Introduction

Bipolar disorder (BD) is a disabling psychiatric disorder that affects 1–2% of the population [1, 2]. BD is defined by the presence of recurrent depressive and manic/hypomanic episodes, as well as disruptions in rhythmic behaviors. For instance, depressive episodes are characterized by changes in sleep, daytime energy, motivation, and appetite; while mania is characterized by decreased need for sleep and increased nocturnal activity. Even during euthymic periods, BD patients commonly show late onset of morning activity, intrusions of sleep during the day, a preference for evening activity, changes in hormonal rhythms and abnormal temperature cycles indicative of circadian rhythm abnormalities and consistent with delayed phase and/or long period [3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10].

Circadian rhythms are maintained in a cell autonomous fashion by a molecular feedback loop comprised of ~20 “clock genes” [11]. The essential elements of the loop include the CLOCK/BMAL1 protein complex that drives expression of PER1/2/3 and CRY1/2. Through negative feedback of BMAL1/CLOCK, PER and CRY inhibit their own expression over ~24 h cycles leading to rhythms in gene expression. In animals, disruption of clock genes has been used to model mania [12] and depression [13], and clock gene mutants show abnormal responses to lithium in animal models of mania [14]. However, these animal models do not address some core features of BD including grandiosity, speech abnormalities and episodic alternations of mood. In humans, post mortem studies show the loss of circadian rhythms across several brain regions in patients with mood disorders [15]. Moreover, variation in clock genes has been linked to morning/evening preference (“chronotype”) both in the general population [16] and in circadian rhythm disorders [17, 18]. Since the circadian clock is cell autonomous, it can be studied in peripheral cell cultures from humans using bioluminescent reporters. Using this approach, we have shown previously that compared to controls, circadian period is typically longer in cells from BD patients, consistent with the behavioral data observed in human subjects indicating a predisposition for evening activity in BD [19].

Mood stabilizer medications positively impact the course of BD. However, many BD patients fail to respond adequately to pharmacotherapy [20]. Lithium is the best studied, and arguably most effective mood stabilizer, with efficacy in depression, mania and maintenance. However, only about 30% of patients with BD respond fully to lithium [21]. Like the risk for BD which is largely genetic [22], lithium response is heritable [23], and influenced by genetic variation [24]. Recent genome-wide association studies have identified loci that predict lithium response [25], but the biological processes governed by these genes remain uncharacterized.

Previous preclinical studies have focused on lithium’s ability to inhibit inositol monophosphates (IMP) [26] and glycogen-synthase kinase 3 (GSK3) [27], molecules with possible links to circadian rhythms. While the role of IMP in circadian timing is relatively unexplored, our recent work indicates inositol metabolism affects rhythms [28]. GSK3 is known to regulate the stability and turnover of clock proteins, and GSK3 inhibition has effects on circadian rhythm amplitude and period [29,30,31,32]. These changes in period may facilitate phase shifting, improve entrainment and have important implications for lithium’s effects on mood. In the dopamine transporter (DAT) knockdown model of mania, mice have a longer circadian period, and the period was shortened by the mood stabilizer valproic acid [33]. Similar period shortening effects of valproic acid were observed in gene expression rhythms in cells from BD patients [33], an effect on rhythms similar to selective GSK3 inhibitors [32]. In humans, clinical interventions including partial sleep deprivation and phototherapy both advance circadian phase and facilitate the therapeutic effects of lithium [34]. These data together indicate that period shortening and/or phase advancing may have beneficial effects on mood, possibly by correcting mismatches between the environmental light/dark cycle and endogenous rhythms that occur in the context of long period and/or phase delays.

Based on the observations that circadian rhythm abnormalities are central to BD, that lithium response is genetically encoded, and that lithium affects the circadian clock, we hypothesized lithium responsive BD patients (Li-R) would have identifiable differences in circadian rhythms compared to lithium non-responders (Li-NR). Moreover, we expected that we could use identifiable circadian biomarkers to better predict therapeutic outcomes after lithium pharmacotherapy.

Methods

Lithium Clinical Trial

Clinical response to lithium was determined prospectively in between January 2011 and January 2016 through the PGBD multi-center treatment trial [35]. Additional detail is summarized in Supplementary Methods.

Determination of clinical response to lithium

Subjects with BD type I were transitioned to lithium monotherapy over 12-weeks. Subjects able to stabilize on lithium monotherapy were classified as lithium responders (Li-R) and entered maintenance treatment. Subjects who were unable to stabilize were classified as lithium non-responders (Li-NR) and discontinued from the study. Subjects who left the study for intolerable side effects or reasons unrelated to their clinical response to lithium were excluded from further analysis. After these exclusions, 193 subjects were considered (N = 135 Li-R/N = 58 Li-NR). Subjects in maintenance were evaluated in person every two months, until either the occurrence of a depressive/manic episode (defined by DSM-IV criteria), or the end of the 2-year follow-up period.

Chronotype analysis

At baseline, subjects completed the Basic Language Morningness scale (BALM), a validated, 13-item self-reported measure of chronotype [36, 37]. Higher BALM values correspond to a greater level of morningness.

Analysis of depressive and manic symptoms, and suicide history

Subjects completed the 16 item Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology Self-Report (QIDS-SR16) [38], the Clinician Administered Rating Scale for Mania (CARS-M) [39], and a detailed self-report of past suicidal behaviors. Scoring was conducted in accordance with the respective protocol for each scale. To minimize survival bias, only baseline mood measures were considered.

Cell culture

Fifty-nine study participants with informative clinical outcomes (N = 44 Li-R/15 Li-NR) donated skin biopsies for fibroblast cell lines (Supplementary Methods). To facilitate studies of molecular mechanisms, we also used a mouse NIH3T3 fibroblast line stably transfected with the Per2-luc reporter. The NIH3T3 model recapitulates key features of lithium’s effects on rhythms [28, 40, 41] and allowed us to conduct drug and transfection studies that would not be practical in primary human fibroblasts. As a fibroblast line, we reasoned that the results are likely comparable across cell types.

Measurement of cellular circadian rhythms

Chronotype is determined in part by circadian period and phase, either of which either individually or in combination affect the onset of activity and entrainment to a 24 h light/dark cycle. Period change is a pharmacological effect of lithium, and we have shown previously that cells from BD patients show irregular amplitude responses to lithium [19]. Therefore, we examined three circadian rhythm parameters (amplitude, period, phase) in fibroblasts under both lithium-treated and untreated conditions using fibroblasts donated by Li-R and Li-NR subjects. We hypothesized that short circadian period would predict clinical stability, and that Li-R cells, especially those with a longer period, would exhibit greater shortening of circadian period by lithium compared to Li-NR cells. To determine if lithium may reverse the long period abnormality associated with Li-NR, we examined the degree to which period changed in Li-R and Li-NR cells after treatment with lithium. Cellular rhythms were measured in parallel under lithium (1 mM) treated and untreated (baseline) conditions [19]. Data were fit to a damped sine wave and rhythm parameters were calculated using commercial software (Lumicycle Analysis). Time of peak signal (first peak after synchronization) was used as a phase marker.

Drugs

Because lithium non-selectively inhibits inositol monophosphate (IMP), we tested whether inhibition of IMP affects circadian rhythms. Cells originating from the same culture were treated simultaneously in parallel with active drug or vehicle control. Lithium chloride was purchased from Sigma. 2-Aminoethoxydiphenylborane (APB), and [1-(4-Hydroxyphenoxy) ethylidene] bisphosphonic acid (L-690330) were purchased from Tocris Biosciences. Concentrated drug solutions were added to the growth media and diluted to the desired concentration. Drugs were dissolved in water or DMSO.

Gene knockdown

Knockdown of Itpr3 was performed using siRNA in NIH3T3 cells stably expressing Per2-luc under hygromycin selection [28]. See Supplementary Methods for details. Commercially available siRNAs (Dharmacon) were used according the manufacturer’s protocol. Bmal1 (also called Arntl, M-040483-01-0005) was targeted as a positive control, and siRNA that does not target any known transcript served as negative control (D-001206-14-05).

Measurement of cellular IP3

NIH3T3 cells were treated with lithium 10 mM or vehicle for 24-h IP3 was measured using ELISA following the manufacturer’s protocol (Life Span BioSciences).

Genotyping

Selective inhibition of GSK3 shortens circadian period [32], whereas lithium, a non-selective GSK3 inhibitor has been shown to paradoxically lengthen period in fibroblasts in a concentration-dependent manner [19]. Therefore, individual differences in the response of cellular period length to lithium may be relevant, possibly reflecting the varying balance of lithium’s pharmacological effects on GSK3 vs. competing pathways. Therefore, we tested whether genetic variation in the IP3 system and GSK3B explains differences in period among Li-R/Li-NR. BD subjects were genotyped at ~420 K single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNP) on PsychChip by TGEN (Phoenix, AZ). Data were deposited on the Genetic Cluster Computer (http://www.geneticcluster.org) for quality control, processing and analysis, using the PGC pipeline (https://github.com/nievergeltlab). Imputation was based on 1000 Genomes phase 3 (1KGP phase 3) using the first five ancestry informative principle components as covariates [42].

Multifactorial model of lithium response

We developed post hoc four criteria by which to score donors on cellular circadian factors associated with lithium response: (1) Period < 25.8 h (shorter than median). (2) Period > 25.8 h and corrected by lithium to <25.8 h. (3) Period > 25.8 h and Tm < 0.79 (top quartile phase advanced). (4) Rare allele ITPR3 genotype. The first three criteria were considered protective factors, while the fourth was considered a risk factor for non-response. One point was assigned for each protective factor, and one point subtracted for each risk factor (range of scores −1 to 3). Mean scores were compared across the two groups using a two-tailed t-test. Specificity and sensitivity for Li-NR were calculated based on the absence of any protective feature (score < 1).

Statistical analyses

Chronotype analysis was conducted in SPSS (version 20) using ANCOVA, corrected for age, race, and sex. Cellular analyses were conducted to examine three related hypotheses evaluating how circadian rhythms associate with treatment response: baseline period, baseline phase, and pharmacological effects of the drug on period/phase. Given our modest sample size, and that the dependent variables were not independent, a stringent correction for multiple comparisons was not employed. The cellular rhythm analyses were completed using GraphPad Prism (version 5.0) using a two-tailed t-test or one-way analysis of variance, and univariate regression analyses of phase and period. An F-test was incorporated into all analyses to ensure the assumption of equal sample variance was satisfied. Group differences in linear regressions were assessed for statistical significance using Fisher’s r to z transformation. Genotypic analysis of period was conducted using linear regression under a dominant model. All analyses used α < 0.05.

Human subjects research oversight

The research protocol was reviewed and approved by the local IRBs pertaining to each participating site to ensure compliance with all pertinent regulations regarding the ethical conduct of human subjects research. All subjects provided informed consent to participate.

Results

Clinical trial subjects

193 lithium-treated subjects were considered for chronotype analysis (Table S1). The majority (N = 135) advanced to the maintenance treatment, while 58 failed to achieve maintenance (Li-NR). Of the 135 Li-R subjects, 104 ultimately had informative clinical outcomes after 2 years, while the remainder left the study before the 2-year endpoint for reasons unrelated to mood stability (Figure S1).

Chronotype is associated with lithium response

Chronotype in people with BD is a stable trait (Figure S2). Past research indicates that low morningness is associated with depression [16], and worse treatment outcomes in major depression [43]. Therefore, we examined chronotype to determine if it was a predictor of lithium response in BD. Using the categorical measure of lithium response, Li-R subjects were significantly higher in morningness compared to Li-NR (mean BALM = 36.1 vs. 32.5, p < 0.02, Fig. 1a). We then analyzed separately three specific sub-groups: subjects who failed to stabilize initially, relapsed during maintenance, and achieved long-term stability. The overall group difference in morningness was significant (p < 0.01). Post hoc tests revealed that there was no difference in chronotype between those who failed to stabilize and those who relapsed (mean BALM 32.5 vs. 34.4), but that among those who remained stable for 2 years, morningness was significantly higher compared to those who failed to stabilize (Fig. 1b, mean BALM 37.1 vs. 32.5, p < 0.05).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Chronotype is associated with clinical response to lithium. a Li-R subjects (entered the maintenance phase, N = 135) were significantly higher in trait morningness at baseline compared to Li-NR (those who failed to stabilize, N = 57). b Those who failed to stabilize mood (N = 58) and those who suffered mood relapse during maintenance (N = 41) were similarly low in morningness. Morningness was highest in those who remained stable for 2-years (N = 63). Morningness is negatively correlated with c baseline depression (for absent/mild/moderate/severe/extreme N = 69/52/31/44/14) d baseline symptoms of mania (for absent/mild/moderate/severe N = 148/26/9/6). e Morningness is negatively correlated with past suicide attempts (N = 135 no attempt / N = 54 past attempt). Numbers vary slightly across analyses due to missing data. Box plots indicate minimum, maximum and mean values. Error bars indicate SEM. * Indicates p < 0.05

In order to understand the particular factors driving the association between chronotype and lithium response, we analyzed specific symptoms of BD. We found that more severely depressed subjects were lower in morningness (Fig. 1c). The most strongly associated depressive features were two sleep items: excessive sleepiness and delayed sleep onset (Table S2). Other functions that typically vary rhythmically over the day like energy, concentration, and psychomotor retardation showed similarly strong negative associations. However, additional depressive symptoms that are not commonly considered rhythmic, including feelings of sadness, and low self-esteem were also negatively associated with morningness.

Similarly, patients with more manic symptoms showed significantly reduced morningness scores (Fig. 1d), and several individual manic symptoms on the CARS-M scale, showed significant negative association with BALM, indicating that decreased need for sleep, disorganization and disorientation were all greater in subjects with low morningness (Table S3).

Finally, we examined suicide attempt (SA) history and chronotype. Subjects who endorsed one or more previous SA were significantly lower in morningness compared to subjects who had not attempted suicide (Mean BALM: SA = 32.9 vs. no SA = 35.9, p < 0.05, Fig. 1e).

Cellular rhythms

There were no significant demographic differences between Li-R and Li-NR cell line donors (Table S4). As expected, the duration of stability on lithium (Mean ± SEM) was significantly different (p < 0.001) with 568.9 ± 39 days for Li-R (N = 44) vs. 97 ± 9 days for Li-NR (N = 15). BALM scores among the BD cell line donors reflected the same overall trends observed in the larger clinical cohort, with higher morningness scores in the Li-R group (Mean BALM Li-R 37.3 vs. Li-NR 31.9, p = 0.05). BALM score was nominally correlated with cellular phase in the expected direction (r = −0.18), but this phase advance was not statistically significant. BALM score was not significantly correlated with any other cellular rhythm parameter.

Amplitude

There was no significant difference in baseline amplitude in cells from Li-R and Li-NR, and no effect on amplitude from lithium, indicating that the drug treatment did not increase amplitude in the cells obtained from either BD group (Fig. 2a). This finding independently replicates our previous work showing that unlike cells from controls in which lithium typically increases amplitude by 30–40%, lithium does not increase amplitude in fibroblasts from BD patients [19].

Fig. 2
figure 2

Circadian rhythm parameters in cells from Li-R and Li-NR donors. a There was no difference in amplitude in cells donated by Li-R (N = 44) and Li-NR (N = 15), either at baseline (untreated) or after cells were treated with lithium 1 mM. b Period length was more likely to be short in cells from Li-R donors compared to cells from Li-NR. Mean period Li-R 25.5 ± 0.14 vs. Li-NR 26.0 ± 0.17 h, two-tailed T test indicates p = 0.08. c The proportion of Li-R and Li-NR differs significantly in donors with short and long circadian period [Χ2 = 4.06(1), OR = 3.6, * indicates p < 0.05]. Short/Long corresponds to below/above the median period of 25.8 h. d Representative rhythm traces of cells from Li-R (blue) and Li-NR (red) subjects. Error bars represent SEM

Period

There was a difference in cellular period between Li-R and Li-NR donors that trended towards significance (mean period: Li-R 25.5 ± 0.14 vs. Li-NR 26.0 ± 0.17 h p = 0.08, Fig. 2b). Further inspection revealed that the cellular periods were not uniformly distributed between groups. Using a median split, a majority (25/29 or 86.2%) of short period (<25.8 h) samples came from Li-R donors, while the majority of Li-NR cells (11/15 or 73%) had long periods (Fig. 2c). These results indicate that the odds of non-response to lithium are significantly higher in cell donors with long period (OR = 3.6, p < 0.05). However, there was overlap between Li-R and Li-NR. The results indicate that short period may be predictive of response, and that while long period may be a relative risk factor, alone it is insufficient to explain non-response. Therefore, additional mitigating factors may influence responsiveness in subjects with long period.

Phase

There were no baseline differences in phase in cells from Li-R and Li-NR donors, and exposing the cells to lithium had no significant effect on phase. However, the relationship between period and phase differed between cells from Li-R and Li-NR. In cells from Li-R there was an inverse linear relationship between period and phase. Li-R cells with long period were typically phase advanced. In Li-NR cells this relationship was not statistically significant. Unlike Li-R cells, Li-NR cells with a long period were no more likely to be phase advanced than cells with a short period. The difference in the variance explained by these relationships was significantly different (z = 2.01, p < 0.05) between Li-R and Li-NR (Fig. 3a, b).

Fig. 3
figure 3

The phase-period relationship differs in cells from Li-NR compared to Li-R. a In Li-R cells (N = 44), samples with the longest period are phase advanced relative to those with shorter periods. b In Li-NR cells (N = 15), there exists no significant relationship between period and phase. c Representative rhythm traces showing the relative phase advance of Li-R cells vs Li-NR cells with similarly long periods. Arrows indicate phase marker (Tmax)

Pharmacological effects of lithium

In Li-R cells there was a positive correlation (r = 0.27, p = 0.06) between period length and the degree to which lithium shortened period (Fig. 4a). This relationship was attenuated in the Li-NR cells (r = 0.15, p = 0.58, Fig. 4b). To investigate further, we used a median split to sub-divide Li-R/Li-NR into long and short period sub groups. Compared to Li-R cells with a short period, Li-R cells with long period shortened period to a greater degree (p < 0.05, Figs. 4c, d). In Li-NR cells, lithium had no effect on period length in either group. Therefore, in the subset of Li-R cells with a longer circadian period, lithium treatment favors period shortening.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Lithium shortens circadian period preferentially in cells from Li-R donors. a In Li-R subjects the period shortening effect of lithium inversely correlates with baseline period length. Li-R subjects with long (>25.8 h) periods show the greatest shortening effect of lithium. b In Li-NR subjects the relationship between baseline period length and the effect of lithium on period is attenuated and no longer significant. c) Li-R cells with long period (N = 18) show significant period shortening after lithium compared to Li-R cells with short period (N = 25). Lithium had no effect on period length in Li-NR with long (N = 11) or short (N = 4) period. * Indicates p < 0.05. d Representative rhythm traces of a Li-R cell with long period after treatment with vehicle or lithium 1 mM

Period change by lithium is affected by IP3

The selective IMP1/2 inhibitor L-690330 caused a concentration-dependent lengthening in circadian period in NIH3T3 cells (Fig. 5a, b). IMP inhibition is predicted to increase the intracellular levels of 1,4,5-trisphosphate (IP3) by reducing its turnover. Supporting this hypothesis, we found that 24-h treatment of NIH3T3 cells with lithium 10 mM (a concentration that lengthens period) revealed a trend towards increased IP3 levels as expected (Fig. 5c). Accordingly, we hypothesized that intracellular IP3 receptors (IP3R) may explain some of the period lengthening effect of lithium. In line with this expectation, the IP3R antagonist APB caused a concentration-dependent period shortening and attenuated the period lengthening effect of lithium (Fig. 5d). Furthermore, genetic knockdown of Itp3r, the predominant IP3 receptor gene in NIH3T3 fibroblasts also blocked the period lengthening effect of lithium (Fig. 5e, f). Using publicly available data (https://data.broadinstitute.org/mpg/ricopili), we determined that a common variant in ITPR3 (rs11758031) showed a modest nominal association with BD (p < 6 × 10−4) in a previous GWAS of BD [44], while neither ITPR1 or ITPR2 showed similar trends. To test the role of ITPR3 in fibroblasts from BD patients, we examined whether this variant is associated with period. We found that when treated with lithium, cells with ITPR3 minor A alleles showed significantly longer period compared to those with two common alleles (AA/AG: 26.04 ± 0.26 vs. GG: 25.44 ± 0.13, Fig. 5g). Unlike at higher concentrations of lithium, the −50T/C variant in GSK3B (rs334558) was not associated with period change after lithium 1 mM (Fig. 5h).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Inhibition of IMP causes period lengthening. Despite its action as an inhibitor of GSK3, lithium lengthens circadian period at high concentrations, perhaps indicating circadian effects of IMP inhibition. a Selective inhibition of IMP with L-690330 causes period lengthening (N = 6–9/group). b Representative trace of the period lengthening effect of L-690330. c Treatment of mouse fibroblast cell lines with lithium for 24 h caused a nominal increase in IP3 that trended towards statistical significance (N = 7-8/group). d Antagonism of the IP3 receptor shortens period and reverses the period lengthening effects of lithium (N = 3/group). e Knockdown of the IP3 receptor gene Itpr3 in NIH3T3 cells (N = 8–14/group) blocks the period lengthening effect of lithium (10 mM). f Representative traces of Itpr3 knockdown experiment. Itpr3 knockdown attenuates the period lengthening effect of lithium. The loss of rhythm from Bmal1 knockdown confirms efficient siRNA transfection. g In cells from BD patients, ITPR3 genotype at the variant rs11758031 predicts circadian period after treatment of the cells with lithium (N = 48 GG and 14 GA/AA, p < 0.01), whereas h GSK3B genotype at rs334558 (−50T/C) does not (N = 23 TT, 38 CT/CC, p = 0.65). Box plots indicate minimum, maximum and mean values. Error bars represent SEM. * Indicates p < 0.05 vs control. ** Indicates p < 0.05 vs lithium treated

Modelling lithium response using circadian factors

To explore the overall relationship of circadian variables to lithium response, we developed a model to predict clinical lithium response that could assess four variables in cells (baseline period, period after lithium, phase, and ITPR3 genotype). Of 40 Li-R cell line donors, 33 (83%) had a score of 1.0 or more, indicating a preponderance of protective factors. Among Li-R only one cell line (3% of Li-R) had more than one protective factor, indicating minimal overlap among factors. The mean (±SEM) composite score among Li-R was 0.85 ± 0.15. Among 15 Li-NR cell line donors, 12 (80%) scored ≤0, indicating a preponderance of risk factors and the mean score was 0.07 ± 0.07 (Figure S3). Accordingly, our four-factor model had a specificity of 83% and sensitivity of 80% for identifying Li-NR.

Discussion

We have shown that chronotype and cellular circadian rhythms predict lithium response in BD. Higher morningness was associated with fewer depressive and manic symptoms, fewer SAs and longer duration of mood stability. In cells, short circadian period predicted lithium response in the donor. In cells with a long period, pharmacologically induced period shortening by lithium or phase advance was also associated with favorable clinical response. Finally, we identified IMP inhibition as a possible mechanism by which lithium affects circadian period. Taken together, our data support the hypothesis that features of circadian period and phase predicted to advance rhythms may positively contribute to lithium responsiveness. The results suggest a model by which lithium response could be predicted using circadian factors. It remains an open question whether in the course of lithium treatment circadian factors impact directly upon pathophysiological mechanisms causally related to BD, or non-specifically modify symptom burden.

Most previous studies of lithium response utilized retrospective clinical assessment and subjects treated with multiple psychotropic medications. A strength of our study is that our relatively large cohort of BD subjects was evaluated prospectively on a standardized lithium monotherapy protocol and that cells were available from these well-characterized donors. Our study also has limitations. We did not have cell lines from every clinical trial subject and accordingly, the sample available for cellular analyses, while comparatively large for this kind of study, is incomplete and had relatively few Li-NR lines. In addition, our cellular model of the 24-h circadian cycle has caveats. Estimates of circadian parameters calculated in vitro likely differ from those obtained in vivo due to artifacts associated with the cell culture environment and the use of peripheral cells. Moreover, with particular respect to phase, the reference point in vitro is the synchronizing biochemical stimuli associated with medium change, whereas in vivo, phase reference point is determined by light exposure. While our chronotype findings in BD patients largely support our cell-based conclusions, underlying mechanistic differences in phase setting could limit their overall generalizability in vivo. Finally, without an independent cohort, we were unable to validate our predictive model in an independent data set.

We reported previously that compared to controls, cells from BD patients had longer circadian periods and were more likely to show a period lengthening effect from lithium at a therapeutically relevant concentration, especially in cells from BD patients with a history of SA [19]. These previous studies were conducted in cells collected from hospitalized patients with complex medication regimens. Therefore, it was previously impossible to determine the relationship between circadian rhythms and lithium response. The present study extends this previous work, and demonstrates that period modulation by lithium may have relevance not only as a discriminator of BD from control cells, but also for predicting therapeutic outcomes to lithium monotherapy. Our previous work also found that lithium amplifies circadian rhythms in control cells, but not in BD cells [19, 40, 41]. In this study, we found that the attenuation of this circadian amplitude phenotype in BD was present regardless of Li-R/Li-NR status. Therefore, it is a phenotypic marker of BD that may point to molecular mechanisms underlying the illness, but is not of any apparent prognostic value for predicting lithium response.

Mood implications for circadian misalignment

Circadian rhythms entrain to environmental cues, most notably light. The intrinsic human circadian oscillator runs with a period of 24.2 h [45]. Therefore, there is typically a slight mismatch between the period of the endogenous circadian rhythm and the 24 h light/dark cycle. Phase advancing in response to light allows the circadian clock to overcome this mismatch and entrain to a 24-h cycle. However, with large mismatches between the light/dark cycle and the endogenous rhythm, entrainment is incomplete and results in syndromes such as advanced sleep phase [17] and delayed sleep phase [18] disorders. With extreme mismatches, the failure to entrain is compete and the endogenous rhythm “free runs” independently of the light/dark cycle. Healthy subjects maintained in a laboratory under free running conditions on a 28 h light/dark cycle demonstrate signs of severe physiological stress, including impairments in autonomic function, cortisol, sleep, cognition, and mood [46, 47]. Therefore, misalignment of the circadian clock causes physiological stress and negatively impacts mood. While subjects living in typical environmental conditions are unlikely to encounter such large clock/light mismatches, BD subjects do have a longer period [19], and frequently show behavior patterns that undermine efficient circadian entrainment, such as less daytime activity and limited exposure to light. In this way, BD subjects may experience mild clock/light mismatches over extended periods of time [5]. Therefore, we propose that the combination of longer period and compromised circadian entrainment causes some BD subjects to experience physiological distress, altered sleep and/or disturbed affect regulation. In this context, the degree to which lithium facilitates or opposes entrainment may affect the clinical course of lithium treatment.

Inositol monophosphatase and circadian rhythms

Lithium differs from selective GSK3 inhibitors with respect to its circadian effects, causing period lengthening rather than shortening. IMP inhibition by lithium and subsequent accumulation of intracellular IP3 may underlie this effect, and variation in ITPR3 may affect the degree to which lithium lengthens period. Therefore, the degree to which mood stability is maintained in BD may relate to genetic variation in the IP3 pathway. In particular, lithium response may be enhanced in individuals where the balance of lithium’s effects is shifted toward GSK3 inhibition (resulting in short period) and away from IMP inhibition (resulting in long period). The rs11758031 A/G intronic nucleotide variant may be an example of a genetic factor that influences the circadian effects of lithium. The SNP has been shown to be functional, affecting the expression of ITPR3 and IP6K3, an adjacent gene involved in signal transduction (https://www.gtexportal.org/home). In a recent GWAS of sleep phenotypes in >1 million subjects, ITPR3 was implicated in insomnia both by gene-based analysis and exon sequencing [48]. In contrast, the most recent GWAS for BD show little evidence of association with ITPR3 [49]. Therefore, it may be that lithium’s effects on IP3 relate most directly to circadian factors and sleep, and perhaps less so to BD susceptibility factors. We have previously shown that IPMK, a gene involved in both GSK3 and inositol signaling may also influence the period lengthening effect of lithium [28]. For these reasons, further work on the inositol pathway as it relates to lithium is warranted.

The role of the clock in mood stability

Presently, we cannot determine whether the circadian influence on lithium response is essential to promoting mood stability. By period lengthening, lithium could cause circadian misalignment, leading to a state of social, environmental and/or physiological distress, leaving only subjects who remain entrained to benefit from lithium’s therapeutic benefits. In these ways, the circadian influence on lithium response may be mediated by a lack of circadian “side effects” and be permissive rather than causal. Alternatively, the clock genes may be causally involved, preferentially activating neuroprotective processes in Li-R BD patients. Clock proteins like PER2, BMAL1 and REV-ERBα regulate neurogenesis [50, 51], and neuroprotective molecules involved in lithium’s therapeutic mechanism such as BDNF are rhythmically expressed [52,53,54,55]. Therefore, the clock may alter lithium’s pharmacological actions directly through neurogenesis and/or neuroprotection, suggesting a direct role for clock genes in modifying lithium response.

Conclusions

Circadian period and phase predict lithium response. Future experiments are needed to determine whether circadian clocks are essential for lithium response and if facilitation of entrainment is the ultimate circadian mechanism.