Abstract
Age, female sex, and apolipoprotein E4 (E4) are risk factors to develop Alzheimer’s disease (AD). There are three major human apoE isoforms: E2, E3, and E4. Compared to E3, E4 increases while E2 decreases AD risk. However, E2 is associated with increased risk and severity of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). In cognitively healthy adults, E4 carriers have greater brain activation during learning and memory tasks in the absence of behavioral differences. Human apoE targeted replacement (TR) mice display differences in fear extinction that parallel human data: E2 mice show impaired extinction, mirroring heightened PTSD symptoms in E2 combat veterans. Recently, an adaptive role of DNA double strand breaks (DSBs) in immediate early gene expression (IEG) has been described. Age and disease synergistically increase DNA damage and decrease DNA repair. As the mechanisms underlying the relative risks of apoE, sex, and their interactions in aging are unclear, we used young (3 months) and middle-aged (12 months) male and female TR mice to investigate the influence of these factors on DSBs and IEGs at baseline and following contextual fear conditioning. We assessed brain-wide changes in neural activation following fear conditioning using whole-brain cFos imaging in young female TR mice. E4 mice froze more during fear conditioning and had lower cFos immunoreactivity across regions important for somatosensation and contextual encoding compared to E2 mice. E4 mice also showed altered co-activation compared to E3 mice, corresponding to human MRI and cognitive data, and indicating that there are differences in brain activity and connectivity at young ages independent of fear learning. There were increased DSB markers in middle-aged animals and alterations to cFos levels dependent on sex and isoform, as well. The increase in hippocampal DSB markers in middle-aged animals and female E4 mice may play a role in the risk for developing AD.
Introduction
Sporadic Alzheimer’s disease (AD) comprises more than 90% of AD diagnoses [1]. Age and female sex are the two highest risk factors for development of late onset AD (LOAD) [2, 3]. Additionally, risk to develop post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is higher in women than men [4]. Apolipoprotein E (apoE) isoform is the next greatest predictor for LOAD [5, 6]. ApoE exists in three major human isoforms: E2, E3, and E4. Compared to E3 carriers, E4 carriers have a higher risk and E2 carriers a lower risk to develop LOAD [6]. This relative protection/risk for LOAD is ethnicity-dependent: the E4-LOAD risk is seen in White and Japanese populations but absent in Native American and non-white Hispanic populations and variable in Black populations [6,7,8]. While E2 is relatively protective against LOAD, it is associated with higher rates and greater severity of PTSD [9,10,11]. Evidence in both people and mice shows that E2 carriers have poorer outcomes in melanoma than E4 carriers (increased tumor size, metastasis, and reduced survival) [12]. How the apoE isoforms lead to differential and specific disease risk or protection is unclear.
Importantly, sex and apoE affect cognition throughout life. For example, 7–10-year-old female E4 carriers perform worse on visual and spatial recognition tasks than female E3 carriers [13]. However, this effect is not seen in males. In young adults, E4 carriers show increased brain glucose metabolism [14]. After AD diagnosis, women show a faster cognitive decline than men despite similar amyloid β levels [15, 16]. This sex difference in cognitive decline is possibly due to increased oxidative stress and pro-inflammatory markers in aging women [17,18,19]. Investigations looking at brain activation at rest and during learning and memory tasks have shown apoE-dependent differences in healthy middle-aged people in the absence of cognitive differences, with E4-carriers displaying greater blood oxygen levels during learning trials compared to age- and sex-matched E3 carriers [20]. These studies together suggest that E4 carriers have increased energy usage at young ages.
In rodents, immediate early genes (IEGs)—such as cFos—are used as markers of neuronal activation, as they have low baseline levels, are rapidly expressed upon stimulation, and return to baseline levels within a few hours [21, 22]. IEGs are important for synaptic plasticity, learning, and memory [21,22,23,24,25,26]. Interfering with typical IEG expression impairs memory retrieval [27] and induced IEG expression can create false memories [28] in rodents. ApoE-dependent differences in expression of IEGs are relatively unexplored. One study reported that E4 mice on a high fat diet show less IEG expression in the hippocampus than E3 mice [29]; another found apoE-dependent changes in phosphorylation of cFos (an IEG) in induced pluripotent stem cells (iPSCs) [30].
The mechanisms that lead to IEG expression are unclear. Recently, evidence has emerged for a role for DNA double strand breaks (DSBs) in IEG expression. Stimulation of hippocampal neurons in vitro leads to DSBs on the transcription start sites of IEGs [31]. Exposure of 4–6 month old C57Bl/6J wild-type (WT) mice to a novel environment leads to a transient increase in γH2Ax—a DSB repair marker—in relevant brain regions [32], and WT mice show increased hippocampal DSBs after fear learning [31]. Tight regulation of DSBs and their proper repair is likely important for this mechanism. Young mice carrying familial dominant mutations for AD have higher baseline levels of γH2Ax at baseline and fail to return to baseline levels 24 hours after stimulation [32]. Increased γH2Ax and 53BP1 (another DSB repair marker) are also observed in post-mortem tissue of patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and AD [33], suggesting that dysregulation of DSBs might be an early, driving factor in age-related cognitive decline and dementia.
Contextual fear conditioning is a well-studied learning test with clearly defined neuroanatomy [34,35,36]. cFos expression increases in specific brain regions of rodents after fear conditioning and is dependent on the paradigm used [37, 38]. The hippocampus is particularly important for contextual fear conditioning. Increases are seen in the hippocampus following contextual fear conditioning [38], and inhibition of cFos in the CA1 impairs contextual recall [39]. Beyond the hippocampus, many regions have been defined that contribute to different aspects of fear encoding, including somatosensation (such as the locus coeruleus), contextual encoding (such as the entorhinal cortex), fear integration (the amygdalar nuclei), and fear expression (such as the periaqueductal gray) [34]. These rodent regions mirror those observed in human functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during fear conditioning [40].
There are apoE isoform differences in anxiety-like and fear learning and memory in human apoE targeted replacement (TR) mice, which express the human isoforms under control of the murine apoE promoter. E2 mice display impaired fear extinction [41], and E4 mice show increased anxiety-like behavior [42]. Moreover, E2 mice show alterations in the endocrine system and behavioral measures following exposure to trauma, which are potentially related to the higher PTSD clinical scores and blunted cortisol levels in E2-carrying combat veterans and refugees [9, 11].
Altogether, early-life differences in IEG signaling and DSB regulation might contribute to the apoE-specific risks for LOAD and PTSD. Here, we used young (3 months) and middle-aged (12 months) male and female apoE TR mice to assess DSBs and IEG expression at baseline and following contextual fear conditioning. Due to increased risk for females to develop AD and PTSD, we also investigated possible early changes using whole-brain cFos imaging in young female apoE TR mice [3] following fear learning. We assessed brain-wide alterations in neuronal activation [43] and co-activation across regions similar to functional MRI analyses [44, 45]. We hypothesized that each apoE isoform would show distinct patterns of cFos activation and DSB formation and repair, and that alterations would become more pronounced with aging.
Materials & methods
Mice
We bred young (3 months old) and middle-aged (12 months old) male and female human apoE TR mice, originally generated by Sullivan et al. [46,47,48]. A total of n = 306 mice were used: experiment 1 (whole brain imaging) n = 60; experiment 2 (cFo + 53BP1 immunohistochemistry) n = 126; and experiment 3 (ChIP-ddPCR) n = 123. Mice were randomly assigned to groups.
Experiment 1 involved 1 behavioral condition (fear training); experiment 2 involved 3 behavioral conditions (behaviorally naïve, fear training, or fear training + recovery); experiment 3 involved 2 behavioral conditions (behaviorally naïve or fear training; Fig. 1A). All mice were singly housed 4 days prior to testing and provided extra nesting material. Food and water were provided ad libitum and lights were on a standard 12 h light: dark cycle.
A Schematic of the experimental design. B Average baseline motion in males during the initial 2 min of fear conditioning. There was a main effect of apoE isoform (p = 0.037, F(2,60) = 3.480), driven by E2 vs. E3 (p = 0.042). There was no effect of age (p = 0.927) and no interaction (p = 0.571). C Average baseline motion in females. There was a main effect of apoE isoform (p = 0.006, F(2,130) = 5.301), driven by E3 vs. E4 (p < 0.001). Middle aged mice moved more than young mice (p < 0.001); there was no interaction (p = 0.403). D Average motion during the two shocks in males. Middle aged mice moved more than young mice (p = 0.0055, F(1,60) = 8.246). There were no apoE isoform differences (p = 0.068) and no interaction (p = 0.458). E Average motion during the two shocks in females. Middle aged mice moved more than young mice (p < 0.0001, F(1,130) = 23.25). There were no apoE isoform differences (p = 0.070) and no interaction (p = 0.317). F Percent time freezing during the ISIs in males. There was an effect of time (p < 0.001, F(1,60) = 22.842), isoform (p = 0.005, F(2,60) = 5.90), age (p < 0.001, F(1,60) = 19.087), and a time-by-isoform interaction (p = 0.043, F(2,60) = 3.318). E4 mice froze more than E2 (p = 0.008) and E3 (p = 0.014) mice. G Percent time freezing during the ISIs in females. There was an effect of time (p < 0.001, F(1,130) = 165.030), isoform (p < 0.001, F(2,130) = 10.258), and a time-by-isoform interaction (p = 0.049, F(2,130) = 3.087). E4 mice froze more than E2 (p = 0.002) and E3 (p < 0.001) mice. H Z-scores of cFos-positive cells in the entire brain of young, female mice. There was a main effect of apoE isoform (p = 0.020, F(2,26) = 4.573), driven by E2 vs. E4 (p = 0.0319) and a trend in E3 vs. E4 (p = 0.0664). I Z-scores of cFos-positive cells in all regions involved in contextual fear learning in young, female mice. There was a main effect of apoE isoform (p = 0.0172, F(2,26) = 4.773), driven by E2 vs. E4 (p = 0.0270) and E3 vs. E4 (p = 0.0434) mice. Behavioral data are presented as mean ± SEM. cFos immunoreactivity data are presented as minimum to maximum boxplots. Each point represents an individual animal. #p < 0.07, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ****p < 0.0001.
All animal procedures were reviewed and approved by the OHSU IACUC and in accordance with AAALAC standards. All procedures followed the ARRIVE guidelines. Researchers were blinded to groups throughout the duration of experiments until all data were analyzed.
Contextual fear conditioning
We used a standard 2-shock contextual fear conditioning paradigm, as described previously [41]. Mice received two 0.5 mA shocks over the course of 6 min in plexiglass chambers with a shock grid (Med Associates, St. Albans, Vermont).
Tissue collection & whole-brain imaging
Experiment 1: Two hours after fear conditioning, mice were intracardially perfused to capture peak cFos levels [49, 50]. Brains were stored overnight in 4% PFA, then switched to PBS and sent to Certerra, Inc. (acquired by Certego Therapeutics) for whole brain imaging as previously described [43].
Experiment 2: Mice were intracardially perfused 1.5 h after fear conditioning (the overlap between peak cFos expression and 53BP1 [50, 51]), 24 h after fear conditioning, or without ever experiencing fear conditioning (behaviorally naïve). Brains were switched to cryopreserve after 24 h.
Experiment 3: Mice were euthanized by cervical dislocation and decapitation either 5 min after fear conditioning [52] or behaviorally naïve. The hippocampus and cortex were dissected and flash frozen in liquid nitrogen, then stored at −80 °C until use.
Dual-label immunofluorescence & microscopy
Immunofluorescence was done on free-floating 40 µm sections [53, 54] using primary antibodies against 53BP1 (Bethyl Labs, #A300-272A) and cFos (PhosphoSolutions, #309-cFOS), secondary antibodies (AlexaFluor647, Invitrogen, #A-21245; and AlexaFluor488, Invitrogen, #A-11001) and a DAPI (Sigma D9542) counterstain. Sections were slide mounted with CitiFluor CFMR2 Antifade Solution and sealed with Biotium CoverGrip Coverslip Sealant.
Z-stack images of the CA3 and CA1 (3 sections/animal) were taken using a Zeiss LSM 980 with Airyscan 2 at 63x zoom. The number of cells with 53BP1 foci, cFos, and co-localized signal were manually counted using ImageJ (NIH, Bethesda, MD).
Chromatin Immunoprecipitation & digital droplet PCR
Chromatin immunoprecipitation (ChIP) was done with a ThermoFisher MAGnify Kit (ThermoFisher, #492024) as previously described [55] on hippocampal tissue using a γH2Ax antibody (Cell Signal, #9718). Concentrations for ChIP and input samples were measured using the Qubit dsDNA High Sensitivity kit (ThermoFisher).
Digital droplet PCR (ddPCR) was used to assess cFos, Npas4, and BDNF expression [56]. Samples were prepared according to the BioRad QX200™ Droplet Digital™ PCR System manual (BioRad, Hercules, CA).
Statistical analysis
All data were first analyzed for normality. We implemented Bonferroni corrections for all post hoc tests. Due to the repeated effects of and interactions with sex, we split males and females for all analyses.
For fear conditioning, average motion and percent time freezing were analyzed using an ANOVA. For learning, percent time spent freezing during the ISIs was analyzed with a repeated-measures ANOVA.
The number of cFos+ cells, 53BP1 + cells, and cFos-53BP1 + cells in the CA1 and CA3 were analyzed with a multi-way ANOVA. The concentration of cFos, Npas4, and BDNF (measured in copies/µL) was normalized to total DNA and analyzed with an ANOVA. To determine relative enrichment, cFos and NPAS4 concentrations were compared to BDNF levels.
For whole-brain analysis, ClearMap was applied to collected light-sheet images to segment brain regions and count number of cFos+ cells [57]. We first analyzed the raw number of cFos+ cells throughout the brain. We discovered significant differences between cohorts, likely due to the sensitive nature of IEGs. As such, we transformed the cFos counts into Z-scores, normalized to the E3 animals in each cohort.
We then proceeded to analyze differences in cFos activation across the brain. We identified brain regions known to be important for contextual fear learning [34, 35] and organized them into 4 distinct subcategories: somatosensation, contextual encoding, fear integration, and fear expression (supplemental table 1). We then used repeated-measures ANOVAs to identify regional and isoform-dependent differences. To localize regional differences, we subsequently used MANOVAs with Bonferroni corrections.
We have previously used this type of data as an indirect measure of connectivity [44, 45]. Here, we calculated the Pearson’s coefficient for all fear-related brain regions, built correlation matrices, and compared the matrices, as previously described [58]. This method is sensitive to small numbers of pairwise differences, making the test appropriate for examining differences in gene expression [59] or neuronal connectivity [45]. This produces a test statistic, \(\hat T\); the p-value is determined by counting the number of multiplier bootstraps larger than the test statistic [60]. This was shown to be appropriate even in cases where the number of dimensions is greater than the number of subjects [58].
Results
ApoE isoform-dependent differences in fear learning and whole-brain cFos expression
We first assessed if apoE isoform affects contextual fear learning (Fig. 1A). Prior to any shocks, we detected a difference in baseline motion between the distinct apoE isoforms in both males (p = 0.037, Fig. 1B) and females (p = 0.006, Fig. 1C). E3 males moved more than E2 males (p = 0.042) and E3 females moved more than E4 females (p < 0.001). Additionally, middle-aged females moved more than young females (p < 0.001). There were no apoE-dependent differences in average motion during the shocks, though middle-aged mice moved more than young mice in both males (p = 0.006, Fig. 1D) and females (p < 0.001, Fig. 1E).
The percent time freezing during the inter-stimulus intervals (ISIs), a measure of fear learning, was dependent on apoE isoform (males: p = 0.005, Fig. 1F; females: p < 0.001, Fig. 1G). Male and female E4 mice froze more during the ISIs than either E2 (p = 0.008, p = 0.002, respectively) or E3 mice (p = 0.014, p < 0.001, respectively). Additionally, middle-aged male mice froze more than young male mice (p < 0.001); middle-aged female mice trended towards freezing more than young female mice (p = 0.071). While all mice displayed an increase in freezing from ISI 1 to ISI 2 (p < 0.001, both sexes), this pattern was distinct in the isoforms as shown by a time-by-isoform interaction (males: p = 0.043; females: p = 0.049).
Following fear conditioning, we assessed brain activation via cFos immunoreactivity across the whole brain and in regions known to contribute to contextual fear learning in young female mice (Supplemental Table 1). There were apoE isoform differences in the global magnitude of cFos activation (p = 0.020, Fig. 1H), with E4 mice having less cFos immunoreactivity than E2 mice (p = 0.0319) and trended towards having less cFos immunoreactivity than E3 mice (p = 0.0664). We next focused our analysis on regions important for contextual fear learning. Again, there was an overall difference dependent on apoE isoform (p = 0.0172; Fig. 1I). Post hoc testing revealed that E4 mice had less cFos immunoreactivity in all fear learning regions compared to E2 (p = 0.0270) and E3 mice (p = 0.0434).
ApoE isoform-dependent differences in magnitude of cFos activation in fear circuitry of young female mice
To clarify differences between the apoE isoforms in brain activation, we organized brain regions involved in contextual fear learning into distinct categories: somatosensation, contextual encoding, fear integration, and fear expression (Supplementary Table 1).
There was a difference between isoforms in regions important for somatosensation (p = 0.031; Fig. 2A, B), driven by the difference between E2 and E4 mice (p = 0.028). The sensory-motor cortex-related area was different based on apoE isoform (p = 0.048, MANOVA; Fig. 2D), with post hoc testing indicating a trend towards a difference in E2 compared to E4 mice (p = 0.058). There was no difference in the posterior complex of the thalamus (Fig. 2E), intralaminar area (Fig. 2C), parabrachial nucleus (Fig. 2F), locus coeruleus (Fig. 2G), or agranular insular area (Fig. 2H).
A Schematic of brain regions involved in somatosensation. B Average of regions involved in somatosensation. A repeated measures ANOVA in these regions indicated a difference based on apoE isoform (p = 0.031, F(2,26) = 4.000), driven by the differences between E2 and E4 (p = 0.028). Z-scores of cFos levels were seen in (C) the intralaminar nucleus of the dorsal thalamus. D The thalamus, sensory-motor cortex related. E The posterior complex of the thalamus. F The parabrachial nucleus. G The locus coeruleus, and (H) the agranular insular area. Subsequent analysis identified apoE isoform-specific differences in the thalamus, sensory-motor cortex related (p = 0.048, F(2,26) = 3.417). The Bonferroni’s post hoc test revealed that the difference was driven by E2 vs. E3 mice (p = 0.048). I Schematic of brain regions involved in contextual encoding. J Average of regions involved in contextual encoding. A repeated measures ANOVA in these regions indicated a difference based on region (p = 0.040, F(3.494,90.837) = 2.743, Greenhouse-Geisser corrected) and apoE isoform (p = 0.002, F(2,26) = = 7.671). The Bonferroni’s post hoc test revealed an overall difference between E2 and E4 mice (p = 0.002) and a trend towards a difference between E3 and E4 mice (p = 0.078). Z-scores of cFos immunoreactivity levels were seen in (K) the piriform area. L The primary somatosensory area. M The subiculum, ventral part. N The perirhinal area. O The entorhinal cortex. P The hippocampus. Subsequent analysis identified apoE isoform-specific differences in the entorhinal cortex (p = 0.018, F(2,26) = 4.695) and the hippocampus (p = 0.045, F(2,26) = 3.506). Trends towards apoE isoform differences were seen in the primary somatosensory area (p = 0.074) and the perirhinal area (p = 0.074). The Bonferroni’s post hoc revealed that the difference in the entorhinal cortex was driven by E3 vs. E4 mice (p = 0.031), with a trend towards a difference between E2 vs. E4 mice (p = 0.057). Data are presented in minimum to maximum boxplots. Each point represents an individual animal. *p < 0.05.
Analysis of regions important for contextual encoding revealed an effect of region (p = 0.040, Greenhouse-Geisser corrected; Fig. 2I, J), indicating that these regions have distinct patterns of cFos activation. There was also an effect of isoform (p = 0.002). Again, E4 mice had lower cFos levels than E2 mice (p = 0.002) and trended towards lower levels than E3 mice (p = 0.078). There were no differences between E2 and E3 mice. Follow-up analyses on specific sub-regions revealed that cFos immunoreactivity was different in the entorhinal cortex (p = 0.018; Fig. 2O) and hippocampus (p = 0.045; Fig. 2P), though post hoc analysis only revealed a difference between E3 and E4 (p = 0.031) and a trend towards a difference between E2 and E4 (p = 0.057) in the entorhinal cortex. There was no difference in the primary somatosensory area (Fig. 2L), perirhinal area (Fig. 2N), piriform area (Fig. 2K) or subiculum, ventral part (Fig. 2M).
Next, we analyzed cFos immunoreactivity in the amygdalar nuclei (Fig. 3A). There were no overall isoform-dependent differences in cFos immunoreactivity (p = 0.272; Fig. 3B), but there was a difference in the basolateral amygdala (p = 0.035; Fig. 3D) with E2 mice having higher cFos levels than E4 mice (p = 0.035). There was no difference in the lateral (Fig. 3C), basomedial (Fig. 3E), or central amygdala (Fig. 3F).
A Schematic of brain regions involved in fear integration. B Average of regions involved in fear integration. A repeated measures ANOVA in these regions did not reveal any overall differences between apoE isoforms (p = 0.272). Z-scores of cFos immunoreactivity levels were seen in (C) the lateral amygdala, D The basolateral amygdala, E The basomedial amygdala, F The central amygdala. Subsequent analysis identified apoE isoform-specific differences in the basolateral amygdala (p = 0.035, F(2,26) = 3.810) and a trend towards apoE isoform differences in the lateral amygdala (p = 0.078). The Bonferroni’s post hoc test revealed that the difference in the basolateral amygdala was driven by E2 vs. E4 mice (p = 0.035). G Average of regions involved in fear expression. H Schematic of brain regions involved in fear expression. A repeated-measures ANOVA in these regions revealed a trend towards an overall differences between apoE isoforms (p = 0.055, F(2,26) = 3.239). Z-scores of cFos immunoreactivity levels were seen in (I) the periaqueductal gray. J The pontine reticular nucleus, caudal part. K The bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. L The ventromedial hypothalamic nucleus. M The hypothalamic lateral zone. N the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus nerve. O The ventral tegmental area. P The paraventricular nucleus of the thalamus. Q The medulla and (R) the substantia innominata. Subsequent analysis identified apoE isoform-specific differences in the periaqueductal gray (p = 0.029, F(2,26) = 4.092). The Bonferroni’s post hoc test revealed that the difference was driven by E3 vs. E4 mice (p = 0.025). Data are presented in minimum to maximum boxplots. Each point represents an individual animal. *p < 0.05.
Lastly, we explored apoE isoform differences in brain regions important for fear expression (Fig. 3F). There was a trend toward a main effect of apoE isoform (p = 0.055; Fig. 3G), again with E4 mice trending towards lower cFos levels than E2 mice (p = 0.052). There were apoE differences in the periaqueductal gray (p = 0.029). E4 mice had less cFos immunoreactivity than E3 mice (p = 0.025; Fig. 3I). No differences were found in the pontine reticular nucleus, caudal part (Fig. 3J), bed nuclei of the stria terminalis (Fig. 3K), ventromedial hypothalamic nucleus (Fig. 3L), hypothalamic lateral zone (Fig. 3M), dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus nerve (Fig. 3N), ventral tegmental area (Fig. 3O), paraventricular nucleus of the thalamus (Fig. 3P), medulla (Fig. 3Q), or substantia innominata (Fig. 3R).
Taken together, these analyses suggest that apoE isoform-dependent differences in contextual fear circuitry are localized to areas important for somatosensation and contextual encoding, with lower cFos immunoreactivity levels in E4 mice.
ApoE isoform-dependent differences in co-activation of cFos immunoreactivity across regions important for contextual fear learning in young female mice
Obtaining data from the entire brain allowed us to analyze the co-activation of cFos cells within the fear circuitry. We calculated the Pearson’s coefficient for these regions and built covariance matrices categorized into the 4 sub-categories for each apoE isoform (Fig. 4A–C), then analyzed these matrices. The co-activation of cFos immunoreactivity in E4 mice was different from that in E3 mice (p = 0.0174; Fig. 4B, C). There was no difference between E2 and E3 mice (p = 0.208; Fig. 4A, B) or E2 and E4 mice (p = 0.2348). These results suggest that communication between fear-related brain regions may be specifically altered in young female E4 mice.
Pearson’s correlations were run for (A) E2, (B) E3, and (C) E4 mice to identify co-activation of cFos immunoreactivity across regions. Comparison of the correlation matrices revealed that E4 mice were different from E3 mice (p = 0.0174, \(\hat T\) = 3.9302). To identify the drivers of this difference, we mapped the regions that had r values > 0.7 or < −0.7 and generated chord diagrams for (D) E2, (E) E3, and (F) E4 animals. G A Venn diagram of the number of regions that were identified as having the strongest correlations in each of the genotypes, and their overlap.
To identify what correlations were driving these results, we examined all brain regions that had r-values above + 0.7 or below −0.7. Figure 4D–F show these analyses, which revealed a few major patterns between the apoE isoforms (Fig. 4G). First, there was only one overlap in regional correlations across the three isoforms (the agranular insular area and the substantia innominata). Second, E2 and E3 mice showed high correlations between contextual encoding regions, while E4 mice did not have any strong correlations between these regions. Third, E4 mice showed more strong negative correlations (5 total) than either E2 or E3 (2 each). Finally, E4 mice showed the least overlap with either E2 or E3.
Age, sex, and apoE isoform affect hippocampal DSBs and IEG expression at baseline and after fear conditioning
To assess possible hippocampus-specific changes, we used immunoreactivity and ChIP-ddPCR to measure DSBs and IEG expression before (behaviorally naïve) and after fear conditioning.
In behaviorally naïve females, there were differences in the amount of DNA pulled down by γH2Ax based on age (p < 0.001) and isoform (p < 0.001): middle-aged females had more γH2Ax-bound DNA than young females, and E4 more than E2 (p < 0.001) and E3 (p < 0.001; Fig. 5A) mice. Behaviorally naïve males similarly showed a difference based on age (p = 0.002) and an age by isoform interaction (p = 0.021). Middle-aged males also had higher levels than young males (Fig. 5B). Counts of the number of cells that contained 53BP1 foci in the CA3 also revealed a main effect of age (p = 0.028) in females (Fig. 5C). This was not seen in males (Fig. 5D). There was no effect of isoform and there were no effects in fear conditioned animals (Supplementary Tables 2 and 3). Together, these data indicate an increase in baseline DSBs with age that are partially dependent on sex and apoE isoform.
A Amount of DNA pulled down by γH2Ax ChIP in naïve females. There was an effect of age (p < 0.001, F(1,18) = 14.501) and genotype (p < 0.001, F(2,18) = 29.522). E4 mice had more γH2Ax-associated DNA than E2 (p < 0.0001) or E3 (p < 0.0001). B Amount of DNA pulled down by γH2Ax ChIP in naive males. There was an effect of age (p = 0.002, F(1,18) = 13.321) and an age-by-genotype interaction (p = 0.021, F(2,18) = 4.847). C Number of cells with 53BP1 foci in the CA3 of naïve females. Middle-aged animals had more 53BP1 than young animals (p = 0.029, F(1, 13) = 6.036). D Number of cells with 53BP1 foci in the CA3 of naïve males. There were no effects. E Relative Fos expression in the hippocampus of naïve females. Middle aged animals had more than young animals (p = 0.008, F(1,16) = 9.082). F Relative Fos expression in the hippocampus of naïve males. There were no effects. G Number of cells with 53BP1 foci in the CA3 of naïve females. There was an effect of age (p = 0.0003, F(1,13) = 24.16) and genotype (p = 0.045, F(2,13) = 3.975). H Number of cells with 53BP1 foci in the CA3 of naive males. There was an effect of genotype (p = 0.025, F(2, 13) = 4.941) and an age-by-genotype interaction (p = 0.023, F(2,13) = 5.096). Middle-aged E2 had more than middle-aged E3 (p = 0.004) and E4 (p = 0.007) mice. I cFos in the CA1 of young females. There was an effect of time post-training (p < 0.0001, F(2, 22) = 67.61); mice euthanized at 1.5 h had mor cFos than in behaviorally naïve or 24 h for all genotypes. J cFos in the CA1 of young males. There was an effect of time post-training (p = 0.015, F(2,20) = 5.233); post hoc testing did not reach significance. K cFos in the CA1 of middle-aged females. There was an effect of time post-training (p = 0.011, F(2,20) = 5.656) and a trend towards an effect of genotype (p = 0.063). Only E2 mice euthanized at 1.5 h had higher cFos than behaviorally naïve or 24 h. L cFos in the CA1 of middle-aged males. There was an effect of time post-training (p < 0.0001, F(2,21) = 29.38) and genotype (p < 0.0001, F(2,21) = 37.70) and a time-by-genotype interaction (p < 0.001, F(4,21) = 8.85). Only E2 mice euthanized at 1.5 h had higher cFos than behaviorally naive or 24 h. M Representative images of the CA1. White arrows indicate cFos+ cells, yellow arrows indicate 53BP1 + cells. Right: zoomed images of the white boxes. Data are presented as the mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ^p < 0.0001.
We next assessed baseline levels of IEG expression. Analysis of Fos levels normalized to BDNF (a control marker) again revealed a main effect of age (p = 0.008) in females (Fig. 5E) but not males (Fig. 5F). There was no effect of genotype, nor any differences in fear conditioned animals. There were no differences in Npas4 levels (Supplemental Table 2). There was a main effect of age in the number cFos+ cells in the CA3 region of females (p < 0.001; Fig. 5G). In contrast, there was a difference in cFos+ cells in behaviorally naive males based on isoform (p = 0.025) and there was an age by isoform interaction (p = 0.023; Fig. 5H). Middle-aged E2 males had more cFos+ cells than E3 (p = 0.004) and E4 (p = 0.007) males. These data point to a change in baseline IEG expression with aging that is dependent on sex and apoE isoform.
Subsequently, we analyzed cFos immunoreactivity in the CA1 and CA3 regions in three conditions (behaviorally naive, 1.5 h, or 24 h post-fear learning). Compared to behaviorally naïve and 24 h post-fear learning, the number of cFos+ cells in the CA1 region of young, female mice was higher at the 1.5 h time point in all isoforms (p < 0.0001; Fig. 5I). This effect was also seen in young, male mice (p = 0.015), though post hoc testing did not reveal significance between conditions within isoforms (Fig. 5J). The effect of condition was observed in middle-aged females (p = 0.011; Fig. 5K) and middle-aged males (p < 0.0001; Fig. 5L); in both sexes, E2 mice were the only to show increases in cFos+ cells at 1.5 h compared to behaviorally naïve and 24 h post-learning. Middle-aged males also showed an effect of genotype (p < 0.0001) and a time-by-genotype interaction (p < 0.001). Similar sex- and isoform-dependent patterns were observed in the CA3 region (Supplemental Fig. 1A–E). Representative CA1 images are shown in Fig. 5M.
Discussion
Our results provide insight into alterations to IEG signaling dependent on age, sex, and human apoE isoforms. Young E4 female mice displayed lower cFos immunoreactivity levels across regions important for somatosensation and contextual encoding, including the thalamus, sensory-motor cortex-related; entorhinal cortex (ETX); hippocampus; basolateral amygdala; and periaqueductal gray. E4 mice also showed increased time freezing during ISIs and altered co-activation of brain regions, with more negative correlations across regions than E3 mice. There was an increase in hippocampal DSB markers in middle-aged animals and female E4 mice, as well as age-, sex-, and apoE-dependent alterations to baseline and post-fear conditioning Fos levels. These changes in IEG expression may play a role in the apoE isoform-specific risks for neurological disorders.
The observed apoE differences in time freezing during the ISIs are in line with our earlier studies [42, 61]. E2 mice show impaired fear extinction compared to E3 and E4 mice [11, 41]. In humans, the E2 isoform has been relatively less-studied due to lower ε2 allele frequency (8%) in the general population [6]. Heterozygous E2-carriers show a decreased risk to develop LOAD and increased longevity [62,63,64,65], though E2 is associated with other neurological diseases, including higher rates of PTSD, PTSD symptom severity as measured by the Clinician-Administered PTSD-scale, and alcohol use disorders [10, 11, 66]. Impaired fear extinction in mice closely matches one of the primary symptoms of PTSD—the persistence of traumatic memories. As there is a lack of data on fear acquisition related to apoE isoform, our results add to knowledge about distinctions during a highly salient learning event.
Compared to E3, E4 has also been linked to PTSD. E4-homozygous combat veterans with high levels of combat exposure have a higher lifetime prevalence of PTSD and increased PTSD severity as measured by the Davidson Trauma Scale [67], a self-report survey [68, 69]. While highly reliable [70], the different rating scales used could partially account for the distinct apoE isoform-related results. Moreover, there is no increased risk for PTSD in E4-carriers with low combat exposure. The neuropathological impact of traumatic brain injury (TBI) is shown to be worse in E4 carrying people [71, 72] and mice [73,74,75]. This includes increased tau phosphorylation, accumulation of amyloid β (Aβ), and inflammation. Thus, the interaction between combat exposure (i.e., repeated TBIs) with apoE isoform may contribute to the divergent PTSD diagnoses and presentations. PTSD also might have a bi-directional relationship with dementia: PTSD increases the risk for dementia, and dementia increases the risk for delayed-onset PTSD [76]. ApoE isoform was not assessed in that study. The interactions between life experience, sex, and apoE isoform on PTSD and AD development are complex; our results that middle-aged E2 mice maintain high levels of post-training cFos activation (while E3 and E4 fail to do so) add clarity to possible underlying neural mechanisms.
Dysregulation in IEGs has been implicated in many neurological diseases, including increased circulating Egr1 in schizophrenia [77, 78], increased Arc in the amygdala in major depressive disorder [79, 80], and increased cFos and cJun in the hippocampus and amygdala in AD [81,82,83]. Rodent research has highlighted dysregulation of IEGs in AD and PTSD models. Young (4–6-month-old) mice carrying amyloid precursor protein (APP) mutations—an autosomal dominant cause of AD—have increased cognitive impairments and hippocampal delta-FosB levels, leading to downregulation of cFos [84]. APP knock out mice display high baseline levels of Egr1, cFos, and BDNF in the prefrontal cortex [85], which might interfere with the expected IEG increase following novelty, suggesting that APP is important for proper IEG regulation. PTSD models show elevated cFos immunoreactivity in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, paraventricular hypothalamus, paraventricular thalamus, basolateral amygdala (BLA), central amygdala, medial amygdala, and periaqueductal gray (PAG) [86, 87]. The BLA and PAG were two regions we identified with apoE-dependent differences, with E4 mice having less cFos immunoreactivity. It is possible that the higher cFos levels in E2 and/or lower levels in E4 mice could contribute to different fear expression—namely, E4 mice freezing more during training and E2 mice exhibiting impaired extinction [11].
Upstream, apoE influences APP transcription via phosphorylation of cFos [30]. In male-derived iPSCs, E4 stimulates phosphorylation of ERK1 and cFos, increases APP levels, and increases Aβ secretion more than E3, and E3 does so more than E2 [30]. RNAi-mediated inhibition of cFos decreases APP levels [30]. There is also a stepwise decrease from E4 to E3 to E2 in activation of synapse-building genes via CREB phosphorylation [88]. These might appear paradoxical to our current findings, however the iPSC experiments involved exogenous apoE in a reduced system. E4 carriers have lower apoE levels than non-E4 carriers throughout the body independently of AD diagnosis [89,90,91]. E4 mice on a high fat diet (HFD) did not show increases in cFos and Arc in the hippocampus, cortex, or hypothalamus, while E3 mice on a HFD did [29]. Thus, it is conceivable that lower apoE levels in E4 carriers lead to less IEG expression. Remarkably, there was higher freezing in E4 mice despite generally lower cFos expression, though alterations in brain activation have been reported in E4 carriers in the absence of cognitive differences [14, 20]. Additionally, young adult E2 carriers had larger hippocampal gray matter and relied more on hippocampus-dependent strategies to solve a navigation task [92]. We found apoE-dependent differences in hippocampal cFos+ cells in young and middle-aged mice, where E2 mice generally had more hippocampal cFos immunoreactivity. Our results suggest E2-specific increased hippocampal activation during fear learning, complementing the human findings.
The data from human imaging studies are in line with the distinctions we observed when we analyzed regional correlations. There was a difference between the correlation matrices of E3 and E4 animals only. When we examined the strongest correlations (r > 0.7 or r < −0.7), there was a single overlapping correlation between all 3 isoforms. This finding—that the 3 isoforms are more separate than similar for this measure—might relate to the differential risks associated with E2 and E4. Moreover, E4 mice did not show any strong positive correlations within the contextual encoding regions and had more strong negative correlations than E2 or E3 mice. This was clear when looking at the ETX and hippocampus correlation: E4 mice displayed a negative correlation (−0.621), while E2 and E3 mice displayed positive correlations (0.71 and 0.906, respectively). Analysis of functional connectivity involving the ETX in an older human population reported similar results: E2 carriers showed the strongest functional connectivity of the ETX with the frontal and middle temporal gyrus; E3 carrier with the postcentral gyrus, inferior parietal lobule, and insula (among others); and E4 carriers with the precuneus, angular gyrus, superior parietal lobule, and posterior cingulate (among others) [93]. This population included healthy controls and patients with MCI and showed that E4 alterations in functional connectivity were associated with increased speed of dementia onset, while the alterations in E2 carriers were associated with protection.
In humans, there are increases in neuronal γH2Ax and 53BP1 in brains from patients with MCI or AD compared to healthy, age-matched controls [33]. Increased oxidative stress, which induces DSBs, is seen in typical and diseased aging [94,95,96,97,98]. Hippocampal 8-Hydroxydeoxyguanosine is higher, while 53BP1 and PTEN (another DNA repair protein) are decreased in post-mortem tissue from diagnosed AD patients, suggesting impaired DSB repair [95]. Age is also associated with increased DNA damage in the frontal cortex, leading to down-regulation of genes important for synaptic plasticity [96]. Decreased DNA repair—specifically in accurate 53BP1 recruitment—was also observed in aged human cell cultures [94,95,96,97,98]. Here, we found an increase in hippocampal γH2Ax and 53BP1 levels in middle-aged animals and an increase in relative Fos immunoreactivity, indicative of increased aberrant signaling with aging. This result was more pronounced in E4 females, paralleling others’ findings that age, sex, and E4 synergistically increase risk for AD [99].
We did not observe increases in DSBs post-training, likely due to differences in γH2Ax and 53BP1 function and kinetics, nor increases in DSB-associated IEGs after fear conditioning as reported by others [52, 100]. Both Navabpour et al. [52] and Stott et al. [100] observed fear conditioning-induced increases in Npas4, but not Fos. This discrepancy with our study could be due to differences in methodology and/or mechanisms of distinct IEGs. However, there was an increase in γH2Ax-associated IEG expression at baseline in middle-aged animals, and in E4 females. These results mirror the previously reported data showing that baseline levels of γH2Ax are higher in APP mutant mice [32]. Notably, that study involved 4–7-month-old mice and collapsed sexes for analysis, while we assessed young (3 months) and middle-aged (12 months) mice without collapsing sexes. Another study reported increased hippocampal γH2Ax, but not 53BP1 levels, in both young (5 months) and aged (16 months) APP/PS1 mice, in line with our results [101].
Altogether, these data highlight that apoE and sex interact and have distinct age-dependent effects on DSBs and IEG expression. These effects, specifically in E4 carriers, could be a driving factor in enhanced E4-specific risk for neurological diseases.
Data availability
All data is freely available upon request.
References
Piaceri I, Nacmias B, Sorbi S. Genetics of familial and sporadic Alzheimer’s disease. Front Biosci (Elite Ed). 2013;5:167–77.
Guerreiro R, Bras J. The age factor in Alzheimer’s disease. Genome Med. 2015;7:106.
Scheyer O, Rahman A, Hristov H, Berkowitz C, Isaacson RS, Diaz Brinton R, et al. Female sex and Alzheimer’s Risk: The menopause connection. J Prev Alzheimers Dis. 2018;5:225–30.
Brookmeyer R, Johnson E, Ziegler-Graham K, Arrighi HM. Forecasting the global burden of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2007;3:186–91.
Raber J, Huang Y, Ashford JW. ApoE genotype accounts for the vast majority of AD risk and AD pathology. Neurobiol Aging. 2004;25:641–50.
Farrer LA, Cupples LA, Haines JL, Hyman B, Kukull WA, Mayeux R, et al. Effects of age, sex, and ethnicity on the association between apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer disease. A meta-analysis. APOE and Alzheimer Disease Meta Analysis Consortium. JAMA. 1997;278:1349–56.
Suchy-Dicey A, Howard B, Longstreth WT, Jr., Reiman EM, Buchwald D. APOE genotype, hippocampus, and cognitive markers of Alzheimer’s disease in American Indians: Data from the Strong Heart Study. Alzheimers Dement. 2022;18:2518–26.
Beydoun MA, Weiss J, Beydoun HA, Hossain S, Maldonado AI, Shen B, et al. Race, APOE genotypes, and cognitive decline among middle-aged urban adults. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2021;13:120.
Torres ERS, Luo J, Boehnlein JK, Towns D, Kinzie JD, DeBarber AE, et al. Apolipoprotein E Isoform-specific changes related to stress and trauma exposure. Transl Psychiatry. 2022;12:125.
Kim TY, Chung HG, Shin HS, Kim SJ, Choi JH, Chung MY, et al. Apolipoprotein E gene polymorphism, alcohol use, and their interactions in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2013;30:1194–201.
Johnson LA, Zuloaga DG, Bidiman E, Marzulla T, Weber S, Wahbeh H, et al. ApoE2 Exaggerates PTSD-Related Behavioral, Cognitive, and Neuroendocrine Alterations. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:2443–53.
Ostendorf BN, Bilanovic J, Adaku N, Tafreshian KN, Tavora B, Vaughan RD, et al. Common germline variants of the human APOE gene modulate melanoma progression and survival. Nat Med. 2020;26:1048–53.
Acevedo SF, Piper BJ, Craytor MJ, Benice TS, Raber J. Apolipoprotein E4 and sex affect neurobehavioral performance in primary school children. Pediatr Res. 2010;67:293–9.
Reiman EM, Chen K, Alexander GE, Caselli RJ, Bandy D, Osborne D, et al. Functional brain abnormalities in young adults at genetic risk for late-onset Alzheimer’s dementia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101:284–9.
Ferretti MT, Iulita MF, Cavedo E, Chiesa PA, Schumacher Dimech A, Santuccione, et al. Sex differences in Alzheimer disease - the gateway to precision medicine. Nat Rev Neurol. 2018;14:457–69.
Toro CA, Zhang L, Cao J, Cai D. Sex differences in Alzheimer’s disease: Understanding the molecular impact. Brain Res. 2019;1719:194–207.
Tower J. Sex-specific regulation of aging and apoptosis. Mech Ageing Dev. 2006;127:705–18.
Beckman KB, Ames BN. The free radical theory of aging matures. Physiol Rev. 1998;78:547–81.
Berchtold NC, Cribbs DH, Coleman PD, Rogers J, Head E, Kim R, et al. Gene expression changes in the course of normal brain aging are sexually dimorphic. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:15605–10.
Bookheimer SY, Strojwas MH, Cohen MS, Saunders AM, Pericak-Vance MA, Mazziotta JC, et al. Patterns of brain activation in people at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. N. Engl J Med. 2000;343:450–6.
Demmer J, Dragunow M, Lawlor PA, Mason SE, Leah JD, Abraham WC, et al. Differential expression of immediate early genes after hippocampal long-term potentiation in awake rats. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 1993;17:279–86.
Minatohara K, Akiyoshi M, Okuno H. Role of immediate-early genes in synaptic plasticity and neuronal ensembles underlying the memory trace. Front Mol Neurosci. 2015;8:78.
Cole AJ, Saffen DW, Baraban JM, Worley PF. Rapid increase of an immediate early gene messenger RNA in hippocampal neurons by synaptic NMDA receptor activation. Nature. 1989;340:474–6.
Guzowski JF, Setlow B, Wagner EK, McGaugh JL. Experience-dependent gene expression in the rat hippocampus after spatial learning: a comparison of the immediate-early genes Arc, c-fos, and zif268. J Neurosci. 2001;21:5089–98.
Healy S, Khan P, Davie JR. Immediate early response genes and cell transformation. Pharm Ther. 2013;137:64–77.
Kovacs KJ. c-Fos as a transcription factor: A stressful (re)view from a functional map. Neurochem Int. 1998;33:287–97.
Denny CA, Kheirbek MA, Alba EL, Tanaka KF, Brachman RA, Laughman KB, et al. Hippocampal memory traces are differentially modulated by experience, time, and adult neurogenesis. Neuron. 2014;83:189–201.
Liu X, Ramirez S, Pang PT, Puryear CB, Govindarajan A, Deisseroth K, et al. Optogenetic stimulation of a hippocampal engram activates fear memory recall. Nature. 2012;484:381–5.
Jones NS, Watson KQ, Rebeck GW. High-fat diet increases gliosis and immediate early gene expression in APOE3 mice, but not APOE4 mice. J Neuroinflammation. 2021;18:214.
Huang YA, Zhou B, Wernig M, Sudhof TC. ApoE2. ApoE3, and ApoE4 differentially stimulate APP transcription and abeta secretion. Cell. 2017;168:427–41.e421.
Madabhushi R, Gao F, Pfenning AR, Pan L, Yamakawa S, Seo J, et al. Activity-induced dna breaks govern the expression of neuronal early-response genes. Cell. 2015;161:1592–605.
Suberbielle E, Sanchez PE, Kravitz AV, Wang X, Ho K, Eilertson K, et al. Physiologic brain activity causes DNA double-strand breaks in neurons, with exacerbation by amyloid-beta. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16:613–21.
Shanbhag NM, Evans MD, Mao W, Nana AL, Seeley WW, Adame A, et al. Early neuronal accumulation of DNA double strand breaks in Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neuropathol Commun. 2019;7:77.
Maren S. Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:897–931.
Fendt M, Fanselow MS. The neuroanatomical and neurochemical basis of conditioned fear. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1999;23:743–60.
Tovote P, Fadok JP, Luthi A. Neuronal circuits for fear and anxiety. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16:317–31.
Conejo NM, Gonzalez-Pardo H, Lopez M, Cantora R, Arias JL. Induction of c-Fos expression in the mammillary bodies, anterior thalamus and dorsal hippocampus after fear conditioning. Brain Res Bull. 2007;74:172–7.
Strekalova T, Zorner B, Zacher C, Sadovska G, Herdegen T, Gass P. Memory retrieval after contextual fear conditioning induces c-Fos and JunB expression in CA1 hippocampus. Genes Brain Behav. 2003;2:3–10.
Tanaka KZ, Pevzner A, Hamidi AB, Nakazawa Y, Graham J, Wiltgen BJ. Cortical representations are reinstated by the hippocampus during memory retrieval. Neuron. 2014;84:347–54.
Fullana MA, Harrison BJ, Soriano-Mas C, Vervliet B, Cardoner N, Avila-Parcet A, et al. Neural signatures of human fear conditioning: an updated and extended meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Mol Psychiatry. 2016;21:500–8.
Olsen RH, Agam M, Davis MJ, Raber J. ApoE isoform-dependent deficits in extinction of contextual fear conditioning. Genes Brain Behav. 2012;11:806–12.
Villasana LE, Weber S, Akinyeke T, Raber J. Genotype differences in anxiety and fear learning and memory of WT and ApoE4 mice associated with enhanced generation of hippocampal reactive oxygen species. J Neurochem. 2016;138:896–908.
Kim Y, Venkataraju KU, Pradhan K, Mende C, Taranda J, Turaga SC, et al. Mapping social behavior-induced brain activation at cellular resolution in the mouse. Cell Rep. 2015;10:292–305.
Zuloaga DG, Iancu OD, Weber S, Etzel D, Marzulla T, Stewart B, et al. Enhanced functional connectivity involving the ventromedial hypothalamus following methamphetamine exposure. Front Neurosci. 2015;9:326.
Boutros SW, Zimmerman B, Nagy SC, Lee JS, Perez R, Raber J. Amifostine (WR-2721) Mitigates Cognitive Injury Induced by Heavy Ion Radiation in Male Mice and Alters Behavior and Brain Connectivity. Front Physiol. 2021;12:770502.
Sullivan PM, Mezdour H, Aratani Y, Knouff C, Najib J, Reddick RL, et al. Targeted replacement of the mouse apolipoprotein E gene with the common human APOE3 allele enhances diet-induced hypercholesterolemia and atherosclerosis. J Biol Chem. 1997;272:17972–80.
Sullivan PM, Mezdour H, Quarfordt SH, Maeda N. Type III hyperlipoproteinemia and spontaneous atherosclerosis in mice resulting from gene replacement of mouse Apoe with human Apoe*2. J Clin Invest. 1998;102:130–5.
Knouff C, Hinsdale ME, Mezdour H, Altenburg MK, Watanabe M, Quarfordt SH, et al. Apo E structure determines VLDL clearance and atherosclerosis risk in mice. J Clin Invest. 1999;103:1579–86.
Zuloaga DG, Johnson LA, Agam M, Raber J. Sex differences in activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis by methamphetamine. J Neurochem. 2014;129:495–508.
Ceccatelli S, Villar MJ, Goldstein M, Hokfelt T. Expression of c-Fos immunoreactivity in transmitter-characterized neurons after stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1989;86:9569–73.
Mirza-Aghazadeh-Attari M, Mohammadzadeh A, Yousefi B, Mihanfar A, Karimian A, Majidinia M. 53BP1: A key player of DNA damage response with critical functions in cancer. DNA Repair (Amst). 2019;73:110–9.
Navabpour S, Rogers J, McFadden T, Jarome TJ. DNA Double-Strand Breaks Are a Critical Regulator of Fear Memory Reconsolidation. Int J Mol Sci. 2020;21:8995–9009.
Kugelman T, Zuloaga DG, Weber S, Raber J. Post-training gamma irradiation-enhanced contextual fear memory associated with reduced neuronal activation of the infralimbic cortex. Behav Brain Res. 2016;298:1–11.
Schaser AJ, Osterberg VR, Dent SE, Stackhouse TL, Wakeham CM, Boutros SW, et al. Alpha-synuclein is a DNA binding protein that modulates DNA repair with implications for Lewy body disorders. Sci Rep. 2019;9:10919.
Hartley GA, Okhovat M, O’Neill RJ, Carbone L. Comparative Analyses of Gibbon Centromeres Reveal Dynamic Genus-Specific Shifts in Repeat Composition. Mol Biol Evol. 2021;38:3972–92.
Taylor SC, Laperriere G, Germain H. Droplet Digital PCR versus qPCR for gene expression analysis with low abundant targets: from variable nonsense to publication quality data. Sci Rep. 2017;7:2409.
Renier N, Adams EL, Kirst C, Wu Z, Azevedo R, Kohl J, et al. Mapping of brain activity by automated volume analysis of immediate early genes. Cell. 2016;165:1789–802.
Chang J, Zhou W, Zhou WX, Wang L. Comparing large covariance matrices under weak conditions on the dependence structure and its application to gene clustering. Biometrics. 2017;73:31–41.
Lin KZ, Liu H, Roeder K. Covariance-based sample selection for heterogeneous data: Applications to gene expression and autism risk gene detection. J Am Stat Assoc. 2021;116:54–67.
Chernozhukov V, Chetverikov D, Kato K. Gaussian approximations and multiplier bootstrap for maxima of sums of high-dimensional random vectors. Ann Stat. 2013;41:2786–819.
Holden S, Kundu P, Torres ERS, Sudhakar R, Krenik D, Grygoryev D, et al. Apolipoprotein E Isoform-Dependent Effects on Human Amyloid Precursor Protein/Abeta-Induced Behavioral Alterations and Cognitive Impairments and Insoluble Cortical Abeta42 Levels. Front Aging Neurosci. 2022;14:767558.
Deelen J, Beekman M, Uh HW, Helmer Q, Kuningas M, Christiansen L, et al. Genome-wide association study identifies a single major locus contributing to survival into old age; the APOE locus revisited. Aging Cell. 2011;10:686–98.
Nebel A, Kleindorp R, Caliebe A, Nothnagel M, Blanche H, Junge O, et al. A genome-wide association study confirms APOE as the major gene influencing survival in long-lived individuals. Mech Ageing Dev. 2011;132:324–30.
Li Z, Shue F, Zhao N, Shinohara M, Bu G. APOE2: protective mechanism and therapeutic implications for Alzheimer’s disease. Mol Neurodegener. 2020;15:63.
Reiman EM, Arboleda-Velasquez JF, Quiroz YT, Huentelman MJ, Beach TG, Caselli RJ, et al. Exceptionally low likelihood of Alzheimer’s dementia in APOE2 homozygotes from a 5,000-person neuropathological study. Nat Commun. 2020;11:667.
Weathers FW, Bovin MJ, Lee DJ, Sloan DM, Schnurr PP, Kaloupek DG, et al. The Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-5 (CAPS-5): Development and initial psychometric evaluation in military veterans. Psychol Assess. 2018;30:383–95.
Kimbrel NA, Hauser MA, Garrett M, Ashley-Koch A, Liu Y, Dennis MF, et al. EFFECT OF THE APOE epsilon4 ALLELE AND COMBAT EXPOSURE ON PTSD AMONG IRAQ/AFGHANISTAN-ERA VETERANS. Depress Anxiety. 2015;32:307–15.
Davidson JR, Book SW, Colket JT, Tupler LA, Roth S, David D, et al. Assessment of a new self-rating scale for post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol Med. 1997;27:153–60.
Meltzer-Brody S, Churchill E, Davidson JR. Derivation of the SPAN, a brief diagnostic screening test for post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res. 1999;88:63–70.
McDonald SD, Beckham JC, Morey RA, Calhoun PS. The validity and diagnostic efficiency of the Davidson Trauma Scale in military veterans who have served since September 11th, 2001. J Anxiety Disord. 2009;23:247–55.
Mayeux R, Ottman R, Maestre G, Ngai C, Tang MX, Ginsberg H, et al. Synergistic effects of traumatic head injury and apolipoprotein-epsilon 4 in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1995;45:555–7.
Halliday MR, Rege SV, Ma Q, Zhao Z, Miller CA, Winkler EA, et al. Accelerated pericyte degeneration and blood–brain barrier breakdown in apolipoprotein E4 carriers with Alzheimer’s disease. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2016;36:216–27.
Cao J, Gaamouch FE, Meabon JS, Meeker KD, Zhu L, Zhong MB, et al. ApoE4-associated phospholipid dysregulation contributes to development of Tau hyper-phosphorylation after traumatic brain injury. Sci Rep. 2017;7:11372.
Giarratana AO, Zheng C, Reddi S, Teng SL, Berger D, Adler D, et al. APOE4 genetic polymorphism results in impaired recovery in a repeated mild traumatic brain injury model and treatment with Bryostatin-1 improves outcomes. Sci Rep. 2020;10:19919.
Washington PM, Burns MP. The Effect of the APOE4 gene on accumulation of abeta40 after brain injury cannot be reversed by increasing apoE4 Protein. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2016;75:770–8.
Desmarais P, Weidman D, Wassef A, Bruneau M-A, Friedland J, Bajsarowicz P, et al. The interplay between post-traumatic stress disorder and dementia: a systematic review. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2020;28:48–60.
Kurian SM, Le-Niculescu H, Patel SD, Bertram D, Davis J, Dike C, et al. Identification of blood biomarkers for psychosis using convergent functional genomics. Mol Psychiatry. 2011;16:37–58.
Cattane N, Minelli A, Milanesi E, Maj C, Bignotti S, Bortolomasi M, et al. Altered gene expression in schizophrenia: findings from transcriptional signatures in fibroblasts and blood. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0116686.
Lee RS, Hermens DF, Porter MA, Redoblado-Hodge MA. A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in first-episode Major Depressive Disorder. J Affect Disord. 2012;140:113–24.
Ons S, Rotllant D, Marin-Blasco IJ, Armario A. Immediate-early gene response to repeated immobilization: Fos protein and arc mRNA levels appear to be less sensitive than c-fos mRNA to adaptation. Eur J Neurosci. 2010;31:2043–52.
Zhang P, Hirsch EC, Damier P, Duyckaerts C, Javoy-Agid F. c-fos protein-like immunoreactivity: distribution in the human brain and over-expression in the hippocampus of patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroscience. 1992;46:9–21.
Anderson AJ, Cummings BJ, Cotman CW. Increased immunoreactivity for Jun- and Fos-related proteins in Alzheimer’s disease: association with pathology. Exp Neurol. 1994;125:286–95.
Marcus DL, Strafaci JA, Miller DC, Masia S, Thomas CG, Rosman J, et al. Quantitative neuronal c-fos and c-jun expression in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 1998;19:393–400.
Corbett BF, You JC, Zhang X, Pyfer MS, Tosi U, Iascone DM, et al. DeltaFosB Regulates Gene Expression and Cognitive Dysfunction in a Mouse Model of Alzheimer’s Disease. Cell Rep. 2017;20:344–55.
Hendrickx A, Pierrot N, Tasiaux B, Schakman O, Kienlen-Campard P, De Smet C, et al. Epigenetic regulations of immediate early genes expression involved in memory formation by the amyloid precursor protein of Alzheimer disease. PLoS One. 2014;9:e99467.
Kung JC, Chen TC, Shyu BC, Hsiao S, Huang AC. Anxiety- and depressive-like responses and c-fos activity in preproenkephalin knockout mice: oversensitivity hypothesis of enkephalin deficit-induced posttraumatic stress disorder. J Biomed Sci. 2010;17:29.
Azevedo H, Ferreira M, Mascarello A, Osten P, Guimaraes CRW. Brain-wide mapping of c-fos expression in the single prolonged stress model and the effects of pretreatment with ACH-000029 or prazosin. Neurobiol Stress. 2020;13:100226.
Huang YA, Zhou B, Nabet AM, Wernig M, Sudhof TC. Differential Signaling Mediated by ApoE2, ApoE3, and ApoE4 in Human Neurons Parallels Alzheimer’s Disease Risk. J Neurosci. 2019;39:7408–27.
Riddell DR, Zhou H, Atchison K, Warwick HK, Atkinson PJ, Jefferson J, et al. Impact of apolipoprotein E (ApoE) polymorphism on brain ApoE levels. J Neurosci. 2008;28:11445–53.
Ramaswamy G, Xu Q, Huang Y, Weisgraber KH. Effect of domain interaction on apolipoprotein E levels in mouse brain. J Neurosci. 2005;25:10658–63.
Rezeli M, Zetterberg H, Blennow K, Brinkmalm A, sLaurell T, Hansson O, et al. Quantification of total apolipoprotein E and its specific isoforms in cerebrospinal fluid and blood in Alzheimer’s disease and other neurodegenerative diseases. EuPA Open Proteom. 2015;8:137–43.
Konishi K, Bhat V, Banner H, Poirier J, Joober R, Bohbot VD. APOE2 Is Associated with Spatial Navigational Strategies and Increased Gray Matter in the Hippocampus. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10:349.
Chen J, Shu H, Wang Z, Liu D, Shi Y, Xu L, et al. Protective effect of APOE epsilon 2 on intrinsic functional connectivity of the entorhinal cortex is associated with better episodic memory in elderly individuals with risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease. Oncotarget. 2016;7:58789–801.
Shackelford DA. DNA end joining activity is reduced in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2006;27:596–605.
Silva AR, Santos AC, Farfel JM, Grinberg LT, Ferretti RE, Campos AH, et al. Repair of oxidative DNA damage, cell-cycle regulation and neuronal death may influence the clinical manifestation of Alzheimer’s disease. PLoS One. 2014;9:e99897.
Lu T, Pan Y, Kao SY, Li C, Kohane I, Chan J, et al. Gene regulation and DNA damage in the ageing human brain. Nature. 2004;429:883–91.
Jacobsen E, Beach T, Shen Y, Li R, Chang Y. Deficiency of the Mre11 DNA repair complex in Alzheimer’s disease brains. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 2004;128:1–7.
Anglada T, Genesca A, Martin M. Age-associated deficient recruitment of 53BP1 in G1 cells directs DNA double-strand break repair to BRCA1/CtIP-mediated DNA-end resection. Aging (Albany NY). 2020;12:24872–93.
Neu SC, Pa J, Kukull W, Beekly D, Kuzma A, Gangadharan P, et al. Apolipoprotein E Genotype and Sex Risk Factors for Alzheimer Disease: A Meta-analysis. JAMA Neurol. 2017;74:1178–89.
Stott RT, Kritsky O, Tsai LH. Profiling DNA break sites and transcriptional changes in response to contextual fear learning. PLoS One. 2021;16:e0249691.
Yu H, Harrison FE, Xia F. Altered DNA repair; an early pathogenic pathway in Alzheimer’s disease and obesity. Sci Rep. 2018;8:5600.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Dr. Pavel Osten and Certerra, Inc (acquired by Certego Therapeutics); the OHSU Advanced Light Microscopy Core; Dr. Jessica Minnier; Dr. Lucia Carbone and the OHSU Epigenetics Core; Dr. Thuy Ngo, Dr. Paul Spellman, and Dmytro Grygorev for guidance on the ChIP-ddPCR analysis; Dr. Claudia Wiese for guidance and expertise on DSBs; and Chloe McQuesten and Jorian Blacker for their help with the whole-brain data analyses and cell counting. Funding for SWB and this project came from NSF GRFP, NIA T32 AG055378, F31 AG067629, the American Federation for Aging Research, and a Knight Cancer Institute Ph.D. Scholars COVID Relief Fund. Support for BZ was provided by a Beckman Institute Postdoctoral Fellowship (University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign), with funding provided by the Arnold and Mabel Beckman Foundation, NIH R90 AT008924, and a Burroughs Wellcome Fund Collaborative Research Travel Grant (1018797). This work was partially supported by NIH RF1 AG059088, R21 AG065914, R01 NS102227, and RF1 NS123457. Biorender.com software was used to generate parts of some figures.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
SWB, VKU, and JR designed the experiments. SWB performed behavioral experiments, collected tissue and performed tissue experiments, analyzed data, and performed statistical analyses. BZ assisted with the analyses of the whole-brain data. SCN assisted with the behavioral experiments, tissue collection, and cell counting. All authors contributed to the writing of the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Boutros, S.W., Zimmerman, B., Nagy, S.C. et al. Age, sex, and apolipoprotein E isoform alter contextual fear learning, neuronal activation, and baseline DNA damage in the hippocampus. Mol Psychiatry (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-023-01966-8
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-023-01966-8