0.01 ± 5.30) than for women in the control group (3.78 ± 6.13; F1,117 = 7.52, P = 0.007, ηp2 = 0.06; Fig. 2). No differences between Group were present in late extinction.

Fig. 2: Differential skin conductance response during early and late extinction in the insulin and placebo group by sex.
figure 2

Significant pairwise comparisons for each time point within each sex, comparing placebo and insulin, are indicated in the graphic. Error bars indicate one standard error; **P < 0.01.

Fear-potentiated startle

The insulin group showed throughout the extinction phase a lower DiffStartle (1.35 ± 5.53) than the placebo group (3.02 ± 5.57) (Group: F1,110 = 4.18, P = 0.04, ηp2 = 0.04; Fig. 3).

Fig. 3: Differential fear-potentiated startle during extinction in the insulin and placebo group.
figure 3

Error bars indicate one standard error; *P < 0.05.

US expectancy

A decay in differential expectancy from pre, to mid and post-ratings (Ps < 0.001) was found (Time: F2,238 = 66.17, P < 0.001; ε = 0.81, ηp2 = 0.36), indicating attenuation of the fear association (Fig. 4). No main effects or interactions with Group were found.

Fig. 4: Differential US expectancy during extinction, reinstatement and re-extinction in the insulin and placebo group.
figure 4

Main effects of time are depicted collapsed across the two groups. Error bars indicate one standard error; ***P < 0.001.

Reinstatement

Skin conductance response

No effects were found for the DiffSCR in the reinstatement of fear (all Ps > 0.05), indicating that there was no reinstatement of SCR.

Fear-potentiated startle

Similarly to the SCR, no effects were found for the FPS (all Ps > 0.05).

US expectancy

DiffUS-expectancy had a significant Time effect (F2,236 = 15.59, P < 0.001, ε = 0.68, ηp2 = 0.12) during reinstatement. An increase in differential expectancy from post-extinction to pre-reinstatement (P < 0.001) was present, suggesting a spontaneous recovery of fear. The reinstatement stimuli had no effect on expectancy ratings, however, with no significant change in US-expectancy ratings from pre- to post-reinstatement (Fig. 4).

Re-extinction

Skin conductance response

In the re-extinction, women (−1.13 ± 7.08) showed lower DiffSCR than men (1.05 ± 8.05; Sex: F1,112 = 4.1, P < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.04). Furthermore, an interaction of Time × Group × Sex was found (F1,112 = 5.74, P = 0.02, ηp2 = 0.05). Post-hoc tests showed that in late re-extinction DiffSCR was lower for women in the insulin group (−3.42 ± 5.24) than for women in the control group (0.73 ± 6.20; F1,112 = 5.14, P = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.04; Fig. 5).

Fig. 5: Differential skin conductance response during early and late re-extinction in the insulin and placebo group divided by sex.
figure 5

Significant pairwise comparisons for each time point within each sex, comparing placebo and insulin, are indicated in the graphic. Error bars indicate one standard error; *P < 0.05.

Fear-potentiated startle

No effects were found for the FPS during re-extinction (all Ps > 0.05).

US expectancy

As expected, a decay in differential expectancy is seen from pre to mid and post-re-extinction (pre: 33.74 ± 30.64, mid: 21.35 ± 24.08, post: 14.61 ± 18.97; Time: F2,230 = 42.35, P < 0.001; ε = 0.71, ηp2 = 0.27; Fig. 4).

Discussion

The present study is the first investigating the hypothesis that intranasal insulin enhances fear extinction and providing first evidence for its confirmation. On day 1, acquisition of fear was established successfully without differences between the insulin and placebo group. Critically, during fear extinction on day 2, the insulin group showed a smaller differential startle response than the placebo group. Additionally, women in the insulin group showed an enhanced reduction of the fear-related SCR during early fear extinction on day 2 as well as in late re-extinction on day 3.

Since SCR is closely associated with declarative memory while startle represents a more primary fear reaction [48], these results might indicate that insulin exerts different effects at different levels of fear extinction learning. Established cognitive effects of insulin are mainly found on short-term declarative memory [32]. It is therefore not surprising that we found an effect of insulin already at the beginning of fear extinction for the SCR. Although men and women seem to benefit from the cognitive effects of insulin, there is some evidence that women might be more sensitive to the beneficial effects of central insulin on hippocampus-dependent memory functions [24]. In line with this, in the present study, although both men and women benefited from the insulin effects in extinction of FPS, only women showed increased extinction and better extinction recall of the SCR. This might indicate that indeed women are more sensitive to the cognitive effects of insulin. This is highly relevant, given that women show not only a higher prevalence of ADs, but also higher associated burden and disability [60]. Notwithstanding, since men and women present general differences in body mass, sex-dependent effects can also be due to different sensitivity to the administered dose. In order to clarify this question, dose-dependent effects need to be explored in future studies.

The enhancing effects of insulin on fear extinction in women could also be seen 24 h later, with a better fear extinction recall on the SCR. Similar to the extinction phase, a sex-specific insulin effect was observed, with women in the insulin group presenting lower differential SCR at the end of re-extinction and therefore better extinction recall. Fear extinction does not erase the original fear memory, but creates a new memory that will hinder fear to reoccur. This process is, however, frail and susceptible to reappearance of the original fear memory [61]. In clinical context, it is known that relapse can occur even after successful extinction [62]. Effects on the level of extinction recall are therefore of special clinical relevance, since more important than the fear extinction is how this new learning can hold up during time. The present results show that insulin might not only enhance learning of fear extinction but also its consolidation. Considering that intranasal insulin did not affect subjective anxiety ratings nor contextual anxiety as measured by the NA trials, the present data seem to indicate an effect of intranasal insulin on fear extinction learning and not on fear expression per se.

The present study applied a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled differential fear-conditioning paradigm, with careful maintenance of control variables. A comparable glycemic state at the beginning of the experiment was assured by food restriction, and control throughout the experiment showed that glucose levels remained in an euglycemic level after intranasal administration, as seen in other similar studies [24, 63,64,65]. The observed differences between insulin and placebo group can therefore be attributed to central nervous insulin effects and not to changes in peripheral glucose levels. Based on the current information, it can only be speculated which brain structures are involved in these effects. In this regard, it is important to note that the effects of intranasal insulin were observed for two physiological measures of fear learning and extinction: FPS and SCR. Long-standing evidence indicates that the two measures have different neural correlates and therefore express different processing mechanisms. It is thought that FPS reflects a more primal form of fear learning, involving neuronal structures like the amygdala, insular cortex, and thalamus [66]. SCR is on the other hand considered to express associative fear learning and anticipatory arousal [67], accompanied by activation of the hippocampus [68]. A concerted activity of the amygdala, hippocampus, and prefrontal cortex is central during fear extinction [69]. Furthermore, the insular cortex, a structure implied in fear-conditioning possibly via interoception and awareness processes [62], has been shown to be sensitive to intranasal insulin [33]. Neuroimaging studies should target those regions to investigate on what level intranasal insulin affects fear learning and extinction.

Insulin can cross the blood–brain barrier through active transport and thus directly affect the CNS [70]. The widespread expression of IR in the brain suggests effects on a broad range of brain structures, including the above-mentioned ones. The influence of central insulin on memory functions might be mediated by different pharmacological mechanisms [71]. Similar to insulin effects in the periphery, central insulin effects include an increased release of glucose from glycogen stores and its transportation across membranes, as well as an enhanced neural uptake of glucose or glucose-analog substances [72,73,74]. An especially sensitive structure to the insulin-dependent energy regulation is the hippocampus [74], and glucose has been shown to increase cognitive functions [75, 76] and, more recently, hippocampus-dependent acquisition of fear memory [77]. It is therefore possible that enhancing effects of intranasal insulin in memory processes are actually mediated by glucose uptake. On the other hand, central insulin exerts effects through additional pathways and especially its capacity to modulate glutaminergic and GABAergic signaling, and consequently excitatory synaptic transmission have been suggested to mediate memory effects [71].

No differences between groups on the US-expectancy ratings were found; however, dissociation between subjective ratings and physiological measures has been repeatedly reported [48, 78, 79] and is in line with the theory of multiple memory systems [80]. A limitation of the present study is that the reinstatement procedure was not successful, neither at a subjective nor at a physiological level. US-expectancy results showed, however, a spontaneous recovery of fear: the difference between the CS was already increased at the beginning of day 3, before the reinstatement procedure, compared to the end of the extinction on the previous day. It could be that the large time interval between extinction and test was enough to prompt ROF [81, 82], or that the context of the laboratory environment might have led to an immediate ROF [62, 83], which was not further exacerbated by the reinstatement procedure. It is important to note that the interval between fear acquisition and reinstatement was longer than what is commonly used in similar paradigms [59]. Although longer temporal intervals might be more ecologically valid, it is possible that the used US was not emotionally salient enough to produce reinstatement after such an interval. Moreover, given that the reinstatement procedure proceeded re-extinction, it is not possible to exclude a potential influence in extinction recall.

It is a further limitation that only women taking hormonal contraceptives were included in this study. Since sex hormones can affect not only insulin sensitivity [84] but also fear-conditioning processes [85], research should be extended to women not taking oral contraceptives.

As insulin exerts a long-term regulatory signal [86], many studies focused on effects of long-term intranasal insulin administration on cognitive enhancement [20, 25, 87]. It would therefore also be important to study the effects of prolonged insulin administration on fear extinction learning. Furthermore, research with cognitive enhancers like cortisol, has shown that such hormones can have enhancing or deteriorating cognitive effects dependent on time of administration and the investigated memory process [88]. The present study cannot disentangle the effects on extinction learning and consolidation as insulin was administered before extinction learning. Future studies should focus on these different processes.

With regard to improving the effectiveness of CBT for fear-related disorders, there is an increased need for substances improving extinction processes. We present first evidence that intranasal insulin might be a promising adjuvant to extinction-based therapies. Further research is necessary to elucidate the effects of insulin in fear learning and extinction, especially in sub-clinical and clinical samples. Furthermore, sex effects need to be taken into consideration.

Funding and disclosure

This work was financially supported by a Startup Funding for Research Projects from the Saarland University (Anschubfinanzierung von Forschungsprojekten im HH-Jahr 2016, 61-cl/Anschub 2016/bew-Ferreira). All authors declare no competing interests.