Introduction

The most frequently replicated pathophysiological correlate of psychotic symptoms (e.g., delusions and hallucinations, with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders being the most common psychotic disorder) is increased striatal dopamine [1, 2], with current evidence most consistent with increased presynaptic striatal dopamine. Psychotic disorders are usually preceded by a significant elevation of subthreshold psychotic symptoms (i.e., attenuated psychotic symptoms, APS [3]), during which individuals are considered at risk for psychosis. Using PET, psychosis risk is also associated with increased dopamine [4, 5]. However, PET imaging is probably not feasible for clinical use. Further, the neural consequences of increased striatal dopamine in psychosis risk remain unclear. Understanding these neural consequences could help better understand the nature of psychosis risk and better detect and prevent psychotic disorders. A long line of evidence from studies involving human and nonhuman subjects suggests that striatal dopamine is involved in feedback-related learning, with dopamine differentially released for unexpected reward versus punishment feedback [6,7,8,9]. The current study used fMRI to examine striatal activation in psychosis risk when receiving unexpected reward versus unexpected punishment feedback.

With regard to the neurobiology of the striatum, 90ā€“95% of striatal neurons are GABAergic medium spiny neurons (MSNs) [10,11,12]. In the striatum, these MSNs project to other basal ganglia regions via two main pathways: the Go (i.e., direct) pathway and the No Go (indirect) pathway [8, 13, 14]. The Go pathway involves primarily dopamine D1 receptors, and is activated by phasic bursts of dopamine, especially after better-than-expected reward feedback (i.e., positive prediction error) [11, 12, 15]. The No Go pathway involves primarily inhibitory dopamine D2 receptors, and is activated by phasic dips of dopamine (i.e., decreased activation of the inhibitory D2 receptor), especially after worse-than-expected punishment feedback (i.e., negative prediction error) [15]. Hence, higher striatal dopamine levels lead to increased activation in the Go pathway through D1 receptors, whereas higher dopamine leads to decreased activation (i.e., increased inhibition) of the indirect pathway through D2 receptors [6, 14].

Therefore, if psychosis risk is associated with increased striatal dopamine, then psychosis risk may be associated with increased striatal activation after unexpected reward than after unexpected punishment feedback (i.e., Goā€‰>ā€‰No Go pathway activation). Consistent with this, we previously used EEG to examine neural responses to unexpected reward versus unexpected punishment feedback [16]. We used a reversal-learning task (RLT) developed by Cools et al. [17] that involved periodic unexpected reward and unexpected punishment feedback. In particular, we examined the feedback-related negativity (FRN), and the subsequent P3a, components critical to feedback learning and reward processing [18], and found that psychosis risk was associated with an increase of both the FRN and P3a to unexpected reward versus unexpected punishment feedback. However, our previous EEG study could not examine neural responses directly in the striatum. Conversely, there may be an inverted-U effect, whereby with increased dopamine, there is initially greater Go pathway than No Go pathway activation; however, with even further increased dopamine, there may be greater ā€œnoiseā€ (i.e., imbuing too many signals with significance), thereby impairing reinforcement learning and appropriate behavioral responses (and perhaps leading to impaired striatal activation) [15, 19, 20]. Consistent with this, previous research has found reduced ventral striatum activation during reward anticipation associated with psychosis, including evidence for a specific association with negative symptoms of psychosis [21, 22]. In contrast, other research indicates that risk for positive symptoms of psychosis may be associated with heightened reward sensitivity [23], consistent with increased striatal dopamine. The current study is the first to examine whether attenuated psychotic symptoms in an antipsychotic-naive population are associated with neural evidence of increased sensitivity to unexpected reward, which would be expected with increased striatal dopamine.

Methods and materials

Participants

Research procedures were approved by the University of Missouriā€™s Institutional Review Board, and all participants provided informed consent. Participants were undergraduate students recruited as in previous research (see Supplement for details) [24]. Data from these participants performing another task (one that could not specifically distinguish between response to positive/reward versus negative/punishment feedback) have been previously reported [25].

The current study involved antipsychotic medication-naive people with increased risk for psychosis (i.e., psychosis risk group; nā€‰=ā€‰20 with useable imaging data; nā€‰=ā€‰21 with behavioral data) and a comparison group not at increased risk for psychosis (nā€‰=ā€‰20). Our previous EEG study comparing psychosis risk and a non-risk comparison group on the same RLT found a large neural effect size difference between groups, dā€‰=ā€‰1.14 [16]. In the current study, given our sample size, our power to detect that same effect size was 0.94. Further, the current study had power of 0.80 to detect a between-group difference effect sizeā€‰=ā€‰0.91.

The psychosis risk group (nā€‰=ā€‰21; TableĀ 1) both (a) scored 1.96 (note, Zā€‰=ā€‰1.96 is ~97.5% percentile of a normal distribution) sex-normed standard deviations above the mean on the Perceptual Aberration (PerAb) [26] or Magical Ideation (MagicId) [27] Wisconsin Positive Schizotypy Scales that assess self-reported psychotic-like beliefs and experiences (see Supplement for details), or a combined three sex-normed standard deviations above the mean on the PerAb and MagicId scales; and (b) had interview-rated current APS [i.e., a rating of at least moderate on the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes/Scale of Prodromal Symptoms (SIPS/SOPS), see Supplement for details] [28], with all participants exhibiting past week APS. Previous research has found that people with both elevated self-reported psychotic-like beliefs and experiences and interview-rated APS have a 14% rate of psychotic disorders at 10-year follow-up [29], with an estimated lifetime risk of psychotic disorders >20% [30], a lifetime risk of psychotic disorder as high as relatives with a first-degree family history of psychosis [31]. Hence, the psychosis risk group in the current study had current APS, was antipsychotic medication-naive and had a level of psychotic disorder risk comparable with a first-degree relative sample. One psychosis risk group participant was excluded from brain imaging analyses due to excessive artifacts (i.e., dental braces), leaving 20 psychosis risk group participants for brain imaging analyses (this subject is included in behavioral data analyses).

Table 1 Sample characteristics of participants

Participants in the comparison group (nā€‰=ā€‰20; TableĀ 1) scored between āˆ’0.6 and 0.6 sex-normed standard deviations around the mean on both the PerAb and MagicId scales. Hence, the comparison groupā€™s mean scores were close to population averages and were not an extreme low-scoring group. In addition, comparison participants had to be rated as not having APS on the SIPS/SOPS.

Reversal-learning task (RLT)

Participants completed a task previously found to activate the striatum, a RLT [16, 17, 32]. In general, on RLTs, participants first learn that one stimulus is rewarded and that a different second stimulus is punished. Subsequently, associations with reward and punishment are switched (i.e., an unexpected feedback trial) and participants have to learn that the first stimulus is now punished and that the second stimulus is now rewarded, with this pattern repeating through a number of reversals. There are two trial types that are most important for this task: (1) unexpected reward trials, an unexpected feedback trial where a stimulus that was previously associated with punishment is now associated with reward; and (2) unexpected punishment trials, an unexpected feedback trial where a stimulus previously associated with reward is now associated with punishment.

During the task, on each trial, two horizontally adjacent stimuli were presented simultaneously, one face and one nature scene (i.e., depicting a mountain range), one of which was highlighted with a yellow border. The participant predicted whether the highlighted stimulus would lead to a reward or a punishment, indicating their prediction by pressing one of two buttons on a response box (see Supplement for timing information). The participantā€™s response was followed by a feedback message. Reward consisted of a green smiley face and the words ā€˜You Win!ā€™ and ā€˜+100ā€™. Punishment consisted of a red sad face and the words ā€˜You Loseā€™ and ā€˜āˆ’100ā€™. Through trial and error, participants learned which image was associated with reward or with punishment (note that participants were not actually rewarded or punished, beyond receipt of this visual feedback). Reward and punishment associations would change after the participant got a random number (between 5 and 9) of consecutive trials correct (i.e., unexpected reward and unexpected punishment trials; [17]). Participants completed a total of 448 RLT trials.

Imaging data analysis

Imaging took place on a 3T Siemens Trio scanner with an eight-channel head coil. T1-weighted structural images were acquired for every participant using a high-resolution T1-weighted sagittal scan (MPRAGE sequence, 176 sagittal slices, 1-mm slice thickness, TRā€‰=ā€‰1920ā€‰ms, TEā€‰=ā€‰2.93, and flip angleā€‰=ā€‰9). Functional images were acquired for every participant using a T2*-weighted gradient-echo planar pulse sequence (TRā€‰=ā€‰2500, TEā€‰=ā€‰25, flip angleā€‰=ā€‰70, FOVā€‰=ā€‰256ā€‰Ć—ā€‰256ā€‰mm, and in-plane resolution 3.75ā€‰Ć—ā€‰3.75ā€‰Ć—ā€‰4.00ā€‰mm; see Supplement for additional details). Imaging data were analyzed using a mixed-effect single-subject general linear model using FSL (http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl) [33]. Motion correction was performed using Motion Correction in FMRIBā€™s Linear Image Registration Tool [34].

The analyses focused on striatum-related activation associated with feedback from joint Unexpected Reward and Unexpected Punishment, Unexpected Reward, Unexpected Punishment, Unexpected Reward versus Unexpected Punishment, and Unexpected Punishment versus Unexpected Reward [fMRI analyses only included incorrect unexpected trials; see Supplement for all non-striatum whole-brain analyses for each of these trial types; see Supplement for Expected Reward and Expected Punishment analyses (i.e., trials that did not involve a change in reward or punishment stimulus mappings, where, as expected, the groups did not differ in striatal activation)]. In whole-brain analyses, cluster thresholding was used in which contiguous clusters were first identified with a threshold of Zā€‰=ā€‰3.10 and then used Gaussian random field theory to estimate each clusterā€™s estimated significance level, with significant clusters having a pā€‰<ā€‰0.01 FWE-corrected for multiple comparisons [34, 35]. The Yeo atlas [36] was used to identify the networks associated with peak MNI coordinates for significant whole-brain activation.

In addition to whole-brain analyses, striatal region of interest (ROI) analyses were conducted. We first examined whether as expected, the striatum was activated both for Unexpected Reward and for Unexpected Punishment trials. We then computed Reward Sensitivity Difference Scores (Unexpected Reward feedback activation minus Unexpected Punishment feedback activation) [16], with higher Reward Sensitivity Difference Scores indicating greater activation for Unexpected Reward than for Unexpected Punishment. The whole striatum was defined as in previous psychosis risk striatum research [25] as the sum of the activation in associative, limbic, and sensorimotor striatal ROIs [37] (Fig.Ā 1a). These ROIs were created based on replicated large sample-resting-state analyses [37] (our associative striatum analyses aggregated the striatal subregions associated with the frontoparietal network (FPN), default mode (DMN), and ventral attention networks; collectively, these subregions have been previously labeled as the associative striatum [5]).

Fig. 1
figure 1

a The three striatum regions of interest (ROIs). b Mean associative striatum activation for Unexpected Reward trials, Unexpected Punishment trials, and Reward Sensitivity Difference Scores (Unexpected Reward minus Unexpected Punishment trials) for the psychosis risk and comparison groups. c Mean limbic striatum activation for Unexpected Reward trials, Unexpected Punishment trials, and Reward Sensitivity Difference Scores for the psychosis risk and comparison groups. d Mean sensorimotor striatum activation for Unexpected Reward trials, Unexpected Punishment trials, and Reward Sensitivity Difference Scores for the psychosis risk and comparison groups. e Mean overall striatum activation for Unexpected Reward trials, Unexpected Punishment trials, and Reward Sensitivity Difference Scores for the psychosis risk and comparison groups. For all charts, error bars reflect standard errors. *pā€‰<ā€‰0.05

For behavioral analyses, for unexpected trials, we examined accuracy and reaction time (RT) on the trial immediately following unexpected feedback (see Supplement for overall RT, overall accuracy, and expected trial analyses). We computed Reward Sensitivity Difference Scores (for accuracy: post unexpected reward trial minus post unexpected punishment trial; for RT: post unexpected punishment trial minus post-unexpected reward trial), with higher Reward Sensitivity Difference Scores indicating more accurate and faster learning after unexpected reward than after unexpected punishment [16]. We focused on comparing the groups on a composite Reversal Learning Score, which was the standardized sum of difference scores for RT and accuracy (see Supplement for correlations between psychosis risk symptoms [i.e., Wisconsin Positive Schizotypy Scales, SIPS subscales] and both striatal activation and behavioral performance).

Results

Imaging analyses

Whole-brain fMRI analyses

First, we examined striatal/basal ganglia regions (see Supplement for non-striatal/basal ganglia regions) that were significantly jointly activated for both Unexpected Reward and Unexpected Punishment (Unexpected Reward and Unexpected Punishment). As can be seen in TableĀ 2 and Fig.Ā 2a, the psychosis risk group showed significantly greater activation in the striatum/basal ganglia than the comparison participants, with a group difference peak in the right pallidum. For the control group, no striatal regions survived thresholding in whole-brain analyses. Next, we examined striatal regions significantly active on Unexpected Reward trials. As can be seen in TableĀ 2 and Fig.Ā 2b, the psychosis risk group showed significantly greater activation in the bilateral caudate and the bilateral globus pallidus (extending into the nucleus accumbens and putamen) striatal in regions functionally connected to the DMN, FPN, and Limbic networks. For Unexpected Punishment trials, as can be seen in TableĀ 2, while overall the putamen was significantly active, no striatal regions survived thresholding in whole-brain analyses for either the control group or the psychosis risk group.

Table 2 Whole-brain analyses for basal ganglia/striatal regions significantly active on the reversal-learning task and group differences
Fig. 2
figure 2

a Regions significantly more active for the psychosis risk group than the comparison group for Unexpected Reward and Unexpected Punishment. b Regions significantly more active for the psychosis risk group than the comparison group for Unexpected Reward. c Regions significantly more active for the psychosis risk group than the comparison group for Unexpected Rewardā€‰>ā€‰Unexpected Punishment

Next, we examined regions that were both (a) significantly active on Unexpected Reward trials and (b) significantly more active on Unexpected Reward than Unexpected Punishment trials (Unexpected Rewardā€‰>ā€‰Unexpected Punishment). As can be seen in TableĀ 2 and Fig.Ā 2c and consistent with the ROI analyses, the psychosis risk group showed significantly greater activation in the bilateral caudate and the bilateral globus pallidus. In the caudate, the peak was in the right associative striatum, more specifically in a subregion previously found to be most functionally connected to the cortical FPN [37] (for regions activated outside of the striatum, see the Supplement). Lastly, we examined regions that were both (a) significantly active on Unexpected Punishment trials and (b) significantly more active on Unexpected Punishment than Unexpected Reward trials (Unexpected Punishmentā€‰>ā€‰Unexpected Reward). For both the comparison group and psychosis risk groups, no striatal regions survived thresholding in whole-brain analyses.

Striatal ROIs

Next, we examined whether participants activated the striatum on Unexpected Reward and Unexpected Punishment feedback trials. Overall, participants did exhibit significant striatal activity on both Unexpected Reward, t(39)ā€‰=ā€‰6.50, pā€‰<ā€‰0.01, dā€‰=ā€‰1.03, 95% CI [0.64, 1.41], and Unexpected Punishment trials, t(39)ā€‰=ā€‰7.14, pā€‰<ā€‰0.01, dā€‰=ā€‰1.13, 95% CI [0.73, 1.52]. Hence, as expected, participants did strongly activate the striatum when they received unexpected feedback on the RLT. However, the groups did not significantly differ on either overall unexpected reward (overall: t(38)ā€‰=ā€‰0.91, pā€‰=ā€‰0.37, dā€‰=ā€‰0.29; associative: t(38)ā€‰=ā€‰1.55, pā€‰=ā€‰0.13, dā€‰=ā€‰0.49; limbic: t(38)ā€‰=ā€‰0.25, pā€‰=ā€‰0.80, dā€‰=ā€‰0.08; sensorimotor: t(38)ā€‰=ā€‰0.74, pā€‰=ā€‰0.47, dā€‰=ā€‰0.23; see TableĀ 3 and Fig.Ā 1) or unexpected punishment (overall: t(38)ā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’0.63, pā€‰=ā€‰0.53, dā€‰=ā€‰0.10; associative: t(38)ā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’0.09, pā€‰=ā€‰0.93, dā€‰=ā€‰0.03; limbic: t(38)ā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’0.91, pā€‰=ā€‰0.37, dā€‰=ā€‰0.29; sensorimotor: t(38)ā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’0.47, pā€‰=ā€‰0.64, dā€‰=ā€‰0.15), although if anything, numerically, the psychosis risk showed greater striatal activation for unexpected rewards (especially for the associative striatum) and reduced activation for unexpected punishments.

Table 3 Striatal region of interest (ROI) means (and standard deviations) for each group for the reversal-learning task (RLT)

Next, we examined whether the psychosis risk group exhibited altered neural activation, such that they tended to exhibit relatively increased activation for Unexpected Reward than for Unexpected Punishment trials (Unexpected Rewardā€‰>ā€‰Unexpected Punishment). As expected, using a 2 (trial type: Unexpected Reward, Unexpected Punishment)ā€‰Ć—ā€‰2 (group: psychosis risk, comparison) ANOVA, there was a significant trial type by group interaction of a large effect size, F(1,38)ā€‰=ā€‰7.22, pā€‰=ā€‰0.011, dā€‰=ā€‰0.85, 95% CI [0.19, 1.48]. As can be seen in TableĀ 3 and Fig.Ā 1, the difference in overall striatal activation between Unexpected Reward and Unexpected Punishment trials (i.e., the Reward Sensitivity Difference Score) was significantly larger in the psychosis risk group than in the comparison group, such that the psychosis risk group demonstrated greater Unexpected Rewardā€‰>ā€‰Unexpected Punishment striatal activation relative to the comparison group [note there was not a significant groupā€‰Ć—ā€‰striatal region (i.e., associative, limbic, and sensorimotor) interaction, pā€‰=ā€‰0.23; see Supplement]. Hence, the psychosis risk group showed greater neural sensitivity to unexpected reward versus unexpected punishment feedback than the comparison group.

Behavioral results

For the Reward Learning Score (i.e., standardized sum of Reward Sensitivity Difference Scores for RT and accuracy), there was not a significant difference between the groups, t(39)ā€‰=ā€‰1.34, pā€‰=ā€‰0.19, dā€‰=ā€‰0.41, 95% CI [āˆ’0.20, 1.04], although if anything, the psychosis risk group showed nonsignificantly greater sensitivity to Unexpected Reward (i.e., better learning for Unexpected Rewardā€‰>ā€‰Unexpected Punishment; see Supplemental TableĀ 1) than the comparison group (and note that the effect size in the current study, dā€‰=ā€‰0.41, is similar to the effect size in our previous study, dā€‰=ā€‰0.44) [16]; further, note that if we meta-analyze results for the two studies [38], there is a trend for a significant group difference in the Reward Learning Score, dā€‰=ā€‰0.43 [0.02, 0.87].

Discussion

Increased striatal dopamine in psychosis risk is expected to result in differential neural activation for unexpected reward compared with unexpected punishment feedback [6, 39]. For the first time, the current study used fMRI to examine striatal activation in an antipsychotic-naive psychosis risk group and found that psychosis risk was associated with relatively greater striatal/basal ganglia activation for unexpected reward than for unexpected punishment feedback relative to a non-risk comparison group in both ROI and whole-brain analyses (hence ROI analyses replicated patterns of striatal activation seen in whole-brain analyses). In whole-brain analyses, the psychosis risk group showed greater unexpected reward than unexpected punishment striatal activation in both the associative striatum portion of the right caudate and globus pallidus. There was also evidence that greater unexpected reward than unexpected punishment striatal activation was specifically associated with psychosis risk symptoms (see Supplement). The increased striatal activation to unexpected rewards is consistent with increased striatal dopamine [15] and greater Go than No Go pathway activation [6]. These findings contribute to the wealth of research, indicating the importance of the striatum in psychosis and indicate that altered striatal functioning can be detected in a group experiencing attenuated psychotic symptoms.

The current results for psychosis risk striatal activation for unexpected reward compared with unexpected punishment feedback are generally consistent with previous research. For example, our results are consistent with the previous EEG study which found that psychosis risk was associated with relatively greater sensitivity to unexpected reward than negative feedback on the RLT [16]. The current results are also consistent with imaging research examining psychosis risk. In particular, one previous study found that increased psychosis polygenetic risk scores are associated with increased striatal activation during reward anticipation [40]. Similarly, another study found that positive symptoms of psychosis (i.e., delusions and hallucinations) were positively correlated with striatal activation for reward anticipation in a psychosis risk group [41]. Recent psychosis risk research also indicates that increased striatal-to-midbrain connectivity positively correlates with severity of delusional ideation [42]. Thus, the current studyā€™s findings are generally consistent with a previous research finding that psychosis risk is associated with increased reward sensitivity [23].

However, the current results finding increased neural activation for unexpected reward than unexpected punishment feedback in psychosis risk appear generally different from what has been reported in psychotic disorders. In psychotic disorders, there is evidence of a blunted responses to reward feedback [22, 43] and blunted striatal activation to prediction errors [44, 45], including in unmedicated samples [46, 47]. However, the results for prediction errors in ultra-high-risk groups are mixed, with some evidence of increased striatal prediction error processing [42], although other research finds blunted striatal prediction error signals [48] or no significant evidence of striatal impairment [19], indicating that perhaps the degree of striatum-related reinforcement learning impairments depends on the degree of current dopaminergic dysfunction [19]. Furthermore, these previous studies did not specifically examine striatal activation for reward versus punishment on a RLT, as we did in the current study.

We have previously reported that psychosis riskĀ is associated with decreased striatal activation on a different feedback-driven learning task, the Probabilistic Category Learning Task (PCLT) [25]. The current study reported that psychosis riskĀ was associated with differential striatal activation for unexpected reward versus unexpected punishment feedback, whereas for the PCLT, psychosis risk is associated with decreased striatal activation. Together, these results appear very consistent with previous research on Parkinsonā€™s disease [49]. Among other neural changes [50], Parkinsonā€™s disease involves degeneration in the ventral substantia nigra, resulting in decreased dorsal striatum dopamine. In an unmedicated, low-dopamine state, people with Parkinsonā€™s disease exhibit better learning from negative than positive outcomes [8]. Nevertheless, despite exhibiting sensitivity to negative outcomes, when people with Parkinsonā€™s disease perform the PCLT, there is evidence of decreased striatal activation [51]. Therefore, the current psychosis risk RLT results (i.e., differential striatal activation for unexpected reward compared with unexpected punishment feedback) when combined with prior psychosis risk PCLT results (i.e., decreased striatal activation for feedback) appear generally consistent with Parkinsonā€™s disease research.

One strength of the current study is that the results cannot be attributed to antipsychotic medication effects, as all participants were antipsychotic-naive. The results of previous psychotic disorder studies may have potentially been conflated with medication use [45] (although as previously mentioned, some research indicates that unmedicated psychosis risk may be associated with attenuated striatum activation when learning from rewards [47]), as dopamine antagonist antipsychotic medication permanently alters striatal functioning. For example, medication usage is associated with both a significantly increased number of striatal dopamine receptors [52] and exerting a strong influence on caudate volume and therefore caudate neural activation [53, 54]. Thus, antipsychotic medication may potentially preclude finding evidence of a relationship between current psychosis and striatal functioning [55]. Another possibility is that while medication explains much of these differences, as previously mentioned, perhaps, these results can be partially explained by an inverted U of dopaminergic functioning, whereby further elevations in dopamine (as seen in full-blown psychotic disorders) might result in impaired reinforcement learning [15]. In addition, another difference between the current study and previous psychotic disorder studies is that previous psychotic disorder studies tend to use chronic samples and find that blunted reward responding is associated with negative symptomology (whereas the current study focused on recruiting a psychosis risk group elevated in current positive symptomology) [15].

Furthermore, finding different results in unmedicated versus medicated samples seems consistent with previous research on Parkinsonā€™s disease. As previously mentioned, people with Parkinsonā€™s disease when unmedicated (i.e., low dopamine state) exhibit increased sensitivity to negative versus positive feedback. However, when medicated (i.e., high dopamine state), this is reversed and now they exhibit increased sensitivity to positive versus negative feedback [8]. Similar to the results in Parkinsonā€™s disease, the results in psychosis risk and in people with psychotic disorders appear to vary depending on medication status. Unmedicated psychosis risk (high dopamine state) individuals generally exhibit increased neural activity for positive relative to negative feedback, but medicated people with psychotic disorders (with dopamine D2 receptor blockade) generally exhibit blunted neural activity for rewards.

Several limitations of the current study should be noted. First, a limitation of the study is the relatively small sample size (nā€‰=ā€‰20 per group), although, as previously mentioned, the imaging analyses examining striatal activation were well powered. Regardless, these results should be considered preliminary and replication is required [56]. Second, the current study did not assess several variables previously linked to striatal functioning, including psychiatric diagnoses and recreational drug use, including cannabis use [57]. Future research should examine how other dimensions of psychopathology (e.g., substance abuse and current functioning) relate to striatal functioning in psychosis risk. Third, although the current study examined striatal activation associated with psychosis risk, the results cannot directly speak to whether these activation differences are the result of increased striatal dopamine. Fourth, we failed to find significant behavioral evidence for greater sensitivity to reward than punishments. This lack of significant behavioral findings may indicate that in this sample, the increased striatal dopamine was sufficient to detect via fMRI but insufficient to result in significant behavioral effects. It is also possible that activation in regions associated with FPN, especially during unexpected punishment feedback, may have ā€œoverriddenā€ any effects that striatal dopamine had on performance. These speculative hypotheses should be further tested in future work. In addition, to further characterize Go pathway activation, future research should examine cortico-striatal functional connectivity to examine whether patterns of activation are consistent with activation of the Go pathway and inhibition of the No Go pathway in psychosis risk. Lastly, the current study used a complex RLT to examine responses to unexpected reward and punishments. However, this is arguably not ideal because reversal learning involves complex processes that go beyond mere responses to unexpected rewards and punishments. Other tasks (including simpler tasks examining rewards and punishments using actual monetary rewards) should examine whether psychosis risk is associated with increased striatal activation to unexpected rewards than punishments.

Our study demonstrates that psychosis risk is associated with relatively increased sensitivity to unexpected reward than to unexpected punishment feedback, as indicated by greater striatal activation. This increased sensitivity predates the emergence of a psychotic disorder, and therefore cannot be attributed to chronic illness or medication effects. These results are also consistent with increased striatal dopamine in psychosis risk. Overall, the current study suggests that altered striatal activation to unexpected reward compared with unexpected punishment outcomes may be a potential biomarker of psychosis risk.