Introduction

Top-down dysregulation of information processing is a core mechanism underlying many psychiatric disorders. For example, in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)–defined mainly by non-emotional symptoms including impulsivity, hyperactivity, and inattention–a core theory has evolved around deficits in executive functions (EF) [1, 2]. EF consists of a set of cognitive control processes associated with top-down regulatory systems in the brain [1, 2]. In line with this hypothesis, reported ADHD-symptoms have been associated with poor results in EF tests although the relation has been surprisingly weak and unspecific [3].

In addition to ADHD, there are also a set of psychiatric disorders which are characterized by top-down dysregulation in emotional processing. These include Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) and Antisocial Personality Disorder (ASPD) in adults, and Conduct Disorder (CD) in children and adolescents [2, 4,5,6,7]. They all comprise emotional instability symptoms (partially overlapping with the concept of emotional lability) such as rapidly shifting emotional states and affective behavior. Also, altered emotional processing and regulation have been noted in these states [2]. Collectively, these disorders may thus serve as valuable models for studying emotional dysregulation. ADHD and emotional instability disorders show substantial symptom overlap and are highly comorbid [8,9,10,11]. A shared genetic contribution [12] has also been suggested. The overlapping relation between ADHD and emotional instability disorders has made it problematic to isolate behaviors and mechanisms specific to each condition.

A model encompassing both ADHD and emotional instability disorders has recently been proposed [2]. The model builds conceptually on the research domain criteria (RDoC) approach [2, 13] and suggests that both forms of disorders include similar top-down dysregulation—with the difference being whether non-emotional or emotional regulatory processes are predominantly affected. An important aspect of the model is that both non-emotional and emotional regulatory capacities are likely to be dimensional and vary among individuals even on a subclinical level within a healthy population [14,15,16]. In addition, some forms of ADHD may show a strong degree of subclinical emotional dysregulation in the absence of other comorbidities [2, 17,18,19].

In children and adolescents, a clinical relation between ADHD and CD has been noted [6]. Although CD involves antisocial behaviors, which are not observed in pure ADHD, both disorders share common symptomatology including problems with sustained attention, cognitive switching, and inhibition [6]. The fact that these two disorders are highly comorbid [11] has often not been considered and might have confounded the findings of previous studies. Thus, it is still not entirely clear which of the features are specific to each disorder. Some studies have compared patients with pure ADHD and CD (without co-morbidities), proposing the presence of a few disorder-specific behaviors and brain functional characteristics [6]. However, it has been suggested that the degree of ADHD symptoms is related to the degree of CD symptoms even on a sub-clinical level [2]. It is thus possible that studies on ADHD still may be confounded by CD features and vice versa. It is therefore of importance to consider both symptom dimensions simultaneously even on a sub-clinical level in relation to behavioral and neurobiological features. An effective approach to better understand the specific behavioral and neurobiological features in these two dimensions is to study the different traits in community populations.

Impaired performance on several EF tests in ADHD [20,21,22] are associated with deficits in top-down regulatory systems of non-emotional processes [2, 17, 18, 23]. Especially EF tests that measure inhibition, switching, set-shifting, updating, and sustained attention (including Stop Signal test, Stroop test, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and Continuous Performance Test) have been shown to be altered in ADHD [3]. Further, working memory (WM), which partially consists of several EF components, has shown to be significantly affected in ADHD [3]. Functional imaging studies have consistently revealed networks underlying non-emotional or “cool” EF processes involving dorsolateral and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dl/dmPFC), ventrolateral PFC (vlPFC), and caudal anterior cingulate cortex (cACC), which work in conjunction with parietal and subcortical regions such as the striatum [24, 25]. These networks interact with the dopamine system and other specific neuromodulatory systems [2]. The aforementioned brain regions (including dl/dmPFC, vlPFC, and cACC) are known to be less activated during EF tasks in ADHD patients vs. controls [26,27,28,29]. On a brain structure level it has been shown that the “ADHD-brain” matures significantly slower than for healthy individuals–especially in the prefrontal and anterior cingulate areas–with an delay of approximately three years (when medians for the two groups were comapred) [30]. Although some of these structural differences seem to disappear in adulthood [31], some studies indicate smaller dl/dmPFC and cACC volume also in adults with ADHD [32]. Recent meta-analyses [29, 33, 34] have also shown decreased gray matter volume (GMV) in insula, ventromedial prefrontal/medial orbitofrontal cortex, rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC), and occipital lobe and deep structures such as amygdala, basal ganglia (including nucleus accumbens), and hippocampus in individuals with ADHD. A major drawback in most previous brain imaging studies on ADHD (including the above mentioned meta-analyses) is that they have often not controlled for other sub-clinical symptoms such as traits of CD.

In analogy with ADHD, it has been suggested that emotional instability disorders show a lack of regulatory capacity for emotional processes including emotional aspects of EF, i.e., “hot” EF [2, 4,5,6,7]. Although there is a large overlap between emotional and non-emotional regulatory processes, there seems to be some specificity, and it has been suggested that lateral orbitofrontal cortex (lOFC) and rACC are more involved in the emotional sub-components of top-down regulation [2]. In line with this, it has been observed that these brain regions are less activated in emotional regulation and “hot” EF tasks in emotional instability disorders as compared to controls [35,36,37,38,39,40,41]. Moreover, several well-powered studies have indicated a smaller volume in lOFC and rACC in patients with these disorders [42,43,44]. Specifically for CD, recent large single-site studies and meta-analyses suggest a smaller GMV in a number of prefrontal structures including lOFC, ventrolateral PFC (vlPFC), medial prefrontal extending into rACC but also other structures involved in emotional processing such as insula and amygdala [45, 46]. However, these studies have rarely controlled for non-emotional dysregulation or ADHD symptoms.

In the present study, we analyzed data from a large sample of adolescents (N = 1093) from the IMAGEN project [47]. While the general relationship between brain structure and ADHD traits has been presented in a recent VBM analysis [48], here we aim to specifically disentangle effects of ADHD and CD traits on both brain structure and behavioral domains. We hypothesized that both ADHD and CD traits are inversely related to common overlapping brain areas including PFC and ACC and in addition will show specific structural signatures unique to the traits. We expected: (1) that ADHD trait will be negatively related to cortical volume in regions involved in non-emotional top-down regulation and cool EF (e.g., dl/dmPFC and cACC) even when controlling for CD trait. (2) Negative correlations between CD trait and cortical volume in regions involved in emotional regulation (e.g., lOFC and rACC) even after correcting for ADHD trait. We primarily used a region of interest (ROI) approach in order to increase sensitivity (including ROIs of dl/dmPFC, cACC lOFC, and rACC). Other prefrontal areas, such as vlPFC, have a less clear specificity, as they seem to be equally involved in emotional and non-emotional regulatory processes [24, 25, 49, 50]. The hypothesis and analysis plan were preregistered within IMAGEN before access to the data was granted. Although our hypothesis is based on cortical volume measures, surface-based methods provided by FreeSurfer allowed the exploration of cortical thickness and surface area measures. We also tested whether a similar division between the traits was mirrored in behavioral tests of EF and delay discounting.

Materials and methods

Participants

Data was obtained from a community sample consisting of 1969 adolescents from the European multi-center genetic-neuroimaging IMAGEN study (https://imagen-europe.com) [47]. Ethical approval had been obtained by the local ethics committees and written informed consent from all participants and their legal guardians. Recruitment procedures have been described in a previous review [47]. Participants completed neuropsychological, clinical, and personality assessments online and at assessments sites. A total of N = 1093 participants (mean age: 14.47 ± 0.39 years; 616 females) provided sufficient MRI data quality and were analyzed in this study (Supplementary Fig. 1). Sample characteristics are described in Supplementary Table 1.

ADHD and CD traits

ADHD and CD symptoms were measured using the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) [51]. Its psychometric properties and reliability have been described by Goodman et al. [51]. SDQ consists of five subscales; Emotional problems scale, Conduct problems scale, Hyperactivity/Inattention scale, Peer problems scale, and Prosocial scale. While the Conduct problems scale has been constructed to measure CD symptoms and mirrors the categorical diagnosis of CD, the Hyperactivity/Inattention scale has been constructed to mirror hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention problems, i.e., core symptoms of ADHD.

We used the Conduct problems scale (five questions), referred to as CD scale in this study, to assess CD-like symptoms, and the Hyperactivity/Inattention scale (five questions), referred to as ADHD scale in this study, to assess ADHD symptoms. In follow-up analyses, the ADHD scale was divided into Hyperactivity/Impulsivity subscale (three questions) and Inattention subscale (two questions), as it has been in previous studies [52]. The CD scale—and not the Emotion problems scale—was used because we focused on the relation between ADHD and CD on a trait level mirroring previous comparisons between the two phenotypes on a categorical diagnostic level [6]. Also, we argue that the CD scale better represents problems of emotional instability, rather than a more sustained low level of mood as in the Emotion problems scale—which is better used to assess highly heterogeneous problems of depression and anxiety.

Each question of the SDQ yields scores from zero to two points. We combined self-rate and parent score (for the relation between the scores see Supplementary Fig. 2), since externalizing problems is assumed to be better assessed by parents, while internalizing problems may be better assessed by the adolescent him/herself.

Magnetic resonance imaging

Full details of the magnetic resonance imaging acquisition protocols and quality control have been described elsewhere [47, 53]. In addition to gray matter volume (GMV), we measured cortical thickness (CT) and surface area (SA) as they independently build up the GMV measure and follow separate and independent developmental trajectories [54, 55]. The structural measures were obtained from T1 weighted images using the semi-automated segmentation and cortical surface reconstruction methods provided by FreeSurfer v5. [47, 53, 56,57,58,59,60,61]. In brief, the procedure includes intensity normalization, removal of non-brain tissue, segmentation of cortical gray, subcortical white, deep gray matter volumetric structures, as well as triangular tessellation of the gray/white matter interface and white matter/cerebrospinal fluid boundary (pial surface). FreeSurfer also allows the automated parcellation of the cortical surfaces into anatomical regions of interest (ROI), 34 per hemisphere [62]. For each ROI cortical measures were obtained from subjects’ native space. Based on our hypothesis, we focussed on four different regions in the main analysis: lateral orbitofrontal cortex (lOFC), rostral and caudal anterior cingulate cortex (rACC and cACC), as well as dorsolateral/dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dl/dmPFC). The dl/dmPFC was constructed by combining average cortical volumes extracted from superior frontal, caudal middle frontal and rostral middle frontal cortex, as done in previous studies [63]. All the ROIs were bilateral as we had no unilateral hypothesis.

The statistical approach and discussion on dmPFC and vlPFC in relation to choice of ROIs is described in Supplementary Fig. 3. In brief, we controlled for sex and imaging center in all structural analyses. We used ADHD and CD scores separately to determine all variance in structure that could account for each trait alone. We also used both scores as independent variables in the same statistical model to determine variance in structure that could be uniquely explained by each of the ADHD and CD scores alone.

We did not correct for head size, since we were interested in a dysfunction of a variety of behaviors that depend on large scale prefrontal networks (as opposed to one specific function). In addition, wide spread cortical alterations associated with ADHD in children have been shown [30]. A correction of head size would therefore partly correct for the measure of interest. Furthermore, in studies focusing on neurodevelopmental disorders, such as ADHD, a correction for IQ is not recommended, since it may also correct for characteristics of the disorder [64]. This reasoning is also relevant for subclinical ADHD traits. We have therefore not corrected for IQ in the main analyses. However, for completeness we provide the results obtained when correcting for intra-cranial volume (ICV) and IQ in the Supplementary Information.

A vertex-wise approach was used to explore more focal associations between the behavioral data and cortical volume that might not be detectable with the ROI approach within the predefined ROIs. In addition, we performed an explorative vertex-wise analysis over the whole brain.

Behavioral tasks

We analyzed tests that were associated with either “cool” EF, i.e., the Stop Signal Test [65, 66] and Spatial Working Memory (SWM) task [67,68,69], or with “hot” EF, i.e., Delay Discounting (DD) [70]. The statistical approach is described in Supplementary Fig. 3.

Results

Descriptive data of ADHD and CD scores

Residual error revealed normal distributions in all analyses performed. Both the ADHD (mean 6.90 ± 3.75) and CD (mean 3.70 ± 2.60) trait scores displayed a wide, but left-skewed distribution (Fig. 1a, b). A correlational analysis disclosed a moderate correlation (r = 0.55, p < 0.001) between the ADHD and the CD scores (Fig. 1c).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Description of SDQ distribution in our sample of the CD (Conduct) and ADHD (Hyperactivity/Inattention) scores (a and b) and the correlation (r = 0.549, p < 0.001) between the two main subscales (c)

MR data (ROI-analysis)

Individual effects of ADHD and CD on brain structure

Both ADHD scores and CD scores correlated negatively with gray matter volumes (GMVs) and surface area (SA) in all ROIs (cACC, dl/dmPFC, lOFC, and rACC), while cortical thickness (CT) did not show any correlation with these traits (Table 1; Supplementary Fig. 4).

Table 1 Main effects of ADHD and CD scores on gray matter volume (GMV; a), surface area (SA; b) and cortical thickness (CT; c) of the pre-defined ROIs when not controlling for the other trait dimension

Specific effects of ADHD and CD on brain structure

GMV in dl/dmPFC and cACC correlated negatively with the ADHD scores after controlling for CD scores, however this effect did not reach significance after correction for the number of tests (Table 2a). SA in dl/dmPFC and cACC correlated negatively with the ADHD scores after controlling for CD scores (Table 2b). There was no significant relation between ADHD scores and CT in any of the ROIs (Table 2c).

Table 2 Testing specific hypothesis 1

There was a significant negative correlation between GMV in rACC and CD scores when controlling for ADHD scores (Table 3a). No significant effect of CD scores was observed in lOFC GMV when controlling for ADHD scores. There was a negative correlation between the SA and CD scores in rACC after controlling for ADHD scores on a threshold-significant level (Table 3b). No significant effect was observed between the CD scores and SA in lOFC after controlling for ADHD scores. There was no significant relation between CT of the predefined ROIs and CD scores (Table 3c). A 3D-representation of the relationships between the two behavioral dimensions (ADHD and CD trait) and GMV in the ACC ROIs is shown in Fig. 2.

Table 3 Testing specific hypothesis 2
Fig. 2
figure 2

3D-representation of the relationships between the two behavioral dimensions—ADHD scores (x-axis) and CD scores (y-axis)—and the residuals of the dependent variable GMV (z-axis), after adjusting for sex and center as obtained from the main analysis. Two representative examples for the rACC volume (bottom) and cACC volume (top) are shown. Respective views along the z-axis (onto the x–y plane), y-axis (onto xz plane) and x-axis (onto xz plane) are shown, displaying the mutual relationships between two measures. For illustrative purpose, the 3D-data was fitted with a two-dimensional linear robust regression using a polynomial according to the equation. Volume =a+b×ADHD score+c×CD score. To better visualize the angle of the obtained plane, its height along the z-axis (volume) has been color-coded with warm colors (yellow) indicating larger volumes, and cold colors (blue) representing lower volumes. In case of cACC, the color gradient changes towards the ADHD score dimension, whereas in case of rACC the gradient direction is more shifted towards the CD score dimension. This reflects the discussed associations between cACC and ADHD score after correcting for CD score, and between rACC and CD scores after correcting for ADHD scores as presented in the main analysis

We had no hypotheses that there would be any specific negative correlations between lOFC / rACC and ADHD scores, or between cACC/dl/dmPFC and CD scores. However, the results of those analyses are shown in Supplementary Tables 2 and 3 for completeness.

Exploratory analysis

A vertex-wise analysis within the ROIs under investigation (Fig. 3) also suggests more focal/localized effects. Moreover, it confirmed the extensive overlap between the effect of ADHD and the effect of CD on brain structure suggested in the ROI-analysis (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

ROIs as defined by the Desikan atlas that are used for the main analysis are shown in the left column. The other columns contain results of the exploratory analysis showing correlations within those ROIs on a vertex-level. In column 2–3 correlations between GMV and ADHD and CD scores, respectively, are shown. Their regional overlap is shown in column 4 (purple areas). Both ADHD and CD score revealed negative correlations with GMV within a sub-region of the left and right dl/dmPFC. Those were mainly localized in a cluster comprising the rostral middle frontal and lateral superior frontal cortex, extending to medial portions of the prefrontal cortex. Similar focal associations were observed in posterior and anterior sub-regions of the left and right lOFC. Correlations observed in the ACC were spread over right caudal and rostral ACC ROIs, whereas a correlation of more focal nature was observed in the left ACC. When correcting for CD trait, the ADHD scores were negatively correlated with the GMV in right rostral middle frontal cortex, right rostral dmPFC, left cACC and bilateral anterior lOFC (column 5). In contrast, when correcting for ADHD trait, CD scores were negatively correlated with the GMV in right rACC (column 6). Moreover, right cACC and posterior lOFC also showed this relation to a certain extent (column 6). Thus, on a vertex-level there seemed to be a more complex and unique relation between local GMV and the different traits: While GMV in left cACC was more negatively related to ADHD scores, GMV in right cACC and rACC was more related to CD scores. Likewise, while the negative relation between ADHD scores in lOFC GMV was more widespread and anterior when controlling for CD, the negative relation between CD scores in lOFC GMV was more ventrolateral (spreading into ventrolateral PFC). Overall, no positive correlations were observed. Significance is represented on a log(p-value) scale, where positive values (warm colors) are assigned to positive associations, and negative values (cold colors) to negative associations. For explorative reasons the display threshold was set to 1.3, corresponding to p = .05 (uncorrected)

In the whole brain analysis (vertex-wise approach, cluster-wise corrected) we observed similar significant and largely overlapping negative relations between (a) the ADHD trait and GMV, and (b) CD trait and GMV in ACC and prefrontal cortex (Fig. 4). We also observed that the ADHD trait was significantly related to smaller GMV in several regions such as temporopolar and orbitofrontal cortex after controlling for CD trait, while the CD trait was more related to smaller GMV in left temporoparietal junction after controlling for ADHD trait (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

An exploratory vertex-wise analysis was performed calculating correlations between GMV and ADHD/CD scores across the whole brain. Multiple comparison correction was done using a Monte Carlo cluster wise approach (cluster threshold p < 0.05). Both ADHD and CD scores were significantly related to smaller GMV in frontal brain region clusters (dl/dmPFC, rostral/caudal ACC, and lOFC). Additional clusters revealing significant negative correlations outside our ROI:s were also observed: ADHD scores were negatively correlated to GMV in right anterior insula, bilateral inferior frontal cortex (pars orbitalis), bilateral temporal cortex, bilateral precentral, and postcentral (motor/sensory) cortex, right medial OFC, left medial and lateral occipital cortex, and bilateral fusiform. When correcting for CD scores clusters in bilateral temporal/temporopolar regions, left medial and lateral occipital cortex, right precentral and postcentral, as well as left lOFC were still present. CD scores showed additional negative correlations in precentral and postcentral cortex, anterior temporal cortex, bilateral anterior insula, left fusiform cortex, and bilateral temporo-parietal junction (TPJ), which, except for the cluster in left TPJ, disappeared when correcting for ADHD scores. Significance is represented on a log(p-value) scale, where positive values (warm colors) are assigned to positive associations, and negative values (cold colors) to negative associations. Right hemisphere is shown on the top and left hemisphere is shown on the bottom in the figure

In order to understand whether hyperactivity or inattention problems better explained the relation between ADHD traits and brain structure we divided the ADHD scale into Hyperactivity/Impulsivity scores and Inattention scores. This analysis showed that both Hyperactivity/Impulsivity scores and Inattention scores contributed to the structural effects in cACC and dl/dmPFC (Supplementary Table 4). However, this contribution was larger for the Hyperactivity/Impulsivity scores.

Similar structural findings as in the main analysis, albeit to a lower significance level, were observed for ADHD scores and for CD scores when controlling for ICV and IQ (Supplementary Tables 5 and 6).

When studying only a sub-population of the subjects with a low indication of a psychiatric diagnosis (n = 860) similar general result were observed for the ADHD trait (Supplementary Table 7). The effects of the CD trait were weaker in this analysis with a border significant negative correlation in the main analysis between CD trait and GMV in rACC.

Our exploratory results also suggest that although both higher ADHD and CD scores are related to a smaller GMV in dmPFC, this effect still remains for the ADHD trait when controlling for the CD trait (Supplementary Table 8; Fig. 3). Moreover, neither the ADHD trait nor the CD trait was significantly related to vlPFC morphology (Supplementary Table 8).

Behavioral results

Stop Signal Test–SDQ

ADHD score correlated with SSRT (standardized β = 0.099, p = 0.021), while CD score did not (standardized β = 0.014, p = 0.740). When controlling for CD score, the ADHD score uniquely explained a significant amount of the SSRT variance (standardized β = 0.131, p = 0.011). Exploratory analysis suggested that this correlation was driven by the Inattention subscore, which uniquely explained a significant part of the variance when controlling for Hyperactivity/Impulsivity subscore and CD score (standardized β = 0.158, p = 0.003). Neither the ADHD nor CD scores correlated with GoRT (ADHD score: standardized β = 0.040, p = 0.343; CD score: standardized β = 0.028, p = 0.494)). Results are shown in Table 4a.

Table 4 Behavioral tests and results

Spatial Working Memory task—SDQ

Both ADHD and CD scores correlated with number of errors in the SWM task (ADHD score: standardized β = 0.167, p < 0.001; CD score: standardized β = 0.149, p < 0.001). The ADHD score correlated with the number of errors in the SWM task, when controlling for CD (standardized β = 0.120, p < 0.001). This correlation seemed to be driven by the Inattention SDQ subscore (standardized β = 0.152, p < .001) when controlling for Hyperactivity/Impulsivity and CD score. The CD score still correlated with the number of errors when controlling for ADHD score (standardized β = 0.083, p = 0.021). Results are shown in Table 4b.

Delay discounting–SDQ

Both ADHD and CD scores correlated with the log-transformed k-coefficient from the Delay discounting (DD) task (ADHD score: standardized β = 0.139, p < 0.001; CD score: standardized β = 0.140, p < 0.001). The CD score correlated with k when controlling for ADHD score (standardized β = 0.093, p = 0.014). ADHD score also explained a unique part of the variance when controlling for CD score (standardized β = 0.088, p = 0.021). Results are shown in Table 4c.

Exploratory analysis

In an exploratory analysis, we investigated whether information from brain structure could explain variability on ADHD and CD scores beyond that explained by behavioral tests alone. The analysis showed that SSRT and brain structure related to non-emotional top-down control (cACC and dl/dmPFC GMV, as well as SA) each uniquely explained a part of the variance of the ADHD score (Supplementary Table 9), while Delay discounting k-coefficient and brain structure related to emotional top-down control (rACC GMV, as well as SA) each uniquely explained part of the CD score variance (Supplementary Table 10).

Discussion

In the present study, we showed that both ADHD and CD trait scores correlated negatively with gray matter volume (GMV) and surface area (SA) in cACC, dl/dmPFC, rACC, and lOFC in a large community sample of adolescents using a ROI approach. As hypothesized, there was a substantial overlap between the effects of the ADHD and the CD traits on brain morphology (Fig. 3). Thus, investigating ADHD symptoms without considering CD symptoms, or vice versa, makes it difficult to distinguish between their unique effects on brain structure. Our analyses indicated that cACC and dl/dmPFC SA correlated negatively with ADHD score after controlling for CD score. Similar findings were observed for GMV, although these did not survive correction for multiple comparisons. In contrast, CD score correlated negatively with the GMV in rACC after controlling for ADHD score (SA results on trend-level). Our study suggests that, apart from the large overlap in structural relations, there are morphological characteristics that are specific to either ADHD symptoms or CD symptoms, and that those relations can be observed on trait level within a community sample.

The current findings are in line with the idea that non-emotional ADHD symptoms are associated with a smaller structure [30,31,32,33] and lower function [26,27,28,29] of cACC and dl/dmPFC, areas that have been linked to "cool" EF [24, 25], while emotional instability and CD symptoms are associated with smaller structure [42,43,44, 45, 46] and lower function [36, 37, 40, 41, 71, 72] of rACC and bordering areas, that are linked to "hot" EF [73,74,75,76,77] and emotional regulation [49, 78, 79]. While these findings corroborate several previous studies on ADHD and CD patients they also suggest that some structural differences are unique to the two different traits. However, we could not find any evidence for the hypothesized relationship between lOFC morphology and CD trait scores, instead we observed more localized associations in the exploratory vertex-level analysis (Fig. 3). One plausible explanation for this is that the lOFC is still under substantial development during adolescence [80] and that this relationship might be detectable only later in life.

Previous studies of inattention/hyperactivity and emotional instability have typically focused on categorically defined patient groups in the respective domains, such as ADHD or BPD, ASPD, and CD. However, it has been shown that subclinical symptoms associated with inattention or emotional instability also occur in the general population to a variable degree, often following a normal or semi-normal distribution [2, 14,15,16]. It has been hypothesized that patients represent extreme cases within this symptom distribution [2]. This variability may occur due to different capacities in the underlying regulatory systems in the brain, i.e., cognitive core capacities [2]. On a whole this model posits that disorders involving top-down dysregulation, such as ADHD or BPD, ASPD, and CD, are better understood using a dimensional model rather than a categorical model [2]. Our findings that dimensional measures of ADHD-symptoms and CD-symptoms in a general population are correlated to cognitive function and some of the brain morphology previously shown to be altered in the respective disorders partially support this hypothesis. However, it is important to stress that clinical disorders such as ADHD and CD typically show significantly larger impairment and often include co-morbidities. This may be a reason why some structural findings observed in large meta-analyses were not present on a trait level. Also, as the present study focused on cortical measures it is not known how the traits relate to subcortical structures. It is noteworthy that dmPFC showed a relation to the ADHD trait in the present study although it has not been found to be altered in two recent meta-analysis on ADHD [29, 34]. However, in another meta-analysis a region in cACC adjacent to and partly overlapping with dmPFC was smaller in adult ADHD patients as compared to controls [33]. It should also be pointed out that dmPFC is related to the term “ADHD” in Neurosynth [81] based on fMRI data. Apart from the present study, two recent studies [48, 82] have used a dimensional approach and observed a negative relation between sub-clinical ADHD symptoms and GMV in medial prefrontal annd anterior cinngulate regions. However, these studies have not disentangled non-emotional ADHD traits from emotional instability or CD traits.

The structural findings that are specific to ADHD or CD scores were assumed to be mirrored in the behavioral measurements. We hypothesized that “cool” EF should be associated specifically to ADHD traits, while “hot” EF should be more related to CD traits. In line with this suggestion, we showed a negative correlation between ADHD score and motor impulse control in the Stop Signal Test, which remained significant when controlling for CD scores. Impaired working memory capacity has also frequently been linked to ADHD problems [3]. Surprisingly, we observed that both the level of ADHD and CD problems uniquely explained part of the working memory performance, making this task less specific to either problem domain. Finally, we observed that both ADHD and CD scores were uniquely related to delay discounting tendency. This was expected, since performance on this task depends on both non-emotional general attentional control, as well as emotionally related regulation of reward processes. This result is in line with previous findings reporting that some ADHD subgroups show suboptimal results in “cool” EF tasks, such as Stop Signal Test and Stroop test, whereas other subgroups show deficits in “hot” EF tasks, such as delay discounting [17]. Interestingly, our data suggests that brain structure explains unique aspects of ADHD and CD traits, beyond what is explained by behavioral results. Thus, a combination of both measurements may be the most optimal marker for these traits.

There are several limitations in the study. First, apart from emotional instability, the CD trait also captures a less common phenotype that includes callous-unemotional and psychopathic behavior associated with different processing of emotional stimuli [4]. Second, although the structural findings related to the ADHD trait were mostly driven by hyperactivity/impulsiveness questions, the behavioral findings related to the ADHD trait were mostly driven by the inattention questions. Thus, future studies should further investigate the inter-relationships between the specific ADHD sub-traits, structural and cognitive characteristics. Third, ADHD has been associated with different types of emotional dysregulation symptoms [19]. Future studies should investigate how such dysregulations are related to more specific features of CD traits. Finally, as common in studies on hyperactivity, movement artefacts may have partly contributed to the structural findings. However, these seem to be mediated by changes in cortical thickness [83] where we did not observe any associations with ADHD traits.