Abstract
Biased emotion processing has been suggested to underlie the etiology and maintenance of depression. Neuroimaging studies have shown mood-congruent alterations in amygdala activity in patients with acute depression, even during early, automatic stages of emotion processing. However, due to a lack of prospective studies over periods longer than 8 weeks, it is unclear whether these neurofunctional abnormalities represent a persistent correlate of depression even in remission. In this prospective case-control study, we aimed to examine brain functional correlates of automatic emotion processing in the long-term course of depression. In a naturalistic design, n = 57 patients with acute major depressive disorder (MDD) and n = 37 healthy controls (HC) were assessed with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) at baseline and after 2 years. Patients were divided into two subgroups according to their course of illness during the study period (n = 37 relapse, n = 20 no-relapse). During fMRI, participants underwent an affective priming task that assessed emotion processing of subliminally presented sad and happy compared to neutral face stimuli. A group × time × condition (3 × 2 × 2) ANOVA was performed for the amygdala as region-of-interest (ROI). At baseline, there was a significant group × condition interaction, resulting from amygdala hyperactivity to sad primes in patients with MDD compared to HC, whereas no difference between groups emerged for happy primes. In both patient subgroups, amygdala hyperactivity to sad primes persisted after 2 years, regardless of relapse or remission at follow-up. The results suggest that amygdala hyperactivity during automatic processing of negative stimuli persists during remission and represents a trait rather than a state marker of depression. Enduring neurofunctional abnormalities may reflect a consequence of or a vulnerability to depression.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Studying the course of depression and its etiologic and maintaining factors has the potential to reduce its high burden of disease [1]. Cognitive models of depression suggest that onset and maintenance of depression are associated with biases in emotion processing operating throughout all stages of attention, memory, and evaluation, favoring the processing of negative emotional stimuli (cognitive vulnerability; [2]). Furthermore, according to Teasdale’s differential activation hypothesis, depressed mood in turn activates negative thought patterns (cognitive reactivity), leading to a negative reciprocal build-up process that creates a vulnerability to persistent depression and depressive relapse [3].
Accordingly, behavioral studies demonstrated a negative bias in attention, interpretation, and memory in depressed patients [4,5,6] as well as in individuals at risk of developing depression [7, 8]. These cognitive biases were found to operate even in early, automatic stages of processing, as demonstrated using subliminally presented stimuli [9]. In addition, a negative bias in emotion processing has been associated with future depressive relapse [10,11,12]. However, most of the behavioral studies have examined the controlled, later stages of emotion processing, which may be influenced by cognitive control and emotion regulation processes.
Recently, cross-sectional neuroimaging studies provided evidence that emotion processing in depressed patients is biased already in the early, automatic stages, pointing to brain functional alterations mainly in the amygdala, a key region of the emotion processing and salience network [13, 14]: Consistently, depressed patients show amygdala hyperactivity to masked, subliminally presented negative stimuli and a hypoactivity to positive stimuli compared to healthy subjects [15,16,17]. Furthermore, higher automatic amygdala reactivity to negative primes has been associated with a negative judgmental bias to the neutral target faces in a subliminal affective priming task [18].
A key question in understanding vulnerability to depression is whether this abnormality in automatic negative processing is an enduring neurofunctional “trait” of depression or whether it is rather associated with the depressive state itself. Long-term neuroimaging studies have only examined changes in controlled stages of emotion processing, and suggest a normalization of activity in limbic regions in case of remission [19,20,21]. Regarding automatic emotion processing, there are no prospective neuroimaging studies examining the course of depression over periods longer than 8 weeks. Short-term studies indicate an initial normalization of amygdala dysfunction during subliminal emotion processing after psychopharmacological treatment and electroconvulsive therapy [17, 22,23,24], suggesting that also the early, automatic bias is a transient phenomenon that can be reversed by therapy. On the other hand, risk factors such as genetic susceptibility [25] or early maltreatment experiences [26] have both been associated with automatic processing biases in amygdala activation in healthy subjects, rather pointing to a trait-like vulnerability factor. Thus, it is unclear whether neurofunctional alterations during automatic emotion processing represent a stable correlate of depression in the long term.
Therefore, the main aim of our study was to investigate the neural correlates of automatic emotion processing of the course of depression over 2 years within a naturalistic study design. Due to its special role in early, automatic emotion processing and based on previous findings, a particular focus was placed on the amygdala. We used a well-established subliminal priming paradigm comprising emotional faces, which was designed to target emotion processing at its early (automatic) stage and to reliably evoke reactions in the amygdala [16, 26, 27]. The objectives of our study were as follows: (a) to replicate the mood-congruent bias on brain functional level during automatic emotion processing in patients with depression; (b) to examine changes in brain function during automatic emotion processing in dependence of the course of depression, more precisely, to investigate whether alterations in brain function normalize upon remission; and (c) to clarify if brain function during automatic emotion processing at baseline is predictive for the subsequent course of depression. Finally, we investigated the potential influence of treatments (medication intake and psychotherapy) during the study period on brain function.
Method
Participants and study design
Participants were examined from May 2010 to June 2015 (baseline) and reassessed after ~2 years from September 2012 to August 2017 (follow-up). The sample is part of an ongoing longitudinal study (Münster Neuroimaging Cohort).
At baseline, all patients suffered from an acute moderate or severe depressive episode and were under inpatient treatment at the Department of Psychiatry at the University Hospital of Münster or at the Psychiatric Hospital of the Landschaftsverband Westfalen-Lippe in Münster. The healthy control (HC) group was recruited through newspaper advertisements and public notices. Exclusion criteria were chronical medical illnesses, neurologic abnormalities, intake of benzodiazepine at any study time point, and any MRI contraindication. HC had to be free from any lifetime psychiatric disorder at both study points. MDD patients were excluded if diagnosed with bipolar or psychotic disorder, in case of acute substance dependence or with a history of electroconvulsive therapy. Diagnoses were verified at both study time points by trained clinical raters using the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV (SCID-I [28]). Details of the exclusion process are provided in Supplementary Fig. 1.
The final study sample comprised n = 57 patients with MDD and n = 37 HC. As described in our previous works [19, 29, 30], we divided the patient group into two subgroups, depending on their course if illness from baseline to follow-up (according to SCID-I criteria): Patients who were in ongoing depression at follow-up or experienced at least one depressive relapse between baseline and follow-up (MDD relapse-group; n = 37), and patients who were in full remission at follow-up without any further depressive episode after baseline (MDD no-relapse-group; n = 20). Demographic and clinical details of the sample are included in Table 1. Details on comorbidities can be found in Supplementary Table 1. There was an overlap of n = 87 participants with our previous study investigating long-term changes in brain function during conscious emotion processing, employing a different, unrelated fMRI paradigm [19].
At both study points, participants underwent fMRI while a subliminal affective priming paradigm was applied. Furthermore, SCID-I, [28] and Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS) [31] were conducted by trained clinical raters. Course of illness before and during study interval as well as type and dose of medication intake at both study points were documented. A composite medication load index (Supplementary Methods 1.1) was computed for each time point to quantify psychiatric medication intake at baseline and follow-up. Additionally, the frequency and type of psychotherapeutic interventions were documented (Table 1).
The study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Münster (2007-307-f-S). All procedures comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All participants gave written informed consent and received financial compensation for study participation.
Subliminal affective priming paradigm
In order to assess automatic emotion processing, a well-established subliminal affective priming paradigm [16, 18, 26] was used during fMRI. For detailed information on the paradigm and an illustration of an example trial, see Supplementary Methods 1.2 and Supplementary Fig. 2. Briefly, the paradigm consisted of 80 trials showing either a sad, happy, neutral face or no-face prime stimuli at subliminal perception level (duration: 33 ms), which was then masked by a neutral target face image of the same person at supraliminal perception level (duration: 467 ms). Face images were derived from the stimulus collection of Ekman and Friesen. Participants were asked to rate their impression of the emotional valence of each neutral target face from negative to positive on a four-point scale by pressing a button. Participants were unaware regarding the presence of emotional prime stimuli.
FMRI methods
Acquisition and preprocessing of fMRI data followed previously published protocols [19, 32, 33].
Data acquisition
T2* functional data were acquired using a single-shot echoplanar sequence with a 3T scanner (Gyroscan Intera 3T, Philips Medical System, Best, The Netherlands). Parameters were chosen to minimize distortion in the region of central interest retaining an adequate signal-to-noise ratio (S/N) and T2* sensitivity: 34 slices, matrix 64 × 64, resolution 3.6 × 3.6 × 3.6 mm; repetition time = 2.1 s, echo time = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°. Slices were acquired in an interleaved mode (first odd, then even), image numbering transversal F»H. To minimize dropout artifacts in the orbitofrontal and mediotemporal regions, slices were tilted 25° from the anterior and posterior commissure line. The paradigm was projected to the rear end of the scanner (Sharp XG-PC10XE with additional HF shielding; Osaka, Japan), while participants lay supine in the MRI scanner.
Data preprocessing
Functional data were realigned, unwarped and spatially normalized to the standard Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space. Images were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 6 mm full-width at half-maximum using Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM8; https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/).
Applying an event-related analysis design, trials were averaged separately for the happy, sad, neutral, and no-face prime condition for each participant and time point, reducing the data to four average trials per participant per time point. The onsets of the four prime conditions (positive, negative, neutral, no-face) were modeled by using a canonical haemodynamic response function. The model was corrected for serial correlations and a high-pass filter of 128 s was applied to remove low-frequency noise. Two individual first-level contrast images (sad > neutral, happy > neutral) were then used in the following second-level, random-effects group analyses. As we were interested in processing emotional stimuli, the no-face prime condition was not included in the analyses of this study.
Statistical analyses
Clinical and demographic data were analyzed using SPSS Statistics (version 28.0.1.0 for Windows; IBM Corporation).
All second-level analyses of fMRI data were performed with Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM12, version 7771 for MATLAB on Ubuntu; Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK; http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) and employed a region-of-interest (ROI) approach of the bilateral amygdala. The mask for the amygdala ROI was generated using the Wake Forest University PickAtlas20 toolbox (version 3.0; NeuroImaging Tools & Resources Collaboratory; https://www.nitrc.org/projects/wfu_pickatlas) within SPM12 according to the automated anatomical labeling atlas (version 3.1; [34]) definitions.
We used threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) as a non-parametric approach for the ROI analyses in the amygdala, by using the TFCE toolbox (version 232; Structural Brain Mapping Group, Jena, Germany; http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/tfce) implemented in SPM12. A combined peak-cluster-level family-wise error (FWE)-corrected threshold of p < 0.05 was applied to correct for multiple testing, obtained by 10,000 permutations per test.
We conducted a 3 × 2 × 2 analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with group (HC, MDD relapse group, MDD no-relapse group) as between-subjects factor and time (baseline, follow-up) as well as condition (happy>neutral, sad>neutral) as within-subjects factors. Age and sex were included as covariates of no interest.
Main effects (group, condition, time) as well as interaction effects (group × condition, group × time, condition × time, group × condition × time) were investigated. To examine our study objectives, we further tested for baseline differences in amygdala activity by assessing the group × condition interaction effect at baseline, as we expected a mood-congruent bias in (acute) depression (objective a). Subsequently, paired post-hoc t-tests within each group were performed to investigate within-group differences between conditions (sad>neutral, happy>neutral) as well as unpaired post-hoc t-tests comparing groups for each condition. Moreover, the group × condition interaction at follow-up (including subsequent post-hoc t-tests) was conducted to test whether the bias in emotion processing was still present after 2 years (objective b). Finally, an unpaired t-test comparing amygdala activity of the two MDD groups at baseline was conducted for each prime condition separately to test the predictive value of baseline amygdala activity for subsequent relapse (objective c).
For exploratory reasons, all analyses were additionally performed at the whole-brain level, by establishing a FWE-corrected threshold of p < 0.05 at voxel level.
Additional analyses
The Supplementary Methods provide a description of our additional analyses (a) controlling for acute comorbid (anxiety) disorders (Supplementary Methods 1.3.1), (b) investigating the effects of medication and psychotherapy on changes in brain function (Supplementary Methods 1.3.2), as well as examining effects of (c) the current mood state (Supplementary Methods 1.3.3), (d) prior disease progression (Supplementary Methods 1.3.4), and (d) environmental risk (Supplementary Methods 1.3.5) on amygdala activity. Details on analyses of behavioral data can be found in the Supplementary Methods 1.3.6.
Results
ROI analysis of amygdala
The 3 × 2 × 2 ANCOVA derived a significant main effect of group (left: k = 126, F2,362 = 6.23, pTFCE-FWE < 0.001, ηp² = 0.122; right: k = 198, F2,362 = 4.72, pTFCE-FWE < 0.001, ηp² = 0.089). Main effects of condition (pTFCE-FWE > 0.999) and time (pTFCE-FWE > 0.999) were not significant. A significant group × condition interaction emerged (right: k = 8, F1,362 = 11.40, pTFCE-FWE = 0.040, ηp² = 0.106), when both patient subgroups were considered together as one group. There was no significant group × time, condition × time or group × condition × time interaction effect on amygdala activity (all pTFCE-FWE ≥ 0.456).
Replication of the automatic mood-congruent emotion processing bias (objective a)
As stated in objective a, a significant group × condition interaction effect emerged in the bilateral amygdala at baseline (left: k = 17, t362 = 3.40, pTFCE-FWE = 0.027; right: k = 14, t362 = 3.14, pTFCE-FWE = 0.035). The post-hoc unpaired between-group t-tests revealed that both patient subgroups (MDD relapse and MDD no-relapse) showed significantly elevated amygdala activity to sad primes compared with HC (Table 2). For happy primes, there were no significant differences in amygdala activity between patients with depression and HC (all pTFCE-FWE ≥ 0.644), albeit HC had nominally higher amygdala contrast values for happy face primes (Fig. 1).
Paired t-tests within groups revealed that HC had higher amygdala activity to happy compared to sad primes (right: k = 45, t(362) = 3.46, pTFCE-FWE = 0.025). Patients with depression did not show significant differences in amygdala activity between happy and sad primes (all pTFCE-FWE ≥ 0.214), with, however, higher contrast values for sad primes compared to happy primes in both patient subgroups (Fig. 1).
The automatic emotion processing bias in the long-term course of depression (objective b)
At follow-up, even though the group x condition interaction effect in the amygdala was not significant (pTFCE-FWE = 0.107), both MDD subgroups continued to show elevated amygdala activity to sad primes compared with HC (Table 2). Surprisingly, for happy primes, the MDD relapse group showed even higher amygdala activity compared to HC at follow-up (left: k = 4, t(362) = 2.66, pTFCE-FWE = 0.046). The MDD relapse and the MDD no-relapse group did not differ significantly in amygdala activity in any condition at follow-up; however the relapse group tended to have higher amygdala activity to happy primes compared to the no-relapse group (pTFCE-FWE = 0.058).
Paired t-tests within groups at follow-up revealed no differences in amygdala activity between happy and sad primes within any subgroup (all pTFCE-FWE ≥ 0.222).
Baseline brain function and subsequent relapse (objective c)
The MDD relapse and the MDD no-relapse group did not differ in baseline amygdala activity neither in the sad prime (pTFCE-FWE = 0.522), nor in the happy prime condition. (pTFCE-FWE = 0.460).
Additional analyses
Effect of comorbid (anxiety) disorders on amygdala activity
When additionally controlling for acute comorbid disorders in general and anxiety disorders in particular, the main effect of group and the group x condition interaction in the amygdala remained significant (Supplementary Results 2.1).
Effects of medication dose and psychotherapy on amygdala activity
There was a significant medication load × time interaction effect specifically for happy primes, indicating that higher medication loads were associated with elevated amygdala activity to happy primes only at follow-up, whereas at baseline, medication load and amygdala activity showed no significant associations (Supplementary Results 2.2). Our analyses showed no significant effect of psychotherapy on (changes in) amygdala function.
Effect of current mood state on amygdala activity
Briefly, there was no significant association between depressive symptom severity and amygdala activity (all p ≥ 0.296; Supplementary Results 2.3). Additionally dividing patient groups by remission status at follow-up still revealed higher amygdala activity to sad primes in all patient subgroups compared to HC—independent of current remission status (Supplementary Table 2).
Effects of prior disease progression and environmental risk on amygdala activity
Patients with the first depressive episode at baseline did not differ in baseline amygdala activity to masked sad faces from patients in recurrence (pTFCE-FWE = 0.190). There was a tendency for patients at high environmental risk (patients who had experienced childhood maltreatment) to have increased baseline amygdala activity to sad primes compared to patients without experience of childhood maltreatment (pTFCE-FWE = 0.077).
Exploratory whole-brain analysis
At whole-brain level, a group × condition interaction effect emerged at baseline resulting from increased activity to sad primes in patients with MDD compared to HC in clusters including the middle temporal pole and middle temporal gyrus as well as the precentral, postcentral and angular gyrus and the superior occipital gyrus (Supplementary Table 3). At follow-up, groups did not differ in activity to sad primes on whole-brain level any more (pFWE ≥ 0.362). When lowering the threshold to punc > 0.001, patient groups—particularly the relapse group—still showed increased activity to sad primes in salience and visual processing networks compared to HC (Supplementary Table 4). For details on exploratory whole-brain analyses, see Supplementary Results 2.4.
Behavioral results of affective priming task
At baseline, the MDD no-relapse group tended to rate neutral targets following sad primes more negatively compared to those followed by happy primes and showed a slight change towards a more positive rating of targets followed by sad primes at follow-up (Supplementary Results 2.5). However, the post-hoc t-test did not survive Bonferroni-correction. Reaction times in the sad prime condition compared to neutral prime condition were significantly longer at baseline, regardless of group.
Discussion
The results of our study indicate that amygdala hyperactivity to masked negative stimuli persists in patients with depression after 2 years, regardless of relapse and current mood state. Thus, our results replicate the mood-congruent bias in depression during subliminal processing of emotional stimuli at the neural level, and furthermore suggest that this neural correlate of automatic emotion processing bias may be an enduring, trait-like feature of depression.
Replication of the automatic mood-congruent emotion processing bias (objective a)
In line with prior cross-sectional studies investigating automatic emotion processing [15,16,17], patients with acute depression showed elevated amygdala activity during masked sad faces compared with HC, as well as nominally lower amygdala responsiveness to masked happy faces. Our results provide evidence of an altered pattern of automatic amygdala responses in depression during automatic stages of the emotional stimuli processing: While HC showed stronger responses to happy than to sad primes, amygdala activity of depressed patients did not differ between the two prime conditions, with however, higher contrast values for sad faces than for happy faces. Such a tendency to favor positive information during automatic processing in HC is consistent with the findings of previous studies [16, 35, 36], and points to a positive processing bias in individuals without mood disorders [15]. Our results indicate not only the absence of a positive processing bias in depressed patients, but even point to a negative bias in depression—thus a heightened sensitivity of the amygdala towards negative information even during early, automatic stages of emotion processing, as depressed patients showed heightened amygdala activity to masked sad faces compared with HC. These findings were independent of depression severity and comorbid (anxiety) disorders. In addition to its central role in fear conditioning [37], the amygdala is involved in reward-based learning [38]. Salience detection by the amygdala is processed by two parallel pathways: a conscious cortical circuit, as well as an unconscious subcortical circuit [39]. The dysfunctional patterns of automatic amygdala responses in depressed patients can lead to distortions of later, more conscious stages of information processing and negatively impact social behavior. This is in line with cognitive models of depression, stating that negative schemata influence information processing and lead to negative automatic thought patterns, which take impact on the individual’s feelings and behavior [2].
Interestingly, the same pattern of results was also found at the whole-brain level at baseline, pointing to an involvement of cortical regions in the negative automatic bias in emotion processing, such as the superior occipital gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, the temporal pole, the precentral and postcentral gyrus, and the angular gyrus. The occipital gyrus is part of the visual network and has shown a covariation with amygdala activity especially during condition with masked stimuli [39]. Furthermore, also the sensory cortex areas and temporal cortical areas have bidirectional connections to the amygdala and are as well involved in emotion processing [40]. The postcentral gyrus involves the somatosensory cortex which is a central hub for processing sensory information [41], and the angular gyrus plays an important role in attention and cognitive processes, particularly in integrating information from different sensory information [42]. These findings support the notion, that besides the amygdala, also further brain regions involved in early attention and visual perception processes underlie the negative bias in automatic emotion processing in patients with depression.
In contrast to psychological studies [18, 43], our analyses did not reveal affective priming effects at the behavioral level, as the prime condition had no effect on valence ratings. Instead, all participants showed longer reaction times to masked sad compared to masked neutral faces. As negative stimuli are evolutionarily more significant, sad primes may require more processing resources, even if presented subliminally. However, there is likewise contrary evidence of faster processing of negative stimuli [44].
Patients with depression also did not show significantly more negative valence ratings compared to HC. Even though there was a tendency for the MDD no-relapse group rating sad primes significantly more negative compared to happy primes at baseline and showing slightly more positive ratings at follow-up, this effect did not survive the Bonferroni correction and should therefore be interpreted with caution.
In summary, we could not clearly demonstrate the affective priming effect on behavioral level, although detectable on brain functional level. Possibly, functional neuroimaging may be more sensitive in measuring biases in subliminal emotion processing compared to behavioral assessments, supporting the relevance of brain functional imaging for investigating the dynamics of emotion processing.
The automatic emotion processing bias in the long-term course of depression (objective b)
Our study reveals that the neurofunctional alterations during automatic emotion processing persist in the 2-year course of depression even in full remission: Both patient subgroups, independent of mood state and course of illness during study interval, still showed elevated amygdala activity to masked sad faces compared with HC. These findings stand in contrast to fMRI studies examining controlled stages of emotion processing employing supraliminal stimuli, that reported a normalization of brain functional alterations in case of remission [19,20,21]. Whereas brain functional alterations in later, controlled stages of emotion processing seem to be more sensitive and variable to the depressive state, the neurofunctional patterns of the early, automatic emotion processing bias seem to represent a stable “trait” marker of depression. Our findings are consistent with a previous cross-sectional study that found elevated amygdala activity to masked sad faces in patients in full remission compared to HC [17]. For happy primes, we could not find significantly reduced activity in patients with depression neither at baseline, nor at FU, which may imply that the bias in automatic emotion processing applies more strongly to negative primes.
However, it is unclear whether amygdala hyperactivity to negative stimuli develops as a consequence of or represents a risk marker for depression. Since all patients in our study were already depressed at baseline, increased amygdala activity to masked negative stimuli may be a result of the prior disease course and thus a scar effect of depression. For example, some brain structural studies showed no differences between subjects at risk of depression and HC or only found atrophy after the onset of depression [45, 46]. At least, our additional analyses revealed no significant difference between patients in their first depressive episode and patients in recurrence—however changes in amygdala activity could manifest as an early consequence of depression already in the first episode.
There are some indications that increased amygdala activity may represent a neural correlate of the vulnerability factors that underlie depression. For example, amygdala activity to automatic processing of subliminally presented negative stimuli has been found in association with childhood maltreatment [26], a relevant environmental risk factor for depression, as well as in carriers of the risk allele of the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism [25]. Furthermore, never-depressed daughters of depressed mothers already show attentional biases to negative facial expressions in dot-probe tasks [47, 48], supporting the notion that the negative bias in automatic emotion processing may be present in individuals at risk for developing depression already in early childhood. Consistent with this, we found that baseline amygdala activity to masked sad faces tended to be increased in patients who had experienced childhood maltreatment, suggesting a link between amygdala hyperactivity and risk factors for depression.
Whereas amygdala hyperactivity to negative stimuli persisted in both patient subgroups at follow-up, the difference in whole-brain activity between patients and HC was no longer significant after 2 years. In line with this, a cross-sectional study found that remitted patients with unipolar depression showed no difference in amygdala connectivity to other brain areas during subliminal emotion processing compared to HC—although amygdala activity itself was increased [49]. Possibly in our study, the conservative whole-brain FWE-corrected p < 0.05 significance threshold may have obscured potential differences between groups at follow-up. Lowering the threshold to punc < 0.001 for exploratory purposes suggested a still elevated activity to sad primes in regions of the salience and visual processing network in patients with depression, particularly the relapse group, compared to HC at follow-up. Together, these findings suggest that hyperactivity to subliminal negative processing represents a stable marker of depression, particularly in the amygdala. Abnormal functional patterns within other brain regions appear to be more closely tied to the depressive mood state, showing a degree of reversibility during periods of remission. Importantly, as the paradigm used in our study especially targets amygdala activity, the results at the whole-brain level should be interpreted carefully.
In contrast to our amygdala results, explicit treatment studies reported a normalization of the automatic emotion-processing bias in limbic regions after treatment with SSRIs [17, 22] and electroconvulsive therapy [24]. In line with this, another study found automatic amygdala activity to masked negative faces to be reduced after one week of citalopram intake in healthy individuals [50] indicating that antidepressants seem to act by reducing negative affective processing biases [51]. Even though we could not demonstrate these effects over the longer-term interval of 2 years in our study, our additional treatment analyses revealed that higher medication loads of psychiatric drugs were associated with elevated activity in the amygdala and caudate nucleus during the processing of masked happy faces. Higher activity in these regions during subliminal processing of happy primes may, therefore, reflect an elevated automatic sensitivity towards positive information and, therefore, a shift towards a positivity bias induced by medication.
These results may also explain why the MDD relapse group even showed higher amygdala activity towards happy primes compared to HC at follow-up. The MDD relapse group had significantly higher medication loads compared to the MDD no-relapse group at follow-up, so the differences may reflect a medication-induced effect rather than differences due to course of illness. Accordingly, a randomized placebo-controlled study showed that short-term treatment with citalopram increased amygdala activity to happy faces in healthy controls [52]. Alternatively, amygdala hyperactivity could also occur in a valence-unspecific manner during the progression of depression, so that the relapse group specifically shows increased amygdala activation at follow-up.
While HC showed significantly higher amygdala activity to happy compared to sad primes at baseline, we could no longer detect this positive bias at follow-up—possibly due to an adaptation to the priming paradigm in our study or due to a higher degree of variation in mood states at follow-up. Nominally, however, HC still showed higher activity to happy compared to sad primes at follow-up.
Baseline brain function and subsequent relapse (objective c)
Contrary to our third hypothesis, brain activity at baseline did not differ between patient subgroups neither in the amygdala nor on whole-brain level. If replicated, this finding indicates that activity during automatic emotion processing does not appear to be a marker for predicting subsequent relapse. In contrast, in one of our earlier studies, we showed baseline differences in limbic activity between patients differing in their subsequent courses of illness [19]. These conflicting results may derive by different grades of consciousness during emotion processing, as in our earlier study, we investigated controlled stages of emotion processing using supraliminal stimuli. As automatic amygdala hyperactivity to negative stimuli seems to be a trait marker of depression in general, it may not be suitable for stratifying patient subgroups. Since our findings are preliminary and the sample size of the subgroup analyses is rather small, replication through additional long-term studies that specifically focus on automatic emotion processing is required.
Strengths and limitations
With a study interval of 2 years, this is the first prospective neuroimaging study examining changes in brain activity during automatic emotion processing in patients with depression over a period of more than 8 weeks. A well-established fMRI paradigm was implemented to measure the amygdala activity during the early, automatic stages of emotion processing. Also, the inclusion of a behavioral data analyses allows us a direct comparison of changes in the emotion processing bias over the depressive course over 2 years on brain functional and behavioral level. The naturalistic study design may allow a good transferability of the results to clinical reality.
Nevertheless, there are also some relevant limitations. The downside of a naturalistic study design is that there are factors that could have influenced the results, which can not be fully controlled (e.g., life events, prior course of illness, specific therapeutic interventions during study interval). It further has to be noted that our sample has an increased clinical severity, as all patients were hospitalized at baseline. About two-thirds relapsed or were still depressed after 2 years. Although there were no effects of first vs. recurrent episodes on baseline amygdala activity, it remains to be investigated whether our results also apply to patients with lower clinical severity. In addition, the sample size of patient subgroups is relatively small, potentially reducing the power of cross-sectional analyses in particular. Long-term fMRI studies with larger samples are challenging, but desirable.
In summary, our study indicates that automatic amygdala hyperactivity to subliminally presented negative stimuli persists even in the case of remission after 2 years. This marks a difference to later, more controlled stages of emotion processing and could be interpreted as an enduring trait marker for depression, possibly representing a neural correlate of vulnerability to or a consequence of depression.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, RR, upon reasonable request.
References
GBD 2019 Mental Disorders Collaborators. Global, regional, and national burden of 12 mental disorders in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Lancet Psychiatry. 2022;9:137–50.
Beck AT. The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:969–77.
Teasdale JD. Negative thinking in depression: cause, effect, or reciprocal relationship? Adv Behav Res Ther. 1983;5:3–25.
Mathews A, MacLeod C. Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2005;1:167–95.
Gotlib IH, Joormann J. Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010;6:285–312.
Leppänen JM. Emotional information processing in mood disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging findings. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2006;19:34–9.
Dearing KF, Gotlib IH. Interpretation of ambiguous information in girls at risk for depression. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37:79–91.
Pérez-Edgar K, Bar-Haim Y, McDermott JM, Gorodetsky E, Hodgkinson CA, Goldman D, et al. Variations in the serotonin-transporter gene are associated with attention bias patterns to positive and negative emotion faces. Biol Psychol. 2010;83:269–71.
Dannlowski U, Kersting A, Donges U–S, Lalee–Mentzel J, Arolt V, Suslow TH. Masked facial affect priming is associated with therapy response in clinical depression. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;256:215–21.
Newman K, Quigley L, Fernandez A, Dobson K, Sears C. Concurrent and prospective relations between attentional biases for emotional images and relapse to depression. Cogn Ther Res. 2019;43:893–909.
Lau MA, Segal ZV, Williams JMG. Teasdale’s differential activation hypothesis: implications for mechanisms of depressive relapse and suicidal behaviour. Behav Res Ther. 2004;42:1001–17.
Bouhuys AL, Geerts E, Gordijn MCM. Depressed patients’ perceptions of facial emotions in depressed and remitted states are associated with relapse: a longitudinal study. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1999;187:595–602.
Davis M, Whalen PJ. The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Mol Psychiatry. 2001;6:13–34.
LeDoux J. The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cell Mol Neurobiol. 2003;23:727–38.
Stuhrmann A, Dohm K, Kugel H, Zwanzger P, Redlich R, Grotegerd D, et al. Mood-congruent amygdala responses to subliminally presented facial expressions in major depression: associations with anhedonia. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2013;38:249–58.
Suslow T, Konrad C, Kugel H, Rumstadt D, Zwitserlood P, Schöning S, et al. Automatic mood-congruent amygdala responses to masked facial expressions in major depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67:155–60.
Victor TA, Furey ML, Fromm SJ, Öhman A, Drevets WC. Relationship between amygdala responses to masked faces and mood state and treatment in major depressive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:1128.
Dannlowski U, Ohrmann P, Bauer J, Kugel H, Arolt V, Heindel W, et al. Amygdala reactivity predicts automatic negative evaluations for facial emotions. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2007;154:13–20.
Enneking V, Klug M, Borgers T, Dohm K, Grotegerd D, Frankenberger LM, et al. Changes in brain function during negative emotion processing in the long-term course of depression. Br J Psychiatry. 2022;221:476–84.
Ai H, Opmeer EM, Marsman JBC, Veltman DJ, van der Wee NJA, Aleman A, et al. Longitudinal brain changes in MDD during emotional encoding: effects of presence and persistence of symptomatology. Psychol Med. 2020;50:1316–26.
Opmeer EM, Kortekaas R, van Tol MJ, Renken RJ, Demenescu LR, Woudstra S, et al. Changes in regional brain activation related to depressive state: a 2-year longitudinal functional MRI study: research article: neural network reflecting remission of depression. Depress Anxiety. 2016;33:35–44.
Sheline YI, Barch DM, Donnelly JM, Ollinger JM, Snyder AZ, Mintun MA. Increased amygdala response to masked emotional faces in depressed subjects resolves with antidepressant treatment: an fMRI study. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:651–8.
Williams LM, Korgaonkar MS, Song YC, Paton R, Eagles S, Goldstein-Piekarski A, et al. Amygdala reactivity to emotional faces in the prediction of general and medication-specific responses to antidepressant treatment in the randomized iSPOT-D trial. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:2398–408.
Redlich R, Bürger C, Dohm K, Grotegerd D, Opel N, Zaremba D, et al. Effects of electroconvulsive therapy on amygdala function in major depression—a longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychol Med. 2017;47:2166–76.
Dannlowski U, Ohrmann P, Bauer J, Deckert J, Hohoff C, Kugel H, et al. 5-HTTLPR biases amygdala activity in response to masked facial expressions in major depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:418–24.
Dannlowski U, Kugel H, Huber F, Stuhrmann A, Redlich R, Grotegerd D, et al. Childhood maltreatment is associated with an automatic negative emotion processing bias in the amygdala: amygdalar emotion processing bias. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013;34:2899–909.
Kugel H, Eichmann M, Dannlowski U, Ohrmann P, Bauer J, Arolt V, et al. Alexithymic features and automatic amygdala reactivity to facial emotion. Neurosci Lett. 2008;435:40–4.
Wittchen HU, Zaudig M, Fydrich T. SKID. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV. Achse I und II. Handanweisung. Göttingen: Hogrefe;
Zaremba D, Dohm K, Redlich R, Grotegerd D, Strojny R, Meinert S, et al. Association of brain cortical changes with relapse in patients with major depressive disorder. JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75:484.
Opel N, Redlich R, Dohm K, Zaremba D, Goltermann J, Repple J, et al. Mediation of the influence of childhood maltreatment on depression relapse by cortical structure: a 2-year longitudinal observational study. Lancet Psychiatry. 2019;6:318–26.
Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;23:56–62.
Dannlowski U, Stuhrmann A, Beutelmann V, Zwanzger P, Lenzen T, Grotegerd D, et al. Limbic scars: long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment revealed by functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71:286–93.
Redlich R, Grotegerd D, Opel N, Kaufmann C, Zwitserlood P, Kugel H, et al. Are you gonna leave me? Separation anxiety is associated with increased amygdala responsiveness and volume. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2015;10:278–84.
Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in spm using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. NeuroImage. 2002;15:273–89.
Herbert C, Ethofer T, Anders S, Junghofer M, Wildgruber D, Grodd W, et al. Amygdala activation during reading of emotional adjectives—an advantage for pleasant content. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2009;4:35–49.
Killgore WDS, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Activation of the amygdala and anterior cingulate during nonconscious processing of sad versus happy faces. NeuroImage. 2004;21:1215–23.
LeDoux JE. Emotional memory systems in the brain. Behav Brain Res. 1993;58:69–79.
Baxter MG, Murray EA. The amygdala and reward. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:563–73.
Morris JS, Öhman A, Dolan RJ. A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating “unseen” fear. Proc Natl Acad Sci 1999;96:1680–5.
Price JL. Comparative aspects of amygdala connectivity. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2006;985:50–8.
Kropf E, Syan SK, Minuzzi L, Frey BN. From anatomy to function: the role of the somatosensory cortex in emotional regulation. Braz J Psychiatry. 2019;41:261–9.
Seghier ML. The angular gyrus: multiple functions and multiple subdivisions. Neuroscientist. 2013;19:43–61.
Murphy ST, Zajonc RB. Affect, cognition, and awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1993;64:723–39.
Ahorsu DK, Tsang HW. Do people with depression always have decreased cognitive processing speed? Evidence through electrophysiological lens. Neuropsychiatry. 2018;8:1227–1231.
Papmeyer M, Sussmann JE, Stewart T, Giles S, Centola JG, Zannias V, et al. Prospective longitudinal study of subcortical brain volumes in individuals at high familial risk of mood disorders with or without subsequent onset of depression. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2016;248:119–25.
Lebedeva A, Sundström A, Lindgren L, Stomby A, Aarsland D, Westman E, et al. Longitudinal relationships among depressive symptoms, cortisol, and brain atrophy in the neocortex and the hippocampus. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2018;137:491–502.
Joormann J, Talbot L, Gotlib IH. Biased processing of emotional information in girls at risk for depression. J Abnorm Psychol. 2007;116:135–43.
Kujawa AJ, Torpey D, Kim J, Hajcak G, Rose S, Gotlib IH, et al. Attentional biases for emotional faces in young children of mothers with chronic or recurrent depression. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2011;39:125–35.
Korgaonkar MS, Erlinger M, Breukelaar IA, Boyce P, Hazell P, Antees C, et al. Amygdala activation and connectivity to emotional processing distinguishes asymptomatic patients with bipolar disorders and unipolar depression. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2019;4:361–70.
Harmer CJ, Mackay CE, Reid CB, Cowen PJ, Goodwin GM. Antidepressant drug treatment modifies the neural processing of nonconscious threat cues. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:816–20.
Pringle A, Browning M, Cowen PJ, Harmer CJ. A cognitive neuropsychological model of antidepressant drug action. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2011;35:1586–92.
Norbury R, Taylor MJ, Selvaraj S, Murphy SE, Harmer CJ, Cowen PJ. Short-term antidepressant treatment modulates amygdala response to happy faces. Psychopharmacology. 2009;206:197–204.
Funding
The study was supported by grants of the Innovative Medizinische Forschung (RE111722 to RR, I-KO121806 to KD), the German Research Foundation (DFG, grant RE4458/1-1 to RR, FOR2107 DA1151/5-1 and DA1151/5-2 to UD), the Interdisciplinary Center for Clinical Research (IZKF) of the medical faculty of Münster (grant Dan3/012/17 to UD), and the BMBF (01EE2305C to R.R.). Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
MK, VE, KD, RR, and UD contributed to the conception and design of the study. All authors contributed to the organization of the conduct of the study (including the acquisition of data). MK, VE, CMJ, RR, and UD analyzed and interpreted the data. MK, VE, RR, and UD drafted the work and all other authors revised it critically for important intellectual content. All authors have read and approved the final version of the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Klug, M., Enneking, V., Borgers, T. et al. Persistence of amygdala hyperactivity to subliminal negative emotion processing in the long-term course of depression. Mol Psychiatry 29, 1501–1509 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-024-02429-4
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-024-02429-4